Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-p2v8j Total loading time: 0.001 Render date: 2024-05-22T15:29:20.126Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Are language–cognition interactions bigger than a breadbox? Integrative modeling and design space thinking temper simplistic questions about causally dense phenomena

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 February 2024

Debra Titone*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada debra.titone@mcgill.ca esteban.hernandezrivera@mcgill.ca antonio.martineziniesta@mcgill.ca Montreal Bilingualism Initiative, Montreal, QC, Canada Centre for Research on Brain, Language, and Music, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
Esteban Hernández-Rivera
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada debra.titone@mcgill.ca esteban.hernandezrivera@mcgill.ca antonio.martineziniesta@mcgill.ca Montreal Bilingualism Initiative, Montreal, QC, Canada Centre for Research on Brain, Language, and Music, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
Antonio Iniesta
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada debra.titone@mcgill.ca esteban.hernandezrivera@mcgill.ca antonio.martineziniesta@mcgill.ca Montreal Bilingualism Initiative, Montreal, QC, Canada Centre for Research on Brain, Language, and Music, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
Anne L. Beatty-Martínez
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada debra.titone@mcgill.ca esteban.hernandezrivera@mcgill.ca antonio.martineziniesta@mcgill.ca Montreal Bilingualism Initiative, Montreal, QC, Canada Centre for Research on Brain, Language, and Music, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA abeattymartinez@ucsd.edu
Jason W. Gullifer
Affiliation:
Department of Computer Science, Marianopolis College, Westmount, QC, Canada j.gullifer@maranopolis.edu
*
*Corresponding author.

Abstract

We affirm the utility of integrative modeling, according to which it is advantageous to move beyond “one-at-a-time binary paradigms” through studies that position themselves within realistic multidimensional design spaces. We extend the integrative modeling approach to a target domain with which we are familiar, the consequences of bilingualism on mind and brain, often referred to as the “bilingual advantage.” In doing so, we highlight work from our group consistent with integrative modeling.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Atkinson, D., Byrnes, H., Doran, M., Duff, P., Ellis, N. C., Hall, J. K.., … Tarone, E. (2016). A transdisciplinary framework for SLA in a multilingual world. Modern Language Journal, 100, 1947. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12301Google Scholar
Baum, S., & Titone, D. (2014). Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism, executive control, and aging. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(5), 857894. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716414000174CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beatty-Martínez, A. L., & Titone, D. A. (2021). The quest for signals in noise: Leveraging experiential variation to identify bilingual phenotypes. Languages, 6(4), 168. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6040168CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beatty-Martínez, A. L., & Titone, D. A. (2024). De-generacy as an organizing principle of bilingual language processing: Evidence from brain and behavior. In Short, K. Morgan & Hell, J. G. van (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition and neurolinguistics (p. 260273). Routledge.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290303. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist, 32(7), 513531. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.32.7.513CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Bot, K., Lowie, W., & Verspoor, M. (2007). A dynamic systems theory approach to second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10(1), 721. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728906002732CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodenough, F. L. (1926). Racial differences in the intelligence of school children. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 9(5), 388397. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0073325CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gullifer, J. W., Pivneva, I., Whitford, V., Sheikh, N. A., & Titone, D. (2023). Bilingual language experience and its effect on conflict adaptation in reactive inhibitory control tasks. Psychological Science, 34(2), 238251. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976221113764CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gullifer, J. W., & Titone, D. (2021). Engaging proactive control: Influences of diverse language experiences using insights from machine learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(3), 414430. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000933CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofman, J. M., Watts, D. J., Athey, S., Garip, F., Griffiths, T. L., Kleinberg, J., … Yarkoni, T. (2021). Integrating explanation and prediction in computational social science. Nature, 595(7866), 181188. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-03659-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Navarro-Torres, C. A., Beatty-Martínez, A. L., Kroll, J. F., & Green, D. W. (2021). Research on bilingualism as discovery science. Brain and Language, 222, 105014. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2021.105014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newell, A. (1973). You can't play 20 questions with nature and win: Projective comments on the papers of this symposium. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-170150-5.50012-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peal, E., & Lambert, W. E. (1962). The relation of bilingualism to intelligence. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 76(27), 123. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093840CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saer, D. J. (1923). The effect of bilingualism on intelligence. British Journal of Psychology, 14, 2538.Google Scholar
Titone, D., & Baum, S. (2014). The future of bilingualism research: Insufferably optimistic and replete with new questions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(5), 933942. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716414000289CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Titone, D., Gullifer, J., Subramaniapillai, S., Rajah, N., & Baum, S. (2017). Chapter 13. History-inspired reflections on the bilingual advantages hypothesis. In Bialystok, E. & Sullivan, M. D. (Eds.), Growing old with two languages: Effects of bilingualism on cognitive aging (pp. 265295). John Benjamins Publishing Company. https://doi.org/10.1075/sibil.53.13titCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Titone, D. A., & Tiv, M. (2023). Rethinking multilingual experience through a systems framework of bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 26(1), 116. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728921001127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tiv, M., Gullifer, J. W., Feng, R. Y., & Titone, D. (2020). Using network science to map what Montréal bilinguals talk about across languages and communicative contexts. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 56, 100913. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100913CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tiv, M., Kutlu, E., Gullifer, J. W., Feng, R. Y., Doucerain, M. M., & Titone, D. A. (2022). Bridging interpersonal and ecological dynamics of cognition through a systems framework of bilingualism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 151(9), 21282143. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001174CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Deventer, F. (1952). Twenty questions [television broadcast]. DuMont Television Network. http://archive.org/details/TwentyQuestionsjanuary181952Google Scholar
Wagner, D., Bekas, K., & Bialystok, E. (2023). Does language entropy shape cognitive performance? A tale of two cities. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 111. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728923000202Google Scholar