Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-sv6ng Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-16T12:17:24.996Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false
This chapter is part of a book that is no longer available to purchase from Cambridge Core

References

Lance Workman
Affiliation:
Bath Spa University
Will Reader
Affiliation:
Sheffield Hallam University
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Evolutionary Psychology
An Introduction
, pp. 379 - 404
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, D.et al. (1986). Seville statement on violence. American Psychologist (publication date 1990). 45(10), 167–8Google Scholar
Aiello, L. C. (1993). The fossil evidence for modern human origins in Africa: A revised view. American Anthropologist. 95, 73–96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ainsworth, M. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant Care and the Growth of Love. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press
Aitchison, J. (1989). The Articulate Mammal (4th edn). London: Routledge
Akhtar, N. and Tomasello, M. (1997). Young children's productivity with word order and verb morphology. Developmental Psychology. 33, 952–65CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alcock, J. (1998). Animal Behaviour: An Evolutionary Approach (6th edn). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer
Alcock, J. (2001). The Triumph of Sociobiology. New York: Oxford University Press
Alexander, R. D. (1974). The evolution of social behaviour. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 5, 325–83CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexander, R. D. (1987). The Biology of Moral Systems. New York: Aldine de Gruyter
Alexander, R. D. (1980). Darwinism and Human Affairs. Seattle: University of Washington Press
Alexander, R. D. and Noonan, K. M. (1979). Concealment of ovulation, parental care and human social evolution. In N. A. Chagnon and W. Irons (eds.). Evolutionary Biology and Human Social Behaviour (402–35). North Scituate MA: Duxbury Press
Alexander, R. D., Hoodland, J. L., Howard, R. D., Noonan, K. M. and Sherman, P. W. (1979). Sexual dimorphisms and breeding systems in pinnepeds, ungulates, primates and humans. In N. A. Chagnon and W. Irons (eds.). Evolutionary Biology and Human Social Behaviour (436–53). North Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press
Anderson, J. R. (1990). The Adaptive Character of Thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Anderson, J. R. and Milson, R. (1989). Human memory: An adaptive perspective. Psychological Review. 96(4), 703–19CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersson, M. (1982). Female choice selects for extreme tail length in a widow bird. Nature. 299, 818–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersson, M. (1986). Evolution of condition-dependent sex ornaments and mating preferences: sexual selection based on viability differences. Evolution. 40, 804–16CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrew, R. J. (1963a). The origins and evolution of the calls and facial expressions of the primates. Behaviour. 20, 1–109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrew, R. J. (1963b). Evolution of facial expressions. Science. 142, 1034–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angrist, B., Lee, H. K. and Gershon, S. (1974). The antagonism of amphetamine-induced symptomology by a neuroleptic. American Journal of Psychiatry. 131, 817–19CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Archer, J. (1996). Evolutionary Social Psychology. In M. Hewstone, S. Wolfgang and G. M. Stephenson (eds.). Introduction to Social Psychology (24–45). Oxford: Blackwell
Archer, J. (2001). Evolving theories of behaviour. The Psychologist. 14, 414–19Google Scholar
Ardrey, R. (1961). African Genesis: A Personal Investigation into the Animal Origins and Nature of Man. New York: Dell
Aunger, R. A. (2000). Darwinizing Culture. The Status of Memetics as a Science. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Averill, J. R. (1980). On the paucity of positive emotions. In K. R. Blankstein, P. Pliner and J. Polivy (eds.). Advances in the Study of Communication and Affect. Vol. 6, Assessment and Modification of Emotional Behaviour (7–45). New York: PlenumCrossRef
Axelrod, R. (1984). The Evolution of Co-operation. New York: Basic Books
Axelrod, R. and Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of co-operation. Science. 211, 1390–6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Badcock, C. (1991). Evolution and Individual Behaviour: An Introduction to Human Sociobiology. Oxford: Blackwell
Badcock, C. (2000). Evolutionary Psychology: A Critical Introduction. Malden, MA: Blackwell
Baillargeon, R. (1986). Representing the existence and the location of hidden objects: Object permanence in six- and eight-month-old infants. Cognition. 23, 21–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (1991). Reasoning about the height and location of a hidden object in 4.5- and 6.5-month-old infants. Cognition. 38, 13–42CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, R. R. and Bellis, M. A. (1993). Number of sperm in human ejaculates varies in accordance with Sperm Competition. Animal Behaviour. 37, 867–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, R. R. and Bellis, M. A. (1995). Human Sperm Competition. London: Chapman and Hall
Baldwin, D. A., Markman, E. M., Bill, B., Desjardins, R. N., Irwin, J. and Tidball, G. (1996). Infants' reliance on a social criterion for establishing word–object relations. Child Development. 67, 3135–53CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (eds.) (1992). The Adapted Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child's theory of mind: a case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 30, 285–97CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: Essays in Autism and Theory of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). The Maladapted Mind: Classic Readings in Evolutionary Psychopathology. London: Psychology Press
Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C. and Robertson, M. M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 37, 813–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M. and Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’?Cognition. 21, 37–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L. and Dunbar, R. I. M. (1994). Not now dear, I'm busy. New Scientist. 142, 30–4Google Scholar
Barrett, L. Dunbar, R. I. M. and Lycett, J. (2002). Human Evolutionary Psychology. New York: Palgrave
Bartlett, F. (1932). Remembering. New York: Cambridge University Press
Basolo, A. L. (1990). Female preference predates the evolution of the sword in swordtail fish. Science. 250, 808–10CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basolo, A. L. (1995). Phylogenetic evidence for the role of a pre-existing bias in sexual selection. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B. 265, 2223–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P. (2000). Taking the stink out of instinct. In H. Rose and S. Rose (eds.). Alas Poor Darwin: Arguments Against Evolutionary Psychology (157–73). London: Jonathan Cape
Bateson, P., Mendl, M. and Feaver, J. (1990). Play in the domestic cat is enhanced by rationing of the mother during lactation. Animal Behaviour. 40, 514–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, G. (1982). The Masterpiece of Nature. London: Croom Helm
Bellugi, U., Bihrle, A., Jernigan, T., Trauner, D. and Doherty, S. (1990). Neuropsychological, neurological, and neuroanatomical profile of Williams syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics Supplement. 6, 115–25Google ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J. (1997). Attachment, mating, and parenting: An evolutionary interpretation. Human Nature. 8, 361–81CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L. and Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal development and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Development. 62(4), 647–70CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, T. (2000). Social causes and natural relations. In H. Rose and S. Rose (eds.). Alas Poor Darwin: Arguments Against Evolutionary Psychology (206–24). London: Jonathan Cape
Berko, J. (1958). The child's learning of English morphology. Word. 14(2–3), 150–77CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berndt, T. J. (1986). Children's comments about their friendships. In M. Perlmutter (ed.). Minnesota Symposia in Child Development. Vol. 18, Cognitive Perspectives on Children's Social and Behavioral Development (189–212). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Beroldi, G. (1994). Critique of the Seville statement on violence. American Psychologist. 49, 847–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Betzig, L. (1997). Human Nature: A Critical Reader. New York: Oxford University Press
Bierhoff, H. W. (1996). Heterosexual partnerships: initiation, maintenance and disengagement. In A. E. Auhagen and M. V. Salisch (eds.). The Diversity of Human Relationships (173–96). Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Bishop, D. V. M., North, T. and Donlan, C. (1995). Genetic basis of specific language impairment. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 37, 56–71CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J., Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. S. and Dolan, R. J. (1999). Dissociable neural responses to facial expressions of sadness and anger. Brain. 122, 883–93CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, N. (1995). How heritability misleads about race. Cognition. 56, 99–128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boden, M. A. (1994). Piaget. London: Fontana
Bower, T. G. R. (1974). Development in Infancy. San Francisco: Freeman
Bowlby, J. (1951). Maternal Care and Mental Health. Geneva: WHO
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and Loss. Vol. 1, Attachment. New York: Basic Books
Boyd, R. and Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago: Chicago University Press
Boyd, R. and Silk, J. B. (2000). How Humans Evolved. New York: Norton
Breland, K. and Breland, M. (1961). The misbehavior of organisms. American Psychologist. 16, 681–4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brennan, K. A., Shaver, P. and Tobey, A. E. (1991). Attachment styles, gender, and parental problem drinking. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 8, 451–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, M. B. and Crano, W. D. (1994). Social Psychology. New York: West Publishing Co
Brown, D. E. (1991). Human Universals. New York: McGraw-Hill
Brown, R. (1973). A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Buck, R. (1988). Human Motivation and Emotion (2nd edn). New York: Wiley
