Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-c9gpj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-13T20:52:18.243Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Cognition, imitation, and culture in the great apes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 September 2009

Craig B. Stanford
Affiliation:
University of Southern California, Los Angeles
Michael A. Arbib
Affiliation:
University of Southern California
Get access

Summary

Introduction: The problem of reconstructing cognition

Most psychologists and cognitive scientists and almost all linguists investigate the nature of the mind, intelligence, and language from a uniquely human perspective. But the origins of these attributes lie in the deep history of the primate brain, and all organic explanations for them must derive from non-human and human primate evolutionary history. There are a variety of approaches to reconstructing the origins of cognition and language. The fossil record is the only direct evidence of human evolution, but it is fragmentary. It can tell us little about the roots of cognition and language because the soft tissues involved do not fossilize.

Our understanding of human evolution is therefore greatly enhanced by evidence from living primates, especially those most closely related to us, the great apes – the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes), bonobo (P. paniscus), gorilla (Gorilla gorilla), and orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus). While we must be careful not to consider apes as living fossils, they are exemplars of the way in which natural selection molds large-brained, social primates to the natural environment. They exhibit learned, interpopulational culturally varied behavior beyond that of any animal other than ourselves. They provide us with a sense of the likeliest range of options, behavioral and cognitive, that natural selection would have taken with ancestral forms of humans.

