Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-lrf7s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-26T00:51:11.706Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Attention and Creativity

from Part III - Attention and Imagination

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 January 2018

Rex E. Jung
Affiliation:
University of New Mexico
Oshin Vartanian
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abraham, A., Beudt, S., Ott, D. V. M., & von Cramon, D. R. (2012). Creative cognition and the brain: Dissociations between frontal, parietal–temporal and basal ganglia groups. Brain Research, 1482, 5570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aiello, J., De Risi, D., Epstein, Y., & Katin, R. (1977). Crowding and the role of interpersonal distance preference. Sociometry, 40, 271282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. M. (1985). Motivation and creativity: Effects of motivational orientation on creative writers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 393399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. M., Goldfarb, P., & Brackfield, S. (1990). Social influences on creativity: Evaluation, coaction, and surveillance. Creativity Research Journal, 3, 621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, F. M., & Farris, G. F. (1972). Time pressure and performance of scientists and engineers: A five-year panel study. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 8, 185200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ansburg, P. I., & Hill, K. (2003). Creative and analytic thinkers differ in their use of attentional resources. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 11411152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 779806.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banich, M. T. (2004). Cognitive neuroscience and neuropsychology. New York, NY: Houghton Mufflin Company.Google Scholar
Beaty, R. E., & Silvia, P. J. (2012). Why do ideas get more creative across time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect in divergent thinking tasks. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 309319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaty, R. E., Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Jauk, E., & Benedek, M. (2014). The role of associative and executive processes in creative cognition. Memory & Cognition, 42, 11861197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedek, M., Franz, F., Heene, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 480485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., Ebner, F., & Neubauer, A. C. (2014). To create or to recall? Neural mechanisms underlying the generation of creative new ideas. NeuroImage, 88, 125133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedek, M., Könen, T., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Associative abilities underlying creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 273281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bond, C. F., & Titus, L. J. (1983). Social facilitation: A meta-analysis of 241 studies. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 265292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowden, E., & Beeman, M. (1998). Getting the right idea: Semantic activation in the right hemisphere may help solve insight problems. Psychological Science, 9, 435440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carson, S. H. (2011). Creativity and psychopathology: A shared vulnerability model. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56, 144153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 499506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chrysikou, E. G., & Thompson- Schill, S. L. (2011). Dissociable brain states linked to common and creative object use. Human Brain Mapping, 32, 665675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornish, K. M., Manly, T., Savage, R., Swanson, J., Morisano, D., Butler, N., … Hollis, C. P. (2005). Association of the dopamine transporter (DAT1) 10/10-repeat genotype with ADHD symptoms and response inhibition in a general population sample. Molecular Psychiatry, 10, 686698.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cottrell, N. B., Wack, D. L., Sekerak, G. J., & Rittle, R. H. (1968). Social facilitation of dominant responses by the presence of an audience and the mere presence of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 245250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curie, E., & Sheean, V. (2001). Madame Curie: A biography. New York, NY: Da Capo Press.Google Scholar
De Dreu, C. K. W., Nijstad, B. A., Baas, M., Wosink, I., & Roskes, M. (2012). Working memory benefits creative insight, musical improvisation, and original ideation through maintained task-focused attention. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 656669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dietrich, A., & Kanso, R. (2010). A review of EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies of creativity and insight. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 822848.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 74, 1628.Google Scholar
Evans, G. W. (1979). Behavioral and physical consequences of crowding in humans. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 9, 2746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feist, G. J. (1999). The influence of personality on artistic and scientific creativity. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 273296). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fink, A., & Benedek, M. (2014). EEG alpha power and creative ideation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 44, 111123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fink, A, Grabner, R. H., Benedek, M., Reishofer, G., Hauswirth, V., Fally, M., … & Neubauer, A. C. (2009). The creative brain: Investigation of brain activity during creative problem solving by means of EEG and fMRI. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 734748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fink, A., Grabner, R. H., Gebauer, D., Reishofer, G., Koschutnig, K., & Ebner, F. (2010). Enhancing creativity by means of cognitive stimulation: Evidence from an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 52, 16871695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Hutterer, L., Steiner, E., Benedek, M., Weber, B., … & Weiss, E. M. (2014). Gray matter density in relation to different facets of verbal creativity. Brain Structure and Function, 219, 12631269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fink, A., Weber, B., Koschutnig, K., Benedek, M., Reishofer, G., Ebner, F., … & Weiss, E. M. (2014). Creativity and schizotypy from the neuroscience perspective. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, 378387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, J. R., & van Pettern, C. (2008). Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: A review. Psychophysiology, 45, 152170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fukuda, K., & Vogel, E. K. (2011). Individual differences in recovery time from attention capture. Psychological Science, 22, 361368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabora, L. (2010). Revenge of the “Neurds:” Characterizing creative thought in terms of the structure and dynamics of memory. Creativity Research Journal, 22, 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia-Garcia, M., Barceló, F., Clemente, , , I. C., & Escera, C. (2010). The role of the dopamine transporter DAT1 genotype on the neural correlates of cognitive flexibility. European Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 754760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gjini, K., Burroughs, S., & Boutros, N. N. (2011). Relevance of attention in auditory sensory gating paradigm in schizophrenia. Journal of Psychophysiology, 25, 6066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilhooly, K. J., Fioratou, E., Anthony, S. H., & Wynn, V. (2007). Divergent thinking: Strategies and executive involvement in generating novel uses for familiar objects. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 611625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonen-Yaacovi, G., de Souza, L. C., Levy, R., Urbanski, M., Josse, G., & Volle, E. (2013). Rostral and caudal prefrontal contributions to creativity: A meta-analysis of functional imaging data. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gusnard, D. A., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 685694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jauk, E., Neubauer, A. C., Dunst, B., Fink, A., & Benedek, M. (2015). Gray matter correlates of creative potential: A latent variable voxel-based morphometry study. NeuroImage, 111, 312320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jung, R. E., Segall, J. M., Bockholt, H. J., Flores, R. A., Smith, S. M., Chavez, R. S., & Haier, R. (2010). Neuroanatomy of creativity. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 398409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karau, S. J., & Kelly, J. R. (1992). The effects of time scarcity and time abundance on group performance quality and interaction process. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 28, 542572.Google Scholar
Kasof, J. (1997). Creativity and breadth of attention. Creativity Research Journal, 10, 303315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kozbelt, A. (2008). Hierarchical liner modeling of creative artists’ problem solving behaviors. Journal of Creative Behavior, 42, 181200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, C. S., & Therriault, D. J. (2013). The cognitive underpinnings of creative thought: A latent variable analysis exploring the roles of intelligence and working memory in three creative thinking processes. Intelligence, 41, 306320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lester, T. (2012). Da Vinci’s ghost: Genius, obsession, and how Leonardo created the world in his own image. New York, NY: Free Press.Google Scholar
Martindale, C. (1995). Creativity and connectionism. In Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Finke, R. A. (Eds.), The creative cognition approach (pp. 249268). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Martindale, C. (2001). Oscillations and analogies: Thomas Young, MD, FRS, genius. American Psychologist, 56, 342345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martindale, C., Anderson, K., Moore, K., & West, A. N. (1996). Creativity, oversensitivity, and rate of habituation. Personality and Individual Differences, 4, 423427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martindale, C., & Armstrong, J. (1974). The relationship of creativity to cortical activation and its operant control. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 124, 311320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matlin, M, & Zajonc, R. (1968). Social facilitation of word associates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 455460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 220232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendelsohn, G., & Griswold, B. (1964). Differential use of incidental stimuli in problem solving as a function of creativity. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 431436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milgram, R. M., & Miigram, N. A. (1976). Group versus individual administration in the measurement of creative thinking in gifted and nongifted children. Child Development, 47, 563565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagar, D., & Pandey, I. (1987). Affect and performance on cognitive task as a function of crowding and noise. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 17, 147157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nijstad, B. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., Rietzchel, E. F., & Baas, M. (2010). The dual pathway to creativity model: Creative ideation as a function of flexibility and persistence. European Review of Social Psychology, 21, 3477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nusbaum, E. C., & Silvia, P. J. (2011). Are intelligence and creativity really so different? Fluid intelligence, executive processes, and strategy use in divergent thinking. Intelligence, 39, 3645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olincy, A., Braff, D. L., Adler, L. E., Cadenhead, K. S., Calkins, M. E., Dobie, D. J., … Freedman, R. (2010). Inhibition of the P50 cerebral evoked response to repeated auditory stimuli: Results from the Consortium on Genetics of Schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 119, 175182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patterson, J. V., Hetrick, W. P., Boutros, N. N., Jin, Y., Sandman, C., Stern, H., … Bunney Jr., W. E. (2008). P50 sensory gating ratios in schizophrenics and controls: A review and data analysis. Psychiatry Research, 158, 226247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I. (1988). Structures and functions of selective attention. In Boll, T. & Bryant, B. (Eds.), Master lectures in clinical neuropsychology and brain function: Research, measurement, and practice (pp. 171202). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Rawlings, D. (1985). Psychoticism, creativity, and dichotic listening. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 737742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richards, R., Kinney, D.K., Binet, M., & Merzel, A.P. (1988). Assessing everyday creativity: Characteristics of the Lifetime Creativity Scales and validation with three large samples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 476485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. (2005). Motivation, competence, and creativity. In Elliot, A. J. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 609623). New York, NY: Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Acar, S. (2012). Divergent thinking as an indicator of creative achievement. Creativity Research Journal, 24, 6675.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. (1976). Utilization of irrelevant information by high and low creatives. Psychological Reports, 39, 105106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sawyer, R. K. (2011). The cognitive neuroscience of creativity: A critical review. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 137154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Beaty, R. E., & Nusbaum, E. C. (2013). Verbal fluency and creativity: General and specific contributions of broad retrieval ability (Gr) factors to divergent thinking. Intelligence, 41, 328340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Beaty, R. E., Nusbaum, E. C., Eddington, K. M., & Kwapil, T. R. (2014). Creative motivation: Creative achievement predicts cardiac autonomic markers of effort during divergent thinking. Biological Psychology, 102, 3037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (2002). The concept of creativity: Prospects and paradigms. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 315). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Süß, H. M., Oberauer, , Wittman, K., Wilhelm, W. W., , O., & Schulze, R. (2002). Working-memory capacity explains reasoning ability – And a little bit more. Intelligence, 30, 261288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Hashizume, H., Sassa, Y., Nagase, T., Nouchi, R., & Kawashima, R. (2011). Failing to deactivate: The association between brain activity during a working memory task and creativity. NeuroImage, 55, 681687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Sassa, Y., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A., Fukushima, A., & Kawashima, R. (2010). Regional gray matter volume of dopaminergic system associate with creativity: Evidence from voxel-based morphometry. NeuroImage, 51, 578585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torrance, E. P. (1969). Creativity. What research says to the teacher. Washington, DC: National Education Association.Google Scholar
Torrance, E. P. (1974). The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking-Norms – Technical manual research edition, figural tests, Forms A and B. Princeton, NJ: Personnel Press.Google Scholar
van den Heuvel, M. P., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2010). Exploring the brain network: A review of resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 20, 519534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vartanian, O. (2009). Variable attention facilitates creative problem solving. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 5759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vartanian, O., Martindale, C., & Kwiatkowski, J. (2007). Creative potential, attention, and speed of information processing. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 14701480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallach, M. A., & Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young children: A study of the creativity–intelligence distinction. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Google Scholar
Wallas, G (1926) The art of thought. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
Wiley, J., & Jarosz, A. (2012). Working memory capacity, attentional focus, and problem solving. Psychological Science, 21, 258262.Google Scholar
Zabelina, D. L., & Andrews-Hanna, J. (2016). Dynamic network interactions supporting internally oriented cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 40, 8693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zabelina, D. L., Colzato, L., Beeman, M., & Hommel, B. (2016). Dopamine and the creative mind: Individual differences in everyday creative performance are predicted by interactions between dopamine genes DAT and COMT. PLoS ONE, 11, e0146768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zabelina, D. L., Condon, D., & Beeman, M. (2014). Do dimensional psychopathology measures relate to divergent thinking or creative achievement? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zabelina, D. L., & Ganis, G. (2017). Divergent thinking, but not real-life creative achievement, relates to better cognitive control, as reflected in the N2 ERP. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Zabelina, D. L., O’Leary, D., Pornpattananangkul, N., Nusslock, R., & Beeman, M. (2015). Creativity and sensory gating indexed by the P50: Selective versus leaky sensory gating in divergent thinkers and creative achievers. Neuropsychologia, 69, 7784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zabelina, D. L., & Robinson, M. D. (2010). Creativity as flexible cognitive control. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4, 136143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zabelina, D. L., Saporta, A., & Beeman, M. (2016). Flexible or leaky attention in creative people? Distinct patterns of attention for different types of creative thinking. Memory & Cognition, 44, 488498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×