Burnstein, E., Crandell, C. and Kitayama, S. (1994). Some neo-Darwinian decision rules for altruism: weighing cues for inclusive fitness as a function of the biological importance of the decision. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 67, 773–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bushnell, I. W. R., Sai, F. and Mullin, J. T. (1989). Neonatal recognition of the mother's face. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 7, 3–15CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 12, 1–49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buss, D. M. (1995). The Evolution of Desire. New York: Basic Books
Buss, D. M. (1997). The emergence of evolutionary social psychology. In J. A. Simpson and D. T. Kenrick (eds.). Evolutionary Social Psychology (21–48). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
Buss, D. M. (1999). Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind. Boston: Allyn and Bacon
Buss, D. M. and Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: an evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review. 100, 204–32CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, D. M.et al. (1990). International preferences in selecting mates: a study of 37 cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 21, 5–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W. (1995). The Thinking Ape: Evolutionary Origins of Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Calder, A. J., Young, A. W., Perret, D. I., Etcoff, N. L. and Rowland, D. (1996). Categorical perception of morphed facial expressions. Visual Cognition. 3, 81–117CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callender, L. A. (1988). Gregor Mendel: An opponent of descent with modification. History of Science. 26, 41–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caporael, L. R. and Baron, R. M. (1997). Groups as the mind's natural environment. In J. A. Simpsons and D. T. Kenrick (eds.). Evolutionary Social Psychology (21–48). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
Carey, S. and Spelke, E. S. (1994). Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change. In L. Hirschfeld and S. Gelman (eds.). Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture (169–200). Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Carlson, N. R. (2002). Foundations of Physiological Psychology. Boston: Allyn and Bacon
Carlson, N. R., Buskist, W. and Martin, G. N. (2000). Psychology: The Science of Behaviour. London: Allyn and Bacon
Casdan, E. (1996). Women's mating strategies. Evolutionary Anthropology. 5, 134–433.0.CO;2-G>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casscells, W., Schoenberger, A. and Grayboys, T. (1978). Interpretation by physicians of clinical laboratory results. New England Journal of Medicine. 299, 999–1000CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1991). Genes, peoples and languages. Scientific American. 265(5), 72–8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chagnon, N. A. (1988). Yanomamo: The Fierce People (4th edn). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston
Chagnon, N. A. (1992). Yanomamo: The Fierce People (5th edn). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston
Chagnon, N. A. (1997). Yanomamö (6th edn). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston
Charlesworth, W. and Dzur, C. (1987). Gender comparisons of preschoolers' behavior and resource utilization in group problem solving. Child Development. 58, 191–200CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1982). How vervet monkeys perceive their grunts: Field playback experiments. Animal Behaviour. 30, 739–51CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chisholm, J. S. (1996). The evolutionary ecology of attachment organization. Human Native. 7, 1–38Google ScholarPubMed
Chisholm, J. S. (1999). Death, Hope and Sex: Steps to an Evolutionary Ecology of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic Structures. The Hague: Mouton
Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of B. F. Skinner's ‘Verbal Behaviour’. Language. 35, 26–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chua, S. E. and McKenna, P. J. (1995). Schizophrenia – a brain disease? A critical review of abnormalities of structural and functional cerebral abnormality in the disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry. 166, 563–82CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clamp, A. (2001). Evolutionary Psychology. London: Hodder Headline
Clark, R. and Hatfield, E. (1989). Gender differences in receptivity to sexual offers. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality. 2, 39–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cloak, F. T. (1975). Is a cultural ethology possible?Human Ecology. 3, 161–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clutton-Brock, T. H. and Harvey, P. (1977). Primate ecological social organization. Journal of Zoology. 183, 1–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clutton-Brock, T. H., Guinness, F. E. and Albon, S. D. (1982). Red Deer: Behaviour and Ecology of Two Sexes. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Connellan, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Ba'tki, A. and Ahluwalia, J. (2001). Sex differences in human neonatal social perception. Infant Behavior and Development. 23, 113–18CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coryell, W. A. (1980). A blind family history study of Briquet's syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry. 37, 1266–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies from the Wason selection task. Cognition. 31, 187–276CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides and J. Tooby (eds.). The Adapted Mind (163–228). New York: Oxford University Press
Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (1994). Origins of domain specificity: The evolution of functional organization. In L. A. Hirschfeld and S. A. Gelman (eds.). Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture (85–116). Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (1996). Are humans rational thinkers after all? Rethinking some conclusions from the literature on judgement under uncertainty. Cognition. 58, 1–73CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, C. R. and Boeuf, B. J. (1977). Female incitation of male competition: A mechanism of mate selection. American Naturalist. 111, 317–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crago, M. B. and Gopnik, M. (1994). From families to phenotypes: theoretical and clinical implications of research into the genetic basis of specific language impairment. In R. Watkins and M. Rice (eds.). Specific Language Impairments in Children (35–51). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes
Crain, S. and Nakayama, M. (1986). Structure dependence in children's language. Language. 62, 522–43Google Scholar
Crawford, C., Smith, M. and Krebs, D. (1987). Sociobiology and Psychology: Ideas, Issues and Applications. London: Lawrence Erlbaum
Crews, D. (1994). Animal sexuality. Scientific American. 270, 108–14CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cronin, H. (1991). The Ant and the Peacock: Altruism and Sexual Selection from Darwin to Today, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Cross, J. E. and Cross, J. (1971). Age, sex, race and the perception of facial beauty. Developmental Psychology. 5, 433–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crow, T. J. (1995). A Darwinian approach to the origins of psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry. 167, 12–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowell, J. A. and Feldman, S. S. (1988). The effects of mothers' internal models of relationships and children's behavioral and developmental status on mother–child interaction. Child Development. 59, 1273–85CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowell, J. A. and Feldman, S. S. (1991). Mothers' working models of relationships and child behavior during separation and reunion. Developmental Psychology. 27, 597–605CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummins, D. D. and Cummins, R. (1999). Biological preparedness and evolutionary explanation. Cognition. 73, 37–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daly, M. and Wilson, M. (1983). Sex, Evolution and Behaviour (2nd edn). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth
Daly, M. and Wilson, M. (1984). A sociobiological analysis of human infanticide. In G. Hausfater and S. B. Hardy (eds.). Infanticide: Comparative and Evolutionary Perspectives (487–502). New York: Aldine
Daly, M. and Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. New York: Aldine de Gruyter
Daly, M. and Wilson, M. (1994). Evolutionary psychology of male violence. In J. Archer (ed.). Male Violence (253–88). London: Routledge
Damasio, A. R. (2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, Sorrow and the Feeling Brain. Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace College Publishers
Damasio, H., Grabowski, T., Frank, R., Galaburda, A. M. and Damasio, A. R. (1994). The return of Phineas Gage: clues about the brain from the skull of a famous patient. Science. 264, 1102–5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darwin, C. (1859). On the Origin of Species by Natural Selection. London: Murray
Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: Murray
Darwin, C. (1872). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: HarperCollins
Darwin, C. (1877). A biographical sketch of an infant. Mind. 2, 285–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1998). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals (3rd edn, with Introduction and Afterword by Paul Ekman). London: HarperCollins (originally published in 1872)
Darwin, L. (1925). Race deterioration and practical politics. Eugenics Review. 141–3Google ScholarPubMed
Davidson, R. J. and Sutton, S. K. (1995). Affective neuroscience: The emergence of a discipline. Special Cognitive Neuroscience issue for Current Opinions in Neurobiology. 5, 217–24CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davison, G. C. and Neale, J. M. (2001). Abnormal Psychology. Chichester: Wiley
Dawkins, R. (1976; 1989). The Selfish Gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Dawkins, R. (1979a). Twelve misunderstandings of kin selection. Zeitschrift fŸr Tierpsychologie. 51, 184–200Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1979b). In defence of selfish genes. Philosophy. 56, 556–73CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1982). The Extended Phenotype. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Dawkins, R. (1986). The Blind Watchmaker. Harlow: Longman
Dawkins, R. (1995). River Out of Eden. New York: Basic Books
Dawkins, R. (2003). A Devil's Chaplain: Reflections on Hope, Lies, Science, and Love. London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson
Dawson, A. and Tylee, A. (2001). Depression: Social and Economic Timebomb. London: BMJ Books
Deacon, T. W. (1997). The Symbolic Species: The Co-Evolution of Language and the Brain. London: Penguin
Deaux, K. and Wrightsman, L. (1983). Social Psychology (4th edn). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole
Degler, C. (1991). In Search of Human Nature: The Decline and Revival of Darwinism in American Social Thought. New York: Oxford University Press
Dennett, D. C. (1995). Darwin's Dangerous Idea: Evolution and the Meanings of Life. New York: Simon and Schuster
Dennett, D. C. (1996). Kinds of Minds: Towards an Understanding of Consciousness. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson
Dennett, D. C. (1999). The evolution of culture. The Edge. Retrieved from http://www.edge.org/3rd_culture/dennett/dennett_p1.html on 28/8/03
Derryberry, D. and Tucker, D. M. (1994). Motivating the focus of attention. In P. M. Neidenlthale and S. Kitayama (eds.). The Heart's Eye: Emotional Influences in Perception and Attention (167–96). San Diego: Academic PressCrossRef
Diamond, J. (1992). The Third Chimpanzee. New York: HarperCollins
Diamond, J. (1997). Why is Sex Fun? New York: Basic Books
Diamond, J. (1998). Guns, Germs and Steel: A Short History of Everybody for the Last 13,000 Years. London: Vintage
Dion, K. K. (1972). Physical attractiveness and evaluation of children's transgressions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 24, 207–13CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Draper, P. and Belsky, J. (1990). Personality development in evolutionary perspective. Journal of Personality. 58, 141–57CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Draper, P. and Harpending, H. (1982). Father absence and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Anthropological Research. 38(3), 255–73CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drickamer, L. C. and Vessey, S. H. (1992). Animal Behaviour: Mechanisms, Ecology and Evolution (3rd edn). Dubuque: Wm C. Brown
Drickamer, L. C. and Vessey, S. H. (1996). Animal Behaviour: Mechanisms, Ecology and Evolution (4th edn). Dubuque: Wm C. Brown
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1988). Primate Social Systems. London: Croom Helm
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Science. 16, 681–735CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996). The Trouble with Science. London: Faber and Faber
Eagly, A. H. and Steffan, V. J. (1986). Gender and aggressive behavior: a meta-analytic review of the social psychological literature. Psychological Bulletin. 100, 283–308CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ebstein, R., Novick, O., Umansky, R., Priel, B., Osher, Y., Blaine, D., Bennett, E. R., Nemanov, L., Katz, M. and Belmaker, R. H. (1996). D4DR exon polymorphism associated with the personality trait of novelty seeking in normal human volunteers. Nature Genetics. 12, 78–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1984). Human Ethology. New York: Aldine de Gruyter
Ekman, P. (1980). The Face of Man: Expressions of Universal Emotions in a New Guinea Village. New York: Garland STPM Press
Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion. 6, 169–200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. (1994). Are there basic emotions? In P. Ekman and R. Davidson (eds.). The Nature of Emotions: Functional Questions (15–19). Oxford: Oxford University Press
Ekman, P. (1998). Charles Darwin: The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals (3rd edn), with Introduction, Afterwords and Commentaries by Paul Ekman). London: HarperCollins
Ekman, P. and Friesen, W. V. (1967). Hand and body cues in the judgement of emotion: A reformulation. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 24, 711–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. and Friesen, W. V. (1969). The repertoire of nonverbal behaviour: Categories, origins, usage and coding. Semiotics. 1, 49–98Google Scholar
Ekman, P. and Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 17, 124–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V. and Ellsworth, P. C. (1972). Emotions in the Human Face: Guidelines for Research and an Integration of Findings. New York: Pergamon Press
Elder, G. H. (1969). Appearance and education in marriage mobility. American Sociological Review. 34, 519–33CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eldridge, N. and Gould, S. (1972). Punctuated equilibria: An alternative to phyletic gradualism (82–115). In J. M. Schopf (ed.). Models in Paleobiology. San Francisco: Freeman
Ellis, L. (1996). A discipline in peril: Sociology's future hinges on curing its biophobia. American Sociologist. 27, 21–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellsberg, D. (1961). Risk, ambiguity, and the Savage axioms. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 75, 643–69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Estioko-Griffin, A. and Griffin, P. B. (1981). Woman the hunter: The Agta. In F. Dahlberg (ed.). Woman the Gatherer (121–51). New Haven: Yale University Press
Evans, J. St. B. T. and Over, D. (1996). Rationality and Reasoning. Hove: Psychology Press
Ewald, P. W. (1994). Evolution of Infectious Disease. New York: Oxford University Press
Fagen, R. (1981). Animal Play Behaviour. New York: Oxford University Press
Fantz, R. L. (1961). The origin of form perception. Scientific American. 204, 66–72CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M. J., Rabinowitz, C., Quinn, G. E. and Liu, G. T. (2000). Early commitment of neural substrates for face recognition. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 17, 117–24CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faris, E. (1921). Are instincts data or hypotheses?American Journal of Sociology. 27, 184–98CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feeney, J. A. and Noller, P. (1992). Attachment style and romantic love: Relationship dissolution. Australian Journal of Psychology. 44(2), 69–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, R. A. (1930). The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection. Oxford: Clarendon Press
Fisher, R. A. (1958). The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection (2nd edn). New York: Dover
Flinn, M. V. (1989). Household composition and female strategies in a Trinidadian village. In A. E. Rasa and C. Vogeland Voland (eds.). The Sociobiology of Sexual and Reproductive Strategies (206–33). New York: Chapman and Hall
Fodor, J. (1983). The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Fodor, J., Bever, T. and Garrett, M. (1974). The Psychology of Language. New York: McGraw-Hill
Ford, C. S. and Beach, F. A. (1951). Patterns of Sexual Behaviour. New York: Harper and Row
Fouts, R. S., Fouts, D. H. and Schoenfeld, D. (1984). Sign language conversational interactions between chimpanzees. Sign Language Studies. 42, 1–12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, R. (1988). On the Seville statement on violence. Human Ethology Newsletter. 5(5), 4Google Scholar
Frangiskakis, J. M., Ewart, A., Morris, C. A., Mervis, C. B., Bertrand, J., Robinson, B. F., Klein, B. P., Ensing, G., Everett, L. A., Green, E. D., Proschel, C., Gutowski, N. J., Noble, M., Atkinson, D. L., Odelberg, S. J. and Keating, M. T. (1996). LIM-kinase1 hemizygosity implicated in impaired visuospatial constructive cognition. Cell. 86, 59–69CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions?Review of General Psychology. 2, 300–19CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D. G. (1983). Margaret Mead and Samoa: The Making and Unmaking of an Anthropological Myth. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Fridlund, A. J. (1992). Darwin's anti-Darwinism and the expression of the emotions in man and animals. In K. T. Strongman (ed.). International Review of Emotion. Vol. 2 (117–37). New York: Wiley
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Galef, B. J. Jr, Manzig, L. A. and Field, R. M. (1986). Imitation learning in budgerigars: Dawson and Foss (1965) revisited. Behavioral Processes. 13, 191–202CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galton, F. (1864). Hereditary talent and character. MacMillan's Magazine. 11, 157–66Google Scholar
Galton, F. (1908). Memories of My Life. London: Methuen
Gangestad, S. W. and Buss, D. M. (1993). Pathogen prevalences and human mate preferences. Ethology and Sociobiology. 14, 89–96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gangestad, S. W. and Simpson, J. A. (1990). Toward an evolutionary history of females' sociosexual variation. Journal of Personality. 58, 69–96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, M. (2000). Kilroy was here. Review of The Meme Machine by Susan J. Blackmore. Los Angeles Times, 5 March
Ge, X., Conger, R. D., Cadoret, R. J., Neiderhiser, J. M., Yates, W.et al. (1996). The developmental interface between nature and nurture: A mutual influence model of child antisocial behaviour and parent behaviours. Developmental Psychology. 32, 574–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (1991). How to make cognitive illusions disappear. Beyond heuristics and biases. In W. Stroebe and M. Hewstone (eds.). European Review of Social Psychology. Vol. 2 (83–115). Chichester, UK: Wiley
Gigerenzer, G. (1997). The modularity of social intelligence. In A. Whiten and R. W. Byrne (eds.). Machiavellian Intelligence II (264–88). Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Gigerenzer, G. and Hug, K. (1992). Domain-specific reasoning: Social contracts, cheating, and perspective change. Cognition. 43, 127–71CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ginsburg, H. J. and Miller, S. M. (1982). Sex differences and children's risk taking behavior. Child Development. 53, 426–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glass, S. P. and Wright, T. L. (1992). Justifications for extramarital relationships: The association between attitudes, behaviours and gender. Journal of Sex Research. 29, 361–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldsmith, T. H. and Zimmerman, W. F. (2001). Biology, Evolution and Human Nature. New York: Wiley
Goodall, J. (1964). Tool-using and aimed throwing in a community of free-living chimpanzees. Nature. 201, 1264–6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe. Cambridge, MA: Belknap
Gopnik, A. and Astington, J. W. (1988). Children's understanding of representational change, and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance–reality distinction. Child Development. 59, 26–37CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N. and Kuhl, P. K. (1999). How Babies Think. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson
Gopnik, M. (1994). Impairments of tense in a familial language disorder. Journal of Neurolinguistics. 8, 109–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, M. (1997). Language deficits and genetic factors. Trends in Cognitive Science. 1(1), 5–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gopnik, M. and Crago, M. (1991). Familial aggregation of a developmental language disorder. Cognition. 39, 1–50CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, P. (1986). Level ordering in lexical development. Cognition. 21, 73–93CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottesman, I. I., McGuffin, P. and Farmer, A. E. (1987). Clinical genetics as clues to the ‘real’ genetics of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 13, 23–47CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gould, J. L. and Gould, G. C. (1989; 1997). Sexual Selection: Mate Choice and Courtship in Nature. New York: W. H. Freeman and Co
Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and Phylogeny. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Gould, S. J. (1981). The Mismeasure of Man. New York: Norton
Gould, S. J. and Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The spandrels of San Marco and the panglossian program: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. 205, 281–8Google Scholar
Grafen, A. (1990). Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 144, 517–46CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graves, H. B., Hable, C. P. and Jenkins, T. H. (1985). Sexual selection in Gallus. Effects of morphology and dominance on female spatial behaviour. Behavioural Processes. 11, 189–97CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, J. H. (1987). Language in the Americas. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press
Griggs, R. A. and Cox, J. R. (1982). The elusive thematics material effect in Wason's selection task. British Journal of Psychology. 73, 407–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., Koller, S. H. and Dias, M. G. (1993). Affect, culture, and morality, or, is it wrong to eat your dog?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 65(4), 613–28CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haig, D. (1993). Genetic conflicts in human pregnancy. Quarterly Review of Biology. 68, 495–523CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haig, D. (1998). Genetic conflicts of pregnancy and childhood. In S. C. Stearns (ed.). Evolution in Health and Disease (77–90). Oxford: Oxford University Press
Haines, S. and Gould, J. (1994). Female platys prefer long tails. Nature. 370, 512CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, T. R. (1994). Sex and Evolution. In P. J. B. Slater and T. R. Halliday. Behaviour and Evolution (150–92). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Hamer, D. and Copeland, P. (1998). Living with Our Genes: Why They Matter More Than You Think. New York: Doubleday
Hamilton, W. D. (1964a and b). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I and II. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 7, 1–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. and Zuk, M. (1982). Heritable true fitness and bright birds: A role for parasites?Science. 218, 384–7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haney, C., Banks, W. C. and Zimbardo, P. G. (1973). A study of prisoners and guards in a simulated prison. Naval Research Review. 30, 4–17Google Scholar
Harlow, J. M. (1848). Passage of an iron rod through the head. Boston Medical and Surgical Journal. 39, 389–93Google Scholar
Harre, R. (1986). The Social Construction of Emotions. Oxford: Basil Blackwell
Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the child's environment? A group-socialization theory of development. Psychological Review. 102(3), 458–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, J. R. (1998). The Nurture Assumption: Why Children Turn Out the Way They Do. New York: Simon and Schuster
Harris, M. (1979). Cultural Materialism: The Struggle for a Science of Culture. New York: Random House
Harris, M. (1985). Good to Eat: Riddles of Food and Culture. New York: Simon and Schuster
Hartle, D. L. and Orel, V. (1992). What did Mendel think he discovered?Genetics. 131, 245–53Google Scholar
Hauser, M. D. and Carey, S. (1998). Building a cognitive creature from a set of primitives: Evolutionary and developmental insights. In D. Cummins and C. Allen (eds.). The Evolution of Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Haviland, J. M. and Malatesta, C. (1981). A description of the development of sex differences in nonverbal signals: Fantasies, fallacies, and facts. In C. Mayo and N. Henley (eds.). Gender and Nonverbal Behaviors. New York: Springer-VerlagCrossRef
Hayes, N. (1995). Access to Psychology. London: Hodder and Stoughton
Hazan, C. and Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 52(3), 511–24CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hewstone, M., Wolfgang, S. and Stephenson, G. M. (eds.) (1996). Introduction to Social Psychology. Oxford: Blackwell
Heyes, C. M. and Dawson, G. R. (1990). A demonstration of observational learning using a bidirectional control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 42, 59–71Google ScholarPubMed
Hill, K. and Kaplan, H. (1988). Trade-offs in male and female reproductive strategies among the Ache. In L. Batzig, M. Borgerhof Mulder and P. Turke (eds.). Human Reproductive Behaviour (277–305). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Hinde, R. A. (1977). Mother-infant separation and the nature of inter-individual relationships: Experiments with rhesus monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B. 196, 29–50CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirschfeld, L. A. and Gelman, S. A. (eds.) (1994). Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture. New York: Cambridge University Press
Hirsh-Pasek, K. and Golinkoff, R. M. (1991). Language comprehension: A new look at some old themes. In N. Krasnegor, D. Rumbaugh, M. Studdert-Kennedy and R. Schiefelbusch (eds.). Biological and Behavioral Determinants of Language Development (301–20). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Hite, S. (1987). Women and Love: A Cultural Revolution in Progress. New York: Knopf
Hoffman, M. L. (1977). Empathy, its development and prosocial implications. In C. B. Keasey (ed.). Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Vol. 25. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press
Hoffman, M. L. (1978). Psychological and biological perspectives on altruism. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1, 323–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, M. L. (1982). Development of prosocial motivation: Empathy and guilt. In N. Eisenberg (ed.). The Development of Prosocial Behavior. New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Hunt, M. (1974). Sexual Behaviour in the 70s. Chicago: Playboy Press
Izard, C. E. (1977). Human Emotions. New York: Plenum
Izard, C. E. (1992). Basic emotions, relations among emotions and emotion–cognition relations. Psychological Review. 99, 561–5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izard, C. E. (1994). Innate and universal facial expressions: Evidence from developmental and cross-cultural research. Psychological Bulletin. 115, 288–99CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jablensky, H., Sartorius, N., Ernberg, G., Anker, M., Korten, A., Cooper, J. E., Day, R. and Bertelsen, A. (1992). Schizophrenia: Manifestations, Incidence and Course in Different Cultures: A WHO, Ten Country Study. Psychological Medicine. Monograph Supplement. 20. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Jackson, D. J. and Huston, T. L. (1975). Physical attractiveness and assertiveness. Journal of Social Psychology. 96, 79–84CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, O. (2002). They F*** You Up: How to Survive Family Life. London: Bloomsbury
James, W. (1884). What is an emotion?Mind. 9, 188–205CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1890). Principles of Psychology. New York: Holt
James, W. H. (1981). The honeymoon effect on marital coitus. Journal of Sex Research. 17, 114–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jamison, K. R. (1993). Touched with Fire: Manic-Depressive Illness and the Artistic Temperament. New York: The Free Press
Jamison, K. R. (1995). Manic-depressive illness and creativity. Scientific American. 272, 62–7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jankowiak, W. and Fischer, C. R. (1992). A cross-cultural perspective on romantic love. Ethology. 31, 149–55Google Scholar
Jenkins, P. F. (1978). Cultural transmission of song patterns and dialect development in a free-living bird population. Animal Behaviour. 25, 50–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johanson, D. C., White, T. D. and Coppens, V. (1978). A new species of the genus Australopithecus (Primate: Hominidae) from the Pliocene of eastern Africa. Kirtlandia. 28, 1–14Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H. and Bolhuis, J. J. (1991). Imprinting, predispositions and filial preference in the chick. In R. J. Andrew (ed.). Neural and Behavioural Plasticity. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRef
Johnson, M. H. and Morton, J. (1991). Biology and Cognitive Development: The Case of Face Recognition. Oxford: Blackwell
Johnson-Laird, P. N. and Oatley, K. (1992). Basic emotions, rationality and folk theory. Cognition and Emotion. 6, 201–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. and Wason, P. C. (1970). Insight into a logical relation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 22, 49–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, V. S. and Franklin, M. (1993). Is beauty in the eye of the beholder?Ethology and Sociobiology. 14, 183–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jolliffe, T. and Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Are people with autism or Asperger's syndrome faster than normal on the Embedded Figures Task?Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 38, 527–34CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J. (1998). A parent's influence is peerless. Boston Globe, 13 September, 3Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1798/1996). Anthropology from a Pragmatic Point of View. Southern Illinois University Press
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1997). Crucial differences between developmental cognitive neuroscience and adult neuropsychology. Developmental Neuropsychology. 13(4), 513–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2000). Why babies' brains are not Swiss-army knives. In S. Rose and H. Rose (eds.). Alas Poor Darwin: Arguments Against Evolutionary Psychology. London: Jonathan Cape
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Klima, E., Bellugi, U., Grant, J. and Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Is there a social module? Language, face processing and theory of mind in subjects with Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 7(2), 196–208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Tyler, L. K., Voice, K., Sims, K., Udwin, O., Davies, M. and Howlin, P. (1998). Linguistic dissociations in Williams syndrome: Evaluating receptive syntax in on-line and off-line tasks. Neuropsychologia. 36(4), 342–51CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kawai, M. (1965). Newly-acquired pre-cultural behavior of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima Islet. Primates. 6, 1–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keeley, L. H. (1996). War Before Civilization. New York: Oxford University Press
Kellogg, W. N. and Kellogg, L. A. (1933). The Ape and the Child. New York: McGraw-Hill