As an anthropologist, I study non-human primates because they inform us about the likely course that the evolution of human form and function has taken.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arbib, M. A., 2005. From monkey-like action recognition to human language: an evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behav. Brain Sci. 28: 105–124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauer, P., and Mandler, J., 1989. One thing follows another: effects of temporal structure on 1- and 2-year-olds' recall of events. Devel. Psychol. 25: 197–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blurton-Jones, N., 1984. A selfish origin for human food sharing: tolerated theft. Ethol. Sociobiol. 4: 145–147.Google Scholar
Boesch, C., and Boesch, H., 1982. Optimization of nut-cracking with natural hammers by wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 3: 265–286.Google Scholar
Boesch, C., and Boesch, H. 1989. Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Taï National Park. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 78: 547–573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C. and Tomasello, M., 1998. Chimpanzee and human culture. Curr. Anthropol. 39: 591–614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W., 1995. The Thinking Ape. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W., and Byrne, J. M. E., 1993. Complex leaf-gathering skills of mountain gorillas (Gorilla g. beringei). Am. J. Primatol. 31: 241–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W., and Whiten, A. (eds.), 1988a. Machiavellian Intelligence. Oxford, UK: Clarendon PressGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W., and Whiten, A. 1988b. Towards the next generation in data quality: a new survey of primate tactical deception. Behav. Brain Sci. 11: 267–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Call, J., and Tomasello, M., 1999. A nonverbal false belief task: the performances of children and great apes. Child Devel. 70: 381–395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R. M., 1991. How Monkeys See the World. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Darwin, C., 1871. The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Waal, F. B. M., 1987. Tension regulation and nonreproductive functions of sex in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus). Natl Geogr. Res. Rep. 3: 318–335.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M., 1992. Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. J. Hum. Evol. 20: 469–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fouts, R., and Mills, S. T., 1997. Next of Kin. New York: William Morrow.Google Scholar
Garber, P. A., and Paciulli, L. M., 1997. Experimental field study of spatial memory and learning in wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus). Folia Primatol. 68: 236–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., and Kiraly, I., 2002. Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature 415: 755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodall, J., 1986. The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Heinrich, B., and Marzluff, J., 1995. Why ravens share. Am. Scientist 83: 342–349.Google Scholar
Hill, K., Kaplan, H., Hawkes, K., and Hurtado, A. M., 1987. Foraging decisions among Aché hunter–gatherers: new data and implications for optimal foraging models. Ethol. Sociobiol. 8: 1–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hohmann, G., and Fruth, B., 2003. Culture in bonobos? Between-species and with-species variation in behavior. Curr. Anthropol. 44: 563–571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kingston, J. D., Marino, B. D., and Hill, A., 1994. Isotopic evidence for Neogene hominid paleoenvironments in the Kenya rift valley. Science 264: 955–959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrew, W. C., 1979. Evolutionary implications of sex differences in chimpanzee predation and tool use. In Hamburg, D. A. and McCown, E. R. (eds.) The Great Apes. Mento Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings, pp. 441–469.Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C. 1992. Chimpanzee Material Culture. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milton, K., 1981. Distribution pattern of tropical food plants as a stimulus to primate mental development. Am. Anthropologist 83: 534–548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C., and Watts, D., 1999. Demographic influences on the hunting behavior of chimpanzees. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 109: 439–454.3.0.CO;2-3>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitani, J. C., Hunley, K. L., and Murdoch, M. E., 1999. Geographic variation in the calls of wild chimpanzees: a reassessment. Am. J. Primatol. 47: 133–151.3.0.CO;2-I>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, T., 1973. The ant-gathering behavior by the use of tools among wild chimpanzees of the Mahali Mountains. J. Hum. Evol. 2: 357–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. 1980. The leaf-clipping display: a newly discovered expressive gesture in wild chimpanzees. J. Hum. Evol. 9: 117–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T.(ed.), 1990. The Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains. Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press.Google Scholar
Packer, C., and Ruttan, L., 1988. The evolution of cooperative hunting. Am. Naturalist 132: 159–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M., 1999. The Alex Studies: Cognitive and Communicative Abilities of Grey Parrots.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., 2000. Folk Physics for Apes: The Chimpanzee's Theory of How the World Works. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., Rulf, A. B., and Biershwale, D., 1994. Absence of knowledge attribution and self-recognition in young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 180: 74–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., 1999. Ape language: between a rock and a hard place. In King, B. J. (ed.) The Origins of Language: What Nonhuman Primates Can Tell Us. Santa Fe, NM: School of American Research Press, pp. 115–189.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., and Lewin, R., 1994. Kanzi: The Ape at the Brint of the Human Mind. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Savage-Rambaugh, S., Shankar, S. G., and Taylor, T. J., 1998. Apes, Language, and the Human Mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stanford, C. B., 1995. The influence of chimpanzee predation on group size and anti-predator behaviour in red colobus monkeys. Anim. Behav., 49: 577–587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanford, C. B., 1998. Chimpanzee and Red Colobus: The Ecology of Predator and Prey. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Stanford, C. B., 1999. The Hunting Apes. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Stanford, C. B., Wallis, J., Matama, H., and Goodall, J., 1994. Patterns of predation by chimpanzees on red colobus monkeys in Gombe National Park, Tanzania, 1982–1991. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 94: 213–228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanford, C. B., Gambaneza, C., Nkurunungi, J. B., and Goldsmith, M., 2000. Chimpanzees in Bwindi Impenetrable National Park, Uganda, use different tools to obtain different types of honey. Primates 41: 335–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephens, D. W., and Krebs, J. R., 1986. Foraging Theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University PressGoogle Scholar
Sugiyama, Y., 1981. Observations on the population dynamics and behavior of wild chimpanzees of Bossou, Guinea, 1979–1980. Primates 22: 435–444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sumita, K., Kitahara-Frisch, J., and Norikoshi, K., 1985. The acquisition of stone-tool use in captive chimpanzees. Primates 26: 168–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teleki, G., 1973. The Predatory Behavior of Wild Chimpanzees. Lewisburg, PA: Bucknell University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., 1996. Do apes ape? In Heyes, C. and Galef, B. Jr. (eds.) Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture. New York: Academic Press, pp. 319–346.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. 1999. Emulation learning and cultural learning. Behav. Brain Sci. 21: 703–704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomsello, M., and Call, J., 1997. Primate Cognition. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Schaik, C. P., Fox, E. A., and Sitompul, A. F., 1996. Manufacture and use of tools in wild Sumatran orangutans: implications for human evolution. Naturwissenschaften 83: 186–188.Google ScholarPubMed
Visalberghi, E., and Trinca, L., 1989. Tool use in capuchin monkeys, or distinguish between performing and understanding. Primates 30: 511–521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A., and Byrne, R. W., 1997. Machiavellian Intelligence, vol. 2, Extensions and Evaluations. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., et al., 1999. Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399: 682–685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkinson, G. S., 1988. Reciprocal altruism in bats and other mammals. Ethol. Sociobiol. 9: 85–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winterhalder, B., 2001. Intragroup resource transfers: comparative evidence, models, and implications for human evolution. In Stanford, C. B. and Bunn, H. T. (eds.) Meat-Eating and Human Evolution.New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 279–301.Google Scholar
Wolschlager, A., and Bekkering, H., 2000. Is human imitation based on a mirror-neuron system? Some behavioral evidence. Exp. Brain Res. 143: 335–341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrangham, R. W., 1975. Behavioural ecology of chimpanzees in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Ph.D. dissertation, Cambridge University.
Yamagiwa, J., Yamoto, T., Mwanza, N., and Maruhashi, T., 1988. Evidence of the tool-use by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes Schwenfurthii) for digging out a bee-nest in the Kahuzi-Biega National Park, Zaire. Primates29: 405–411.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×