Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ndmmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-09T08:26:31.138Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

What semantic dementia tells us about the ability to infer others' communicative intentions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2023

François Osiurak
Affiliation:
Laboratoire d’Étude des Mécanismes Cognitifs, Université de Lyon, 5 avenue Pierre Mendès France, 69676 Bron Cedex, France. francois.osiurak@univ-lyon2.fr https://emc.univ-lyon2.fr/fr/equipes/cognition-outils-systemes/francois-osiurak/ Institut Universitaire de France, 1 rue Descartes, 75231 Paris Cedex 5, France
Giovanni Federico
Affiliation:
IRCCS Synlab SDN S.p.A., Via Emanuele Gianturco 113, 80143 Naples, Italy. research@giovannifederico.net http://www.giovannifederico.net/

Abstract

As Heintz & Scott-Phillips rightly argued, pragmatics has been too commonly considered as a supplement to linguistic communication. Their aim to reorient the study of cognitive pragmatics as the foundation of many distinctive features of human behavior finds echo in the neuropsychological literature on tool use, in which the investigation of semantic dementia challenges the classical semantics versus pragmatics dissociation.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baumard, J., Lesourd, M., Jarry, C., Merck, C., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., Chauviré, V., … Le Gall, D. (2016). Tool use disorders in neurodegenerative diseases: Roles of semantic memory and technical reasoning. Cortex, 82, 119132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumard, J., Lesourd, M., Remigereau, C., Merck, C., Jarry, C., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., … Le Gall, D. (2019). The – weak – role of memory in tool use: Evidence from neurodegenerative diseases. Neuropsychologia, 129, 117132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2002a). The influence of personal familiarity and context on object use in semantic dementia. Neurocase, 8, 127134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2002b). When objects lose their meaning: What happens to their use? Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, 236251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buxbaum, L. J., Schwartz, M. F., & Carew, T. G. (1997). The role of semantic memory in object use. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 219254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, H. L., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of “theory of mind.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 7783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldenberg, G., & Hagmann, S. (1998). Tool use and mechanical problem solving in apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 36, 581589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodges, J. R., Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Spatt, J. (2000). The role of conceptual knowledge in object use: Evidence from semantic dementia. Brain, 123, 19131925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambon Ralph, M. A., Jefferies, E., Patterson, K., & Rogers, T. T. (2017). The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18, 4255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lauro-Grotto, R., Piccini, C., & Shallice, T. (1997). Modality-specific operations in semantic dementia. Cortex, 33, 593622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lesourd, M., Baumard, J., Jarry, C., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., Belliard, S., Moreaud, O., … Osiurak, F. (2016). Mechanical problem-solving in Alzheimer's disease and semantic dementia. Neuropsychology, 30, 612623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moreaud, O., Belliard, S., Snowden, J., Auriacombe, S., Basaglia-Pappas, S., Bernard, F., … Virat-Brassaud, M. E. (2008). Semantic dementia: Reflexions of a French working group for diagnostic criteria and constitution of a patient cohort. Revue Neurologique, 164, 343353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osiurak, F., & Reynaud, E. (2020). The elephant in the room: What matters cognitively in cumulative technological culture. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43, e156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osiurak, F., Reynaud, E., Baumard, J., Rossetti, Y., Bartolo, A., & Lesourd, M. (2021). Pantomime of tool use: Looking beyond apraxia. Brain Communications, 3, facb263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osiurak, F., & Rossetti, Y. (2017). Definition: Limb apraxia. Cortex, 93, 228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothi, L. J. G., Ochipa, C., & Heilman, K. M. (1991). A cognitive neuropsychological model of limb praxis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 443458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowden, J. S., Bathgate, D., Varma, A., Blackshaw, A., Gibbons, Z. C., & Neary, D. (2001). Distinct behavioural profiles in frontotemporal dementia and semantic dementia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 70, 323332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed