Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-cjp7w Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-25T23:53:25.506Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false
This chapter is part of a book that is no longer available to purchase from Cambridge Core

References

Eve V. Clark
Affiliation:
Stanford University, California
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbot-Smith, Kirsten, & Behrens, Heike. 2006. How known constructions influence the acquisition of other constructions: The German passive and future constructions. Cognitive Science 30, 995–1026.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Acredolo, Linda, & Goodwyn, Susan. 1988. Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development 59, 450–466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, Valerie. 1973. An introduction to modern English word formation. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Adone, Dany. 1994. The acquisition of Mauritian creole. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aimard, Paule. 1975. Les jeux de mots de l'enfant. Villeurbanne: Simép Editions.Google Scholar
Akhtar, Nameera. 2005. The robustness of learning through overhearing. Developmental Science 8, 199–209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Akhtar, Nameera; Carpenter, Malinda; & Tomasello, Michael. 1996. The role of discourse novelty in word learning. Child Development 67, 635–645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Akhtar, Nameera; Jipson, Jennifer; & Callanan, Maureen. 2001. Learning words through overhearing. Child Development 72, 416–430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Akhtar, Nameera, & Tomasello, Michael. 1997. Young children's productivity with word order and verb morphology. Developmental Psychology 33, 952–965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aksu-Koç, Ayhan. 1988. The acquisition of tense and modality: The case of past reference in Turkish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aksu-Koç, Ayhan, & Alıcı, Didem M. 2000. Understanding sources of beliefs and marking of uncertainty: The child's theory of evidentiality. In Clark, E. V. (ed.), Proceedings of the thirtieth annual Child Language Research Forum 1999 (123–130). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Aksu-Koç, Ayhan, & Slobin, Dan I. 1985. The acquisition of Turkish. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vol. I (839–878). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Albin, Drema D., & Echols, Catharine H. 1996. Stressed and word-final syllables in infant-directed speech. Infant Behavior & Development 19, 401–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, Shanley E. M., & Schröder, Heike. 2003. Preferred argument structure in early Inuktitut spontaneous speech data. In Du Bois, J. W., Kumpf, L. E., & Ashby, W. J. (eds.), Preferred argument structure: Grammar as architecture for function (301–338). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ameel, Eef; Storms, Gert; Malt, Barbara C.; & Sloman, Steven A. 2005. How bilinguals solve the naming problem. Journal of Memory & Language 53, 60–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ament, Wilhelm. 1899. Die Entwicklung von Sprechen und Denken beim Kinde. Leipzig: Ernst Wunderlich.Google Scholar
Ammon, Mary Sue. 1980. Development in the linguistic expression of causal relations: Comprehension of features of lexical and periphrastic causatives. PhD dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.
Ammon, Mary Sue. 1981. Semantic differentiation among expressions of causality by children and adults. Papers & Reports on Child Language Development [Stanford University] 20, 25–33.Google Scholar
Ammon, Mary Sue, & Slobin, Dan I. 1979. A cross-linguistic study of the processing of causative sentences. Cognition 7, 3–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amodeo, Luiza B., & Cárdenas, Manuel. 1983. Private speech production during task performance by bilingual and monolingual children. In Escobedo, T. H. (ed.), Early childhood bilingual education: A Hispanic perspective (50–62). New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
Andersen, Elaine S. 1990. Speaking with style: The sociolinguistic skills of children. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Anglin, Jeremy M. 1976. Word, object, and conceptual development. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Anglin, Jeremy M. 1993. Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 58 (serial no. 238).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anisfeld, Moshe; Rosenberg, Erica S.; Hoberman, Mara J.; & Gasparini, Don. 1998. Lexical acceleration coincides with the onset of combinatorial speech. First Language 18, 165–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antinucci, Francesco, & Miller, Ruth. 1976. How children talk about what happened. Journal of Child Language 3, 167–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antinucci, Francesco, & Volterra, Virginia. 1975. Lo sviluppo della negazione nel linguaggio infantile: Un studio pragmatico. Lingua e Stile 10, 231–260.Google Scholar
Archibald, John. 1995. The acquisition of stress. In Archibald, J. (ed.), Phonological acquisition and phonological theory (81–109). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ardery, Gail. 1979. The development of coordinations in child language. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 18, 745–756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ardery, Gail. 1980. On coordination in child language. Journal of Child Language 7, 305–320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnberg, Lenore. 1981. A longitudinal study of language development in four young children exposed to English and Swedish in their home. Linköping Studies in Education Reports, no. 6. Linköping, Sweden: Department of Education, University of Linköping.Google Scholar
Aronoff, Mark; Meir, Irit; & Sandler, Wendy. 2005. The paradox of sign language morphology. Language 81, 301–344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arriaga, Rose I.; Fenson, Larry; Cronan, Terry; & Pethick, Stephen J. 1998. Scores on the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory of children from low- and middle-income families. Applied Psycholinguistics 19, 209–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aslin, Richard N. 2007. What's in a look?Developmental Science 10, 48–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aslin, Richard N.; Saffran, Jenny R.; & Newport, Elissa L. 1998. Computation of conditional probabilities by 8-month-old infants. Psychological Science 9, 321–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aslin, Richard N.; Saffran, Jenny R.; & Newport, Elissa L. 1999. Statistical learning in linguistic and nonlinguistic domains. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), The emergence of language (359–380). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Au, Terry K.-F.; Dapretto, Mirella; & Song, You-Kyung. 1994. Input vs. constraints: Early word acquisition in Korean and English. Journal of Memory & Language 33, 567–582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Au, Terry K.-F., & Markman, Ellen M. 1987. Acquiring word meanings via linguistic contrast. Cognitive Development 2, 217–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Austin, John L. 1962. How to do things with words. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Aviezer, Ora. 2003. Bedtime talk of three-year-olds: Collaborative repair of miscommunication. First Language 23, 117–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Axia, Giovanna. 1996. How to persuade mum to buy a toy. First Language 16, 301–317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Axia, Giovanna, & Baroni, Maria R. 1985. Linguistic politeness at different ages. Child Development 56, 918–927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harold. 1994. Productivity in language production. Language & Cognitive Processes 9, 447–469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, Dare A. 1989. Priorities in children's expectations about object-label reference: Form over color. Child Development 60, 1291–1306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, Dare A. 1991. Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development 62, 875–890.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, Dare A. 1993. Infants' ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference. Journal of Child Language 20, 395–418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, Dare A.; Markman, Ellen M.; Bill, Brigitte; Desjardins, Renee; Irwin, Jane M.; & Tidball, Glynnis. 1996. Infants' reliance on a social criterion for establishing word-object relations. Child Development 67, 3135–3153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ballem, Kate D., & Plunkett, Kim. 2005. Phonological specificity in children at 1;2. Journal of Child Language 32, 159–173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bamberg, Michael G. W. 1987. The acquisition of narratives: Learning to use language. Berlin and New York: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baratz, Joan C. 1969. Teaching reading in an urban Negro school system. In Baratz, J. C. & Shuy, R. W. (eds.), Teaching Black children to read (92–116). Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Baratz, Joan C. 1970. Educational considerations for teaching Standard English to Negro children. In Fasold, R. W. & Shuy, R. W. (eds.), Teaching Standard English in the Inner City (20–40). Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Baratz, Joan C. 1971. Language and cognitive assessment of Negro children: Assumptions and research needs. In Barker, L. L. & Kibler, R. J. (eds.), Speech communication behavior: Perspectives and principles (133–143). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Bard, Ellen G., & Anderson, Anne H. 1994. The unintelligibility of speech to children: Effects of referent availability. Journal of Child Language 21, 623–648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barinaga, Marcia. 1995. Researchers get a sharper image of the human brain. Science 268, 803–804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baroni, Maria R., & Axia, Giovanna. 1989. Children's meta-pragmatic abilities and the identification of polite and impolite requests. First Language 9, 285–297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barresi, John, & Moore, Chris. 