Kenrick, D. T. and Keefe, R. C. (1992). Age preferences in mates reflects sex differences in reproductive strategies. Behavioural and Brain Sciences. 15, 75–133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenrick, D. T. and Simpson, J. A. (1997). Why social psychology and evolutionary psychology need one another. In J. A. Simpson and D. T. Kenrick (eds.). Evolutionary Social Psychology (1–20). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
King-Hele, D. (1968). The Essential Writings of Erasmus Darwin. London: MacGibbon and Kee
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. E. and Martin, C. E. (1953). Sexual Behaviour in the Human Female. Philadelphia: Saunders
Kirkpatrick, L. A. and Hazan, C. (1994). Attachment styles and close relationships: A four-year prospective study. Personal Relationships. 1, 123–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, S. B., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J. and Chance, S. (2002). Decisions and the evolution of memory: Multiple systems, multiple functions. Psychological Review. 109, 306–29CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, S. B., Loftus, J. and Kihlstrom, J. F. (1996). Self-knowledge of an amnesic patient: Toward a neuropsychology of personality and social psychology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 13, 501–18Google Scholar
Kleinginna, P. R. and Kleinginna, A. M. (1981a). A categorized list of emotion definitions with suggestions for a consensual definition. Motivation and Emotion. 5, 263–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleinginna, P. R. and Kleinginna, A. M. (1981b). A categorized list of emotion definitions with suggestions for a consensual definition. Motivation and Emotion. 5, 345–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koss, M. (in press). Evolutionary models of why men rape: Acknowledging the complexities. Trauma, Violence and Abuse: A Review Journal
Krebs, D. L. and Denton, K. (1997). Social illusions and self-deception: The evolution of biases in person perception. In J. A. Simpson and D. T. Kenrick (eds.). Evolutionary Social Psychology (21–48). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
Krebs, J. R. and Davies, N. B. (1978). Behavioural Ecology. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Assoc
Krebs, J. R. and Davies, N. B. (1981). An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications
Kuhl, P., Williams, K. A., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K. N. and Lindblom, B. (1992). Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by six months of age. Science. 255, 606–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunce, L. J. and Shaver, P. R. (1994). An attachment-theoretical approach to caregiving in romantic relationships. In K. Bartholomew and D. Perlman (eds.). Advances in Personal Relationships. Vol. 5 (205–37). London: Kingsley
Lahr, M. M. and Foley, R. (1994). Multiple dispersals and modern human origins. Evolutionary Anthropology. 3, 48–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landy, D. and Aronson, E. (1969). The influence of the character of the criminal and his victim on the decisions of simulated jurors. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 5, 141–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lange, C. G. (1885/1922). The emotions: A psychophysiological study, trans. I. A. Haupt. In C. G. and W. James (eds.). Psychology Classica. Vol. 1. Baltimore, MD: Wilkins
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and Adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press
Leakey, R. and Lewin, R. (1992). Origins Reconsidered: In Search of What Makes us Human. London: Little, Brown
Leakey, R. and Lewin, R. (1996). The Sixth Extinction: Biodiversity and its Survival. London: Poenix
LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life. New York: Simon and Schuster
Lee, R. B. (1972). The!Kung Bushmen of Botswana. In M. C. Bicchieri (ed.). Hunters and Gatherers Today. New York: Holt Rinehart Winston
Lee, R. B. (1979). The!Kung San. Men, Women and Work in Foraging Society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Lee, Y. T., Jussim, L. J. and McCauley, C. R. (eds.) (1995). Stereotyped Accuracy: Toward Appreciating Group Differences. Washington DC: American Psychological Association Press
Lehman, H. C. (1953). Age and Achievement. Princeton: Princeton University Press
Leonard, L. (1992). Specific language impairments in three languages: Some cross-linguistic evidence. In P. Fletcher and D. Hall (eds.). Specific Speech and Language Disorder in Children (119–26). London: Whurr
Leonard, L. (1998). Children with Specific Language Impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Leslie, A. M. (1994). ToMM, ToBy, and agency: Core architecture and domain specificity in cognition and culture. In L. A. Hirschfeld and S. A. Gelman (eds.). Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture (119–48). New York: Cambridge University Press
Levy, J., Heller, W., Branch, M. and Burton, L. (1983). Asymmetry of perception in free viewing of chimeric faces. Brain and Cognition. 2, 404–19CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewin, R. (1998). Principles of Human Evolution. London: Blackwell Science
Lewontin, R. C. (2001). It Ain't Necessarily So: The Dream of the Human Genome and other Confusions. New York: Granta
Lewontin, R. C., Rose, S. and Kamin, L. J. (1984). Not in Our Genes: Biology, Ideology, and Human Nature. New York: Pantheon
Ligon, J. P. and Ligon, S. H. (1978). Communal breeding in green woodhoopoes as a case for reciprocity. Nature. 276, 496–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lively, C. (1987). Evidence from a New Zealand snail for the maintenance of sex by parasitism. Nature. 328, 519–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lively, C., Craddock, C. and Vrijenhoek, R. C. (1990). Red Queen hypothesis supported by parasitism in sexual and clonal fish. Nature. 344, 864–6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loehlin, J. C., McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T. Jr and John, O. P. (1998). Heritabilities of common and measure-specific components of the Big Five personality factors. Journal of Research in Personality. 32, 431–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Low, B. S. and Heinen, J. T. (1993). Population, resources and environment: Implications of human behavioural ecology for conservation. Population and Environment. 15, 7–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lumsden, C. J. and Wilson, E. O. (1981). Genes, Mind, and Culture: The Coevolutionary Process. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Luria, A. (1968). The Mind of a Mnemonist. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Lutchmaya, S., Baron-Cohen, S. and Raggett, P. (2002). Foetal testosterone and eye contact at 12 months. Infant Behavior and Development. 25, 327–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyons, M. J., True, W. R., Eisen, S. A., Goldberg, J., Meyer, J. M., Farone, S. V., Eaves, L. J. and Tsuang, M. T. (1995). Differential heritability of adult and juvenile antisocial traits. Archives of General Psychiatry. 52, 906–15CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCloskey, M. (1983). Naive theories of motion. In D. Gentner and A. L. Stevens (eds.). Mental Models. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Maccoby, E. E. (1990). Gender and relationships: a developmental account. American Psychologist. 45, 513–20CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrae, R. R. and Costa, P. T. (1996). Toward a new generation of personality theories: Theoretical contexts for the five-factor model. In J. S. Wiggins (ed.). The Five-Factor Model of Personality: Theoretical Perspectives (51–87). New York: Guilford
MacDonald, K. B. (1998). Evolution and development. In A. Campbell and S. Muncer (eds.). Social Development (21–49). London: UCL Press
McFarland, D. (1999). Animal Behaviour: Psychobiology, Ethology and Evolution (3rd edn). Harlow: Addison Wesley Longman
McGaugh, J. L. (1992). Neuromodulatory regulation of memory – role of the amygdaloid complex. International Journal of Psychology. 27, 403Google Scholar
McGuire, M. and Troisi, A. (1998). Darwinian Psychiatry. Oxford: Oxford University Press
McGuire, M., Marks, I., Nesse, R. M. and Troisi, A. (1992). Evolutionary biology: A basic science for psychiatry?Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 86, 89–96CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGuire, M., Troisi, A. and Raleigh, M. M. (1997). Depression in an evolutionary context. In S. Baron-Cohen (ed.). The Maladapted Mind (255–82). Hove: Psychology Press
MacLarnon, A. and Hewitt, G. P. (1999). The evolution of human speech: the role of enhanced breathing control. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 10(3), 341–33.0.CO;2-2>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madsen, D. (1985). A biochemical property relating to power seeking in humans. American Political Science Review. 79, 448–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madsen, D. and McGuire, M. (1984). Rapid communication whole blood serotonin and the type A behavior pattern. Psychosomatic Medicine. 46, 546–8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malinowski, B. (1929). The sexual life of savages in North-Western Melanesia. London: Routledge
Manktelow, K. I. and Evans, J. St. B. T. (1979). Facilitation of reasoning by realism: Effect or non-effect?British Journal of Psychology. 70, 477–488CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manktelow, K. I. and Over, D. E. (1990). Deontic thought and the selection task. In K. I. Gilhooly, M. Keane, R. H. Logie and G. Erdos (eds.). Lines of Thinking: Reflections on the Psychology of Thought. Vol. 1 (153–64). Chichester: Wiley
Manning, A. and Stamp-Dawkins, M. (1998). An Introduction to Animal Behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Markman, E. (1989). Categorization and Naming in Children: Problems of Induction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Marks, I. M. and Nesse, R. M. (1994). Fear and fitness: An evolutionary analysis of anxiety disorders. Ethology and Sociobiology. 15, 247–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marr, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco: Freeman
Martin, P. (1997) The Sickening Mind: Brain, Behaviour, Immunity and Disease. London: HarperCollins
Martin, R. D. (1983). Human Brain Evolution in an Ecological Context. New York: American Museum of Natural History
Maynard-Smith, J. (1964). Group selection and kin selection. Nature. 201, 1145–7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maynard-Smith, J. (1971). What is sex?Journal of Theoretical Biology. 30, 319–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maynard-Smith, J. (1974). The theory of games and the evolution of animal conflicts. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 47, 209–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maynard-Smith, J. (1978). The Evolution of Sex. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Maynard-Smith, J. (1993). The Theory of Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Mead, M. (1928). Coming of Age in Samoa. New York: William Morrow
