Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-rkxrd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-16T15:58:45.568Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

23 - Information structure

from Part V - The interfaces

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2016

Enric Vallduví
Affiliation:
Universitat Pompeu Fabra
Maria Aloni
Affiliation:
Universiteit van Amsterdam
Paul Dekker
Affiliation:
Universiteit van Amsterdam
Get access

Summary

Introduction

The term information structure is often used as a convenient cover expression for a bundle of phenomena – referred to by (back)ground, comment, contrast, focus, given, new, rheme, theme, topic and associated terms – that exhibit pragmatic, semantic,morphosyntactic and phonological features. It is clear that information structure affects content, and in particular that it concerns context-sensitive aspects of content, but it is not universally agreed whether information structure forms a distinct dimension within the interpretative component of language.

In this chapter, for expository purposes and as in Kruijff-Korbayová and Steedman (2003), the term information structure is used broadly as encompassing utterance-level features of both a semantic and a structural nature. The discussion will be centred around the following information-structural notions: the theme–rheme distinction (Section 23.3), givenness and topic (Section 23.4), and contrast (Section 23.5). These descriptive notions allow linguists to go a long way in analysing phenomena that have generally been thought of as concerning information structure. The facts concerning focus, one of the most (ab)used labels in information structure research, will be discussed in connection with the notions contrast and theme–rheme.

As a first approximation to information structure, consider (1) (small caps identify the lexical item associated with nuclear prominence):

(1)

  1. a. We like hokey-pokey.

  2. b. Hokey-pokey we like.

  3. c. Hokey-pokey we hate.

  4. d. We like hokey-pokey.

Examples (1a) and (1b) have identical truth conditions but still differ interpretatively; there is something in the content, understood broadly, of (1a) which sets it apart from the synonymous (1b); this interpretative difference correlates with the difference in word order. Interestingly, (1b) and (1c) display a certain interpretative equivalence which is obviously not connected to their truth-conditional meaning; this interpretative equivalence correlates with the structural ‘sameness’ that they display. Both the interpretative difference between (1a) and (1b) and the partial interpretative equivalence between (1b) and (1c) are taken to be information structural in nature. There is also a difference in (non-truth-conditional) content between (1a) and (1d), which is of a nature similar to the difference between (1a) and (1b); here, however, the structural contrast associated with the interpretative difference is not in the word order but rather in the intonation. In English and many other languages, if not all, intonation is an important correlate of information-structural content.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Information structure
  • Edited by Maria Aloni, Universiteit van Amsterdam, Paul Dekker, Universiteit van Amsterdam
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Formal Semantics
  • Online publication: 05 July 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139236157.024
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Information structure
  • Edited by Maria Aloni, Universiteit van Amsterdam, Paul Dekker, Universiteit van Amsterdam
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Formal Semantics
  • Online publication: 05 July 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139236157.024
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Information structure
  • Edited by Maria Aloni, Universiteit van Amsterdam, Paul Dekker, Universiteit van Amsterdam
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Formal Semantics
  • Online publication: 05 July 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139236157.024
Available formats
×