1993. Sharing a perspective precedes understanding of that perspective. Behavioral & Brain Sciences 16, 513–514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, Martyn D. 1978. Lexical development and overextension in child language. Journal of Child Language 5, 205–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, Martyn D. 1995. Early lexical development. In Fletcher, P. & MacWhinney, B. (eds.), Handbook of child language (362–392). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Barton, David. 1978. The discrimination of minimally-different pairs of real words by children aged 2;3 to 2;11. In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (eds.), The development of communication (255–261). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Barton, Michelle E., & Tomasello, Michael. 1991. Joint attention and conversation in mother-infant-sibling triads. Child Development 62, 517–529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barton, Michelle E., & Tomasello, Michael. 1994. The rest of the family: The role of fathers and siblings in early language development. In Gallaway, C. & Richards, B. J. (eds.), Input and interaction in language acquisition (109–134). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bassano, Dominique, & Eme, Pascale-Elsa. 2001. Development of noun determiner use in French children: Lexical and grammatical bases. In Almgren, M., Barreña, A., Ezeizabarrena, M.-J., Idiazabal, I., & MacWhinney, B. (eds.), Research on child language acquisition: Proceedings of the 8th Congress of the International Society for the Study of Child Language (1207–1220). Boston, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Basser, L. S. 1962. Hemiplegia of early onset and the faculty of speech with special reference to the effects of hemispherectomy. Brain 85, 427–460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, Elizabeth. 1976. Language and context: The acquisition of pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, Elizabeth; Camaioni, Luigia; & Volterra, Virginia. 1975. The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 21, 205–226.Google Scholar
Bates, Elizabeth, & Silvern, Louise. 1977. Social adjustment and politeness in preschoolers. Journal of Communication 27, 104–111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, Elizabeth; Thal, Donna; Finlay, Barbara; & Clancy, Barbara. 2002. Early language development and its neural correlates. In Boller, F. & Grafman, J. (series eds.), Segalowitz, S. J. & Rapin, I. (vol. eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology, vol. VIII: Child neurology (2nd edn, 109–176) Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Courtenay, Baudouin, Jan. 1974. Spostrzezenia nad jezykiem dziecka [Observations on child language] (ed. Chmura-Klekotowa, M.). Wroclaw: Polska Akademia Nauk, Komitet Jezykoznawstwa.Google Scholar
Bauer, Laurie. 1983. English word-formation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumgartner, Emma, & Devescovi, Antonella. 1996. Come e perché nelle favole raccontate dai bambini. Ripatransone: Edizioni Sestante.Google Scholar
Bavelier, Daphne; Corina, David; Jezzard, Peter; Clark, Vince; Karni, Avi; Lalwani, Anil; Rauschecker, Josef P.; Braun, Allen; Turner, Robert; & Neville, Helen J. 1998. Hemispheric specialization for English and ASL: Left invariance–right variability. Neuroreport: An International Journal for the Rapid Communication of Research in Neuroscience 9, 1537–1542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Becker, Judith A. 1982. Children's strategic uses of requests to mark and manipulate social status. In Kuczaj II, S. A. (ed.), Language development, vol. II (1–35). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Becker, Judith A. 1994. “Sneak-shoes,” “sworders,” and “nose-beards”: A case study of lexical innovation. First Language 14, 195–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Behne, Tanya; Carpenter, Malinda, & Tomasello, Michael. 2006. One-year-olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind gestures in a hiding game. Developmental Science 8, 492–499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Behrend, Douglas A. 1990. Constraints and development: A reply to Nelson (1988). Cognitive Development 5, 313–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Behrend, Douglas A.; Harris, L. Lynn; & Cartwright, Kelly B. 1995. Morphological clues to verb meaning: Verb inflections and the initial mapping of verb meanings. Journal of Child Language 22, 89–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Behrens, Heike, & Gut, Ulrike. 2005. The relationship between prosodic and syntactic organization in early multiword speech. Journal of Child Language 31, 2–24.Google Scholar