Mead, M. (1935). Sex and Temperament in Three Primitive Societies. New York: William Morrow
Mead, M. (1949). Male and Female. New York: William Morrow
Mead, M. (1961). Anthropology among the sciences. American Anthropologist. 63, 475–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mealey, L. (1995). The sociobiology of sociopathy: An integrated evolutionary model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 18, 523–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. and Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science. 198, 75–8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N. and Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development. 54, 702–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Midgley, M. (1979). Beast and Man. Ithaca: Cornell University Press
Miller, G. F. (2000). The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped the Evolution of Human Nature. London: Heinemann/Doubleday
Mineka, S. and Cook, M. (1988). Social learning and the acquisition of snake fear in monkeys. In T. Zentall and B. G. Galef (eds.). Social Learning: Psychological and Biological Perspectives (51–73). Hillsdale, NJ: Laerence Erlbaum
Minton, C., Kagan, J. and Levine, J. (1971). Maternal control and obedience in the two-year-old. Child Development. 42, 1873–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, P. (1996). Acquiring a Concept of Mind: A Review of Psychological Research and Theory. Hove: Psychology Press
Moffitt, T., Caspi, A., Belsky, J. and Silva, P. (1992). Childhood experience and the onset of menarche: a test of a sociobiological model. Child Development. 63, 47–58CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moller, A. P. (1988). Female choice selects for male tail ornaments in the monogamous swallow. Nature. 332, 640–2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moller, A. P. (1990). Effects of a haematophagous mite on secondary sexual tail ornaments in the barn swallow: a test of the Hamilton and Zuk hypothesis. Evolution. 44, 771–84Google Scholar
Morris, J. S., Friston, K. J., Beuchel, C., Frith, C. D., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J. and Dolan, R. J. (1998). A neuromodularity role for the human amygdale in processing emotional facial expressions. Brain. 121, 47–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morton, J. and Johnson, M. H. (1991). CONSPEC and CONLERN: A two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review. 98, 164–81CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muller, H. J. (1964). The relation of recombination to mutational advance. Mutation Research. 1, 2–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagell, K., Olguin, K. and Tomasello, M. (1993). Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 107, 174–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagy, W. E. and Anderson, R. C. (1984). How many words are there in printed English?Reading Research Quarterly. 19, 304–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nesse, R. M. (1990). Evolutionary explanations of emotions. Human Nature. 1, 261–89CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nesse, R. M. and Williams, G. C. (1995). Evolution and Healing: The New Science of Darwinian Medicine. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson
Nolan-Hocksema, S. (2001). Abnormal Psychology. Boston: McGraw-Hill
Norberg, R. Å. (1994). Swallow tail streamer is a mechanical device for self-deflection of tail leading edge, enhancing aerodynamic efficiency and flight manoeuvrability. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B. 257, 227–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oaksford, M. and Chater, N. (1994). A rational analysis of the selection task as optimal data selection. Psychological Review. 101, 608–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oaksford, M. and Chater, N. (1999). Ten years of the rational analysis of cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3(2), 57–65Google Scholar
Ortony, A. and Turner, T. J. (1990). What's basic about emotions?Psychological Review. 97, 315–31CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Over, R. (1988). Does scholarly impact decline with age?Scientometrics. 13, 215–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Packer, C. (1977). Reciprocal altruism in Papio anubis. Nature. 265, 441–3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J., Leekam, S. and Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds' difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 5, 125–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrie, M., Halliday, T. and Sanders, C. (1991). Peahens prefer peacocks with elaborate trains. Animal Behaviour. 41, 323–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piattelli Palmarini, M. (1994a). Ever since language and learning: afterthoughts on the Piaget–Chomsky debate. Cognition. 50, 315–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piattelli Palmarini, M. (1994b). Inevitable Illusions: How the Mistakes of Reason Rule Our Minds. Somerset, NJ: John Wiley and Sons
Pilleri, G. and Knuckey, J. (1969). Behaviour patterns of some Delphinidae observed in the western Mediterranean. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie. 26, 48–72Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language. London: Penguin
Pinker, S. (1997). How the Mind Works. London: Allen Lane
Pinker, S. (2002). The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature. London: Allen Lane
Pinker, S. and Bloom, P. (1990). Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 13(4), 707–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pirolli, P. and Card, S. K. (1999). Information foraging. Psychological Review. 105(1), 58–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R. and Daniels, D. (1987). Why are children in the same family so different from one another?Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 10, 1–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R., Chipuer, H. M. and Neiderhiser, J. M. (1994). Behavior genetic evidence for the importance of nonshared environment. In Hetherington, E. M., Reiss, D. and Plomin, R. (eds.). Separate Social Worlds of Siblings: The Impact of Nonshared Environment on Development. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McClearn, G. E. and Rutter, M. (1997). Behavioral Genetics (3rd edn). New York: W. H. Freeman
Pollard, P. and Evans, J. St. B. T. (1987). Content and context effects in reasoning. American Journal of Psychology. 100, 41–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Popper K. (1959). The Logic of Scientific Discovery. London: Hutchinson
Posner, R. A. (1992). Sex and Reason. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Potts, R. (1996). Humanity's Descent: The Consequences of an Ecological Instability. New York: William Morrow
Pratto, F. and John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 61, 380–91CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Premack, A. J. and Premack, D. (1972). Teaching language to an ape. Scientific American, October. 227, 4, 92–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Price, J. S. (1967). Hypothesis: The dominance hierarchy and the evolution of mental illness. Lancet. 2, 243–6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Price, J. S., Sloman, L., Gardner, R., Gilbert, P. and Rohde, P. (1994). The social competition hypothesis of depression. British Journal of Psychiatry. 164, 309–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Profit, M. (1992). Pregnancy sickness as an adaptation: A deterrent to maternal ingestion of teratogens. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby (eds.). The Adapted Mind (327–65). New York: Oxford University Press
Quine, W. V. O. (1960). Word and Object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Rabbie, J. M. (1992) The effects of intragroup cooperation and intergroup competition on in-group cohesion and out-group hostility. In Harcourt, A. H. and de Waal, F. B. M. (eds.). Coalitions and Alliances in Humans and Other Animals (175–205). New York: Oxford University Press
Radke-Yarrow, M. and Zahn-Waxler, C. (1986) The role of familial factors in the development of prosocial behavior: Research findings and questions. In D. Olweus, J. Block and M. Radke-Yarrow (eds.). Development of Antisocial and Prosocial Behavior: Research, Theories, and Issues (207–33). New York: Academic Press
Raleigh, M. and McGuire, M. (1991) Serotonin in vervet monkeys. Brain Research. 559, 181–90CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapoport, A. and Chummah, A. M. (1965). Prisoner's Dilemma. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press
Reader, W. R. and Payne, S. J. (2002a). Adaptive browsing: Sampling and satisficing in text choice. The Thirteenth Annual Winter Conference On Discourse, Text, & Cognition. January 25–8, Jackson Hole, Wyoming
Reader, W. R. and Payne, S. J. (2002b). Browsing multiple texts under time pressure. 24th Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, George Mason University. Washington, DC
Real, L. A. (1991). Animal choice behavior and the evolution of cognitive architecture. Science. 253, 980–6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reilly, P. (1991). The Surgical Solution: A History of Involuntary Sterilization in the United States. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press
Renfrew, C. (1987). Archaeology and Language. New York: Penguin
Resnick, P. J. (1970). Murder of the newborn: a psychiatric review of neonaticide, American Journal of Psychiatry. 126, 58–64CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reuter-Lorenz, P. and Davidson, R. (1981). Differential contributions of the two cerebral hemispheres to the perception of happy and sad faces. Neuropsychologia. 19, 609–13CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richell, R. A., Mitchell, D. G. V., Newam, C., Leonard, A., Baron-Cohen, S., and Blair, R. J. R. (2002). Theory of mind and psychopathy: can psychopathic individuals read ‘the language of the eyes’?Neuropsychologia. 41, 523–6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richerson, P. J. and Boyd, R. (2001). Culture is part of human biology: why the superorganic concept serves the human sciences badly. In S. Maasen and M. Winterhager (eds.). Science Studies: Probing the Dynamics of Scientific Knowledge (147–77). New York: Transcript Verlag
Ridley, M. (1993). The Red Queen: Sex and the Evolution of Human Nature. London: Penguin
Ridley, M. (1997). The Origins of Virtue. London: Viking Press
Ridley, M. (1999). Genome: The Autobiography of a Species in 23 Chapters. New York: HarperCollins
Rode, C., Cosmides, L., Hell, W. and Tooby, J. (1999). When and why do people avoid unknown probabilities in decisions under uncertainty? Testing some predictions from optimal foraging theory. Cognition. 72, 269–304CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roediger, H. L. and McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 21, 803–14Google Scholar