Benoit, Pamela J. 1982. Formal coherence production in children's discourse. First Language 3, 161–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benson, Nancy J., & Anglin, Jeremy M. 1987. The child's knowledge of English kin terms. First Language 7, 41–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berko, Jean. 1958. The child's learning of English morphology. Word 14, 150–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berko, Jean, & Brown, Roger. 1960. Psycholinguistic research methods. In Mussen, P. H. (ed.), Handbook of research methods in child development (517–557). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Berko Gleason, Jean. 1977. Talking to children: Some notes on feedback. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (199–205). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Berko Gleason, Jean. 1988. Language and socialization. In Kessel, F. S. (ed.), The development of language and language researchers (269–280). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berko Gleason, Jean, & Greif, Esther B. 1983. Men's speech to young children. In Thorne, B., Kramarae, C., & Henley, N. (eds.), Language, gender, and society (140–149). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Berko Gleason, Jean; Perlman, Rivka Y.; & Greif, Esther. 1984. What's the magic word: Learning language through politeness routines. Discourse Processes 7, 493–502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlin, Brent; Breedlove, David E.; & Raven, Peter H. 1973. General principles of classification and nomenclature in folk biology. American Anthropologist 75, 214–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, Ruth A. 1982. Verb-pattern alternation: The interface of morphology, syntax, and semantics in Hebrew child language. Journal of Child Language 9, 169–191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berman, Ruth A. 1985. The acquisition of Hebrew. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vol. I (255–371). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berman, Ruth A. 1987. Productivity in the lexicon: New-word formation in modern Hebrew. Folia Linguistica 21, 425–461.Google Scholar
Berman, Ruth A., & Armon-Lotem, Sharon. 1997. How grammatical are early verbs? In Actes du colloque international sur l'acquisition de la syntaxe en langue maternelle et en langue étrangère (17–56). Nancy, France: Annales Littéraires de l'Université de Franche-Comté, no. 631.Google Scholar
Berman, Ruth A., & Clark, Eve V. 1989. Learning to use compounds for contrast: Data from Hebrew. First Language 9, 247–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, Ruth A., & Slobin, Dan I. 1994a. Relating events in narrative: A cross-linguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berman, Ruth A., & Slobin, Dan I. 1994b. Becoming a proficient speaker (Chapter Va); Becoming a native speaker (Chapter Vb). In Berman, R. A. & Slobin, D. I., Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study (597–610, 611–641). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bernicot, Josie, & Roux, Mireille. 1998. La structure et l'usage des énoncés: Comparaison d'enfants uniques et d'enfants seconds-nés. In Bernicot, J., Marcos, H., Day, C., Guidetti, M., Laval, V., Rabain-Jamin, J., & Babelot, G. (eds.), De l'usage des gestes et des mots chez l'enfant (157–178). Paris: Colin.Google Scholar
Bernstein-Ratner, Nan, & Pye, Clifton. 1984. Higher pitch in BT is not universal: Acoustic evidence from Quiché Mayan. Journal of Child Language 11, 515–522.Google Scholar
Bertoncini, Josiane; Bijeljac-Babic, Ranka; Blumstein, Sheila E.; & Mehler, Jacques. 1987. Discrimination in neonates of very short CVs. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 82, 31–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertoncini, Josiane, & Mehler, Jacques. 1981. Syllables as units in infants' speech perception. Infant Behavior & Development 4, 247–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, Ellen. 2001. Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, Ellen. 2007. Language acquisition and bilingualism: Consequences for a multilingual society. Applied Psycholingistics 28, 393–397.Google Scholar
Bialystok, Ellen, & Hakuta, Kenji. 1994. In other words: The science and psychology of second-language acquisition. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bialystok, Ellen, & Hakuta, Kenji. 1999. Confounded age: Linguistics and cognitive factors in age differences for second language acquisition. In Birdsong, D. (ed.), Second language acquisition and the critical period (161–181). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bishop, Dorothy V. M. 1997. Uncommon understanding: Development and disorders of language comprehension in children. Hove, Sussex: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Blake, Joanna, & Boysson-Bardies, Bénédicte. 1992. Patterns in babbling: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Child Language 19, 51–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blake, Joanna; Osborne, Patricia; Cabral, Marlene; & Gluck, Pamela. 2003. The development of communicative gestures in Japanese infants. First Language 23, 3–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloch, Oscar. 1924. La phrase dans le langage de l'enfant. Journal de Psychologie 21, 18–43.Google Scholar
Bloom, Lois. 1970. Language development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, Lois. 1973. One word at a time. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bloom, Lois. 1991. Language development from two to three. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, Lois. 1997. Intentionality is the basis for the social foundations of language development. Paper presented at the symposium on Social Foundations of Language Development: Theoretical Issues, Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research on Child Development, Washington, DC.