Rood, J. P. (1986). Ecology and social evolution in the mongooses. In D. I. Rubenstein and R. W. Wrangham (eds.). Ecological Aspects of Social Evolution. Birds and Mammals (131–52). Princeton: Princeton University Press
Rosch, E. (1973). On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. In T. M. Moore (ed.). Cognitive Development and the Acquisition of Language. New York: Academic Press
Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M. and Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology. 8, 382–439CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, H. and Rose, S. (2000). Alas Poor Darwin: Arguments against Evolutionary Psychology. London: Jonathan Cape
Rose, H. and Rose, S. (2001). Much ado about very little. The Psychologist. 14, 428–9Google Scholar
Rose, S., Lewontin, R. C. and Kamin, L. J. (1984). Not in Our Genes. London: Pelican
Rothbart, M. K. (1989). Temperament and development. In G. Kohnstamm, J. Bates and M. K. Rothbart (eds.). Temperament in Childhood (187–248). Chichester: Wiley
Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S. and Haidt, J. (1999) The moral–emotion triad hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral ethics (community, autonomy, divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 76, 574–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruffman, T., Perner, J., Naito, M., Parkin, L. and Clements, W. (1998). Older (but not younger) siblings facilitate false belief understanding. Developmental Psychology. 34, 161–74CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruse, M. (1987). Sociobiology and knowledge: Is evolutionary epistemology a viable option? In C. Crawford, M. Smith and D. Krebs (eds.). Sociobiology and Psychology: Ideas, Issues and Applications (61–79). London: Lawrence Erlbaum
Rutherford, M. J., Cacciola, J. S. and Alterman, A. I. (1999). Antisocial personality disorder and psychopathy in cocaine-dependent women. American Journal of Psychiatry. 156, 849–56CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sahlins, M. (1976). The Use and Abuse of Biology: An Anthropological Critique of Socio-Biology. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press
Samuels R., Stich, S. and Bishop, M. (2002). Ending the rationality wars: How to make disputes about human rationality disappear. In R. Elio (ed.). Common Sense, Reasoning and Rationality. New York: Oxford University PressCrossRef
Sandstrom, P. E. (1994). An optimal foraging approach to information seeking and use. Library Quarterly. 64, 414–49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Murphy, J., Sevick, R. A., Brakke, K. E., Williams, S. L. and Rumbaugh, D. (1993). Language comprehension in ape and child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 233. Chicago: University of Chicago PressCrossRef
Schacter, D. (2001). The Seven Sins of Memory: How the Mind Forgets and Remembers. New York: Houghton Mifflin
Schaller, G. B. (1972). The Serengeti Lion. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Seligman, M. E. P. (1970). On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychology Review. 77, 406–18CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M. and Cheney, D. L. (1984). Grooming, alliances and reciprocal altruism in vervet monkeys. Nature. 308, 541–3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shankman, P. (1998). Margaret Mead, Derek Freeman, and the issue of evolution. Skeptical Inquirer. 22, 35–9Google Scholar
Shavit, V., Fischer, C. S. and Koresh, Y. (1994). Kin and non-kin under collective threat: Israeli networks during the Gulf war. Social Forces. 72, 1197–215CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R. (1990). Mind Sights. New York: W. H. Freeman
Sherif, M. (1956). In Common Predicament: Social Psychology of Intergroup Conflict and Cooperation. New York: Houghton Mifflin
Sherman, P. W., Jarvis, J. U. M. and Alexander, R. D. (1991). The Biology of the Naked Mole-Rat. Princeton: Princeton University Press
Short, R. V. (1979). Sexual selection and its component parts, somatic and genital selection, as illustrated by man and the Great Apes. Advances in the Study of Behaviour. 9, 131–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shweder, R. A., Much, N. C., Mahapatra, M. and Park, L. (1994). The ‘big three’ of morality (autonomy, community and divinity) and the ‘big three’ explanations of suffering. In A. Brand and P. Rozin (eds.). Morality and Health. New York: Routledge
Silk, J. B. (1980). Adoption and Kinship in Oceania. American Anthropologist. 82, 799–820CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silk, J. B. (1990). Human adoption in evolutionary perspective. Human Nature. 1, 25–52CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simner, M. L. (1971). Newborn's response to the crying of another infant. Developmental Psychology. 5, 136–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simpson, J. A., Gangestad, S. W. and Biek, M. (1993). Personality and nonverbal behaviour: An ethological perspective of relationship initiation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 29, 434–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simpson, J. A. and Kenrick, D. T. (1997). Evolutionary Social Psychology. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Singer, P. (1981). The Expanding Circle: Ethics and Sociobiology. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux
Singh, D. (1993) Adaptive significance of female physical attractiveness: Role of waist-to-hip ratio. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 65, 293–307CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, D. and Luis, S. (1995). Ethic and gender consensus for the effect of waist-to-hip ratio on judgement of women's attractiveness. Human Nature. 6, 51–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal Behaviour. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts
Slater, J. S. B. (1994). Kinship and altruism. In J. S. B. Slater and T. R. Halliday (eds.). Behaviour and Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Slater, J. S. B. and Halliday, T. R. (1994). Behaviour and Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Slotta, J. D., Chi, M. T. H. and Joram, E. (1995). Assessing students' misclassification of physics concepts: An ontological basis for conceptual change. Cognition and Instruction. 13, 373–400CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, M. S. (1987). Evolution and developmental psychology: Towards a sociobiology of human development. In C. Crawford, M. Smith and D. Krebs (eds.). Sociobiology and Psychology: Ideas, Issues, and Applications (225–52). New Jersey: Hillsdale
Smith, R. L. (1984). Human sperm competition. In R. L. Smith (ed.). Sperm Competition and the Evolution of Mating Systems (601–59). New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Sociobiology Study Group (1975). New York Review of Books, 13 November
Sokal, A. (1996). Transgressing the boundaries: Towards a transformative hermeneutics of quantum gravity. The Social Text. 46/47, 217–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soldz, S. and Vaillant, G. E. (1999). The big five personality traits and the life course: A 45-year longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Personality. 33, 208–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spelke, E. S., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J. and Jacobson, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review. 99(4), 605–32CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spencer, H. (1864). Principles of Biology. London: Williams and Norgate
Sperber, D. (1994). The modularity of thought and the epidemiology of representations. In L. A. Hirschfeld and S. A. Gelman (eds.). Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture. New York: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach. Oxford: Blackwell
Springer, S. P. and Deutsch, G. (1998). Left Brain, Right Brain: Perspectives from Cognitive Neurosciences. New York: Freeman and Co
Stanford, C. B. (1995). Chimpanzee hunting behaviour. American Scientist. 83, 256–61Google Scholar
Stephens, D. W. and Krebs, J. R. (1986). Foraging Theory. Princeton: Princeton University Press
Stevens, A. and Price, J. (1996). Evolutionary Psychiatry. London: Routledge
Straus, M. A. (1971). Some social antecedents of physical punishment: A linkage theory interpretation. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 33, 658–63CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Straus, M. A., Gelles, R. J. and Steinmetz, S. K. (1980). Behind Closed Doors: Violence in American Families. New York: Doubleday
Strauss, E., Gaddes, W. H. and Wada, J. (1987). Performance on a free-recall verbal dichotic listening task and cerebral dominance determined by carotid amytal test. Neuropsychologia. 25, 747–53CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strum, S. C. (1981). Processes and products of change: Baboon predatory behaviour at Gilgil, Kenya. In R. Harding and G. Teleki (eds.). Omnivorous Primates (255–302). New York: Columbia University Press
Strum, S. C. (1987). Almost Human. New York: W. W. Norton
Strum, S. C. and Mitchell, W. (1987). Baboon models and muddles. In W. G. Kinzey (ed.). The Evolution of Human Behavior: Primate Models (87–104). Albany: State University of New York Press
Sussman, R. L. (1987). Pygmy chimpanzees and common chimpanzees: Models for the behavioural ecology of the earliest hominids. In W. G. Kinzey (ed.). The Evolution of Human Behavior: Primate Models (87–104). Albany: State University of New York Press
Symons, D. (1979). The Evolution of Human Sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press
Symons, D. (1989). The psychology of human mate preferences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 12, 34–5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. and Sullivan, K. (2000). A componential view of theory of mind: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognition. 76(1), 59–89CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H., Boshart, J. and Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Reading the windows to the soul: Evidence of domain-specific sparing in Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 10(5), 631–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tajfel, H. L. (1970). Experiments in intergroup discrimination. Scientific American. 223, 96–102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H. L., Billig, M., Bundy, R. and Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology. 1, 149–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tallal, P., Stark, R. E. and Mellitts, E. D. (1985). Identification of language-impaired children on the basis of rapid perception and production skills. Brain and Language. 25, 314–22CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanner, N. M. (1981). On Becoming Human. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Tanner, N. M. and Zihlman, A. (1976). Women in evolution part 1: Innovation and selection in human origins. Signs: Women, Culture and Society. 1, 585–608CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tattersall, I. and Matternes, J. H. (2000). Once we were not alone. Scientific American. 282, 56–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, C. P. A. and Glenn, N. D. (1976). The utility of education and attractiveness for females' status attainment through marriage. American Sociological Review. 41, 484–98CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in animals. Psychological Review, Monograph Supplements. No. 8. New York: MacmillanCrossRef
Thornhill, R. and Palmer, C. T. (2000). A Natural History of Rape: Biological Bases of Sexual Coercion. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Tinbergen, N. (1951). The Study of Instinct. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Toates, F. (2001). Biological Psychology: An Integrative Approach. Harlow: Pearson Education
Tomasello, M. (1999). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C. and Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 16, 495–552CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomlin, R. S. (1986). Basic Word Order: Functional Principles. London: Croom Helm
Tooby, J. and Cosmides, L. (1990). On the universality of human nature and the uniqueness of the individual: The role of genetics and adaptation. Journal of Personality. 58, 17–67CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tooby, J. and Cosmides, L. (1997). Evolutionary psychology: A primer. www.psych.ucsb.edu/research/cep/primer.html (accessed 11 February 2004)
Tooby, J. and DeVore, I. (1987). The reconstruction of hominid behavioural evolution through strategic modelling. In W. G. Kinzey (ed.). The Evolution of Human Behaviour: Primate Models (183–237). Albany: State University of New York Press
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology. 46, 35–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (ed.). Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man (139–79). Chicago: Aldine
Trivers, R. L. (1974). Parent–offspring conflict. American Zoologist. 14, 249–264CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1976). Foreword in R. Dawkins' The Selfish Gene. London: Oxford University Press
Trivers, R. L. (1985). Social Evolution. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings
Tudge, C. (1995). The Day before Yesterday: Five Million Years of Human History. London: Jonathan Cape
Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving and W. Donaldson (eds.). Organization of Memory. New York: Academic Press
Turkheimer, E. (2000). Three laws of behavior genetics and what they mean. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 9, 160–4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, A. K. (1994). Genetic and hormonal influences on male violence. In J. Archer (ed.). Male Violence (233–52). New York and London: Routledge
Turner, P. E. and Chao, L. (1999). Prisoner's dilemma in an RNA virus. Nature. 398, 441–3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology. 5, 207–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1982). Judgments of and by representativeness. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic and A. Tversky (eds.). Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Ustun, T. B. and Chatterji, S. (2001). Global burden of depressive disorders and future projections. In A. Dawson and A. Tylee (eds.). Depression: Social and Economic Time Bomb (31–43). London: BMJ Books
Lely, H. and Ullman, M. (2001). Past tense morphology in specifically language impaired and normally developing children. Language and Cognitive Processes. 16, 177–217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1967). Facial displays of catarrhine monkeys and apes. In D. Morris (ed.). Primate Ethology (7–68). London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson
Van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1972). A comparative approach to the phylogeny of laughter and smile. In R. A. Hinde (ed.). Non-Verbal Communication (209–41). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Valen, L. (1973). A New Evolutionary Law. Evolutionary Theory. 1, 1–30Google Scholar
Vandel, D. L. (2000). Parents, peer groups, and other socializing influences. Developmental Psychology. 36(6), 600–710Google Scholar
Frisch, K. (1967). Honeybees: Do they use direction and distance information provided by their dances?Science. 158, 1072–6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waal de, F. B. M. (2001). Tree of Origin. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Waddington, C. (1975). The Evolution of an Evolutionist. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press
Wallace, A. R. (1864). The development of human races under the law of natural selection. Reprinted in A. R. Wallace (ed.). Contributions to the Theory of Natural Selection (303–31). London: Macmillan (1875)
Walsh, A. (1993). Love styles, masculinity/feminity, physical attractiveness and sexual behaviour: A test of evolutionary theory. Ethology and Sociobiology. 14, 25–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walton, G., Bower, N. and Bower, T. (1992). Recognition of familiar faces by newborns. Infant Behaviour and Development. 15, 265–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Washburn, S. L. (1968). The Study of Human Evolution. Eugene: Oregon State System of Higher Education
Washburn, S. L. and Lancaster, J. S. (1968). The evolution of hunting. In R. B. Lee and I. De Vore (eds.). Man the Hunter. Chicago: Aldine
Wason, P. C (1966). Reasoning. In B. M. Foss (ed.). New Horizons in Psychology. Harmondsworth: Penguin
Waynforth, D., Hurtado, A. M. and Hill, K. (1998). Environmentally contingent reproductive strategies in Ache and Mayan men. Evolution and Human Behavior. 19, 369–85CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weissman, M. M. (1985). The epidemiology of anxiety disorders: Rates, risks and familial patters. In H. A. Tuma and J. Masser (eds.). Anxiety and the Anxiety Disorders (275–96). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
Wellman, H. M. (1988). First steps in the child's theorizing about the mind. In J. Astington, P. Harris and D. Olson (eds.). Developing Theories of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Whiten, A. (1998). Imitation of the sequential structure of actions by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 112, 270–81CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A. and Byrne, R. (eds.). (1988). Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes and Humans. Oxford: Clarendon Press
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C. E. G., Wrangham, R. W. and Boesch, C. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature. 399, 682–5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiting, B. B. (1965). Sex identity conflict and physical violence: A comparative study. American Anthropologist. 67, 123–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkinson, G. S. (1984). Reciprocal food sharing in the vampire bat. Nature. 308, 181–4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and Natural Selection: A Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought. Princeton: Princeton University Press
Williams, G. C. (1975). Sex and Evolution. In Monographs in Population Biology. Princeton: Princeton University Press
Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Wilson, E. O. (1978). On Human Nature. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Wilson, E. O. (1994). Naturalist. Washington, DC: Island Press
Wilson, E. O. (1998). Consilience: The Unity of Knowledge. New York: Vintage Books
Wimmer, H. and Hartl, M. (1991). Against the Cartesian view on mind: Young children's difficulty with own false belief. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 9, 125–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wimmer, H. and Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition. 13, 103–28CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woolfenden, G. E. and Fitzpatrick, J. W. (1984). The Florida Scrub Jay: Demography of a Cooperative-Breeding Bird. Princeton: Princeton University Press
Workman, L., Adam, J. and Andrew, R. J. (2000). Opportunities for visual experience which might allow imprinting in chicks raised by broody hens. Behaviour. 137, 221–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Workman, L., Peters, S. and Taylor, S. (2000). Lateralisation of perceptual processing of pro- and anti-social emotions displayed in chimeric faces. Laterality. 5, 237–49CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wrangham, R. W. (1987). Evolution of social structure. In B. B. Smuts, D. L. Cheney, R. M. Seyfarth, R. W. Wrangham and T. T. Struhsaker (eds.). Primate Societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Wrangham, R. W. (1993). The evolution of sexuality in chimpanzees and bonobos. Human Nature. 4, 47–79CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, R. (1994). The Moral Animal. London: Abacus
Wynn, K. (1993). An evolved capacity for number. In D. Cummins and C. Allen (eds.). The Evolution of Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Wynn, T. (1998). Did Homo erectus speak?Cambridge Archaeological Journal. 8(1), 78–81Google Scholar
Wynne-Edwards, V. C. (1962). Animal Dispersion in Relation to Social Behaviour. Edinburgh: Oliver and Boyd
Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection – a selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 53, 205–14CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahavi, A. (2003). Indirect selection and individual selection in socio-biology: My personal views on theories of social behaviour. Animal Behaviour. 65, 859–63CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zahn-Waxler, C. and Radke-Yarrow, M. (1982). The development of altruism: Alternative research strategies. In N. Eisenberg (ed.). The Development of Prosocial Behavior (109–37). New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E. and Chapman, M. (1992). Development of concern for others. Developmental Psychology. 28, 126–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuk, M. (1992). The role of parasites in sexual selection: Current evidence and future directions. Advances in the Study of Behavior. 21, 39–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Lance Workman, Bath Spa University, Will Reader, Sheffield Hallam University
  • Book: Evolutionary Psychology
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139164757.015
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Lance Workman, Bath Spa University, Will Reader, Sheffield Hallam University
  • Book: Evolutionary Psychology
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139164757.015
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Lance Workman, Bath Spa University, Will Reader, Sheffield Hallam University
  • Book: Evolutionary Psychology
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139164757.015
Available formats
×