Bloom, Lois; Hood, Lois; & Lightbown, Patsy M. 1974. Imitation in language development: If, when, and why. Cognitive Development 6, 380–420.Google Scholar
Bloom, Lois; Lahey, Margaret; Hood, Lois; Lifter, Karen; & Fiess, Karen. 1980. Complex sentences: Acquisition of syntactic connectives and the meaning relations they encode. Journal of Child Language 7, 235–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, Lois; Lifter, Karen; & Hafitz, Jeremie. 1980. Semantics of verbs and the development of verb inflection in child language. Language 56, 386–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, Lois; Margulis, Cheryl; Tinker, Erin; & Fujita, Naomi. 1996. Early conversations and word learning: Contributions from child and adult. Child Development 67, 3154–3175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, Lois; Miller, Patsy; & Hood, Lois. 1975. Variation and reduction as aspects of competence in language development. In Pick, A. (ed.), Minnesota symposia on child psychology, vol. IX (3–55). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, Lois; Rispoli, Matthew; Gartner, Barbara; & Hafitz, Jeremie. 1989. Acquisition of complementation. Journal of Child Language 16, 101–120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, Lois; Tackeff, Jo; & Lahey, Margaret. 1984. Learning to in complement constructions. Journal of Child Language 11, 391–406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, Lois; Tinker, Erin; & Margulis, Cheryl. 1993. The words children learn: Evidence against a noun bias in early vocabularies. Cognitive Development 8, 431–450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, Paul. 2000. How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Boas, Franz. 1911. Introduction, Handbook of American Indian languages. Bulletin 40, Part I, Bureau of American Ethnology. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. (Reprinted in F. Boas, Introduction to handbook of American Indian languages. In J. W. Powell, Indian linguistic families of America north of Mexico [ed. P. Holder]. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1966.)Google Scholar
Bock, J. Kathryn, & Miller, Carol A. 1991. Broken agreement. Cognitive Psychology 23, 45–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohn, William E. 1914. First steps in verbal expression. Pedagogical Seminary 21, 578–595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolinger, Dwight. 1972. That's that. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bolinger, Dwight. 1975. Aspects of language (2nd edn). New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Bolinger, Dwight. 1976. Meaning and memory. Forum Linguisticum 1, 1–14.Google Scholar
Bolinger, Dwight. 1977. Idioms have relations. Forum Linguisticum 2, 157–169.Google Scholar
Borer, Hagit, & Wexler, Ken. 1987. The maturation of syntax. In Roeper, T. & Williams, E. (eds.), Parameter setting (123–172). Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosch, Laura; Ramón, Marta; Águila, Eva; & Pons, Ferrán. 2005. Efecto de la exposición bilingüe sobre el desarrollo léxico inicial. In Díez-Villoria, E., Zubiauz, B., & Cinca, M. A. Mayor (eds.), Estudio sobre la adquisición del lenguaje (676–692). Salamanca, ES: Ediciones Universidad de Salamanca.Google Scholar
Bosch, Laura, & Sebastián-Gallés, Núria. 2001. Evidence of early language discrimination abilities in infants from bilingual environments. Infancy 2, 29–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouvier, Nadine, & Platone, Françoise. 1976. Etude génétique de la construction d'une détermination linguistique complexe: L'expression d'un même contenu par des enfants d'âges différents. Etudes sur le développement du langage de l'enfant, vol. I, 16A (25–165). Paris: Centre de Recherche de l'Education Spécialisée et de l'Adaptation Scolaire (Institut Pédagogique National).Google Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa. 1973a. Early syntactic development: A cross-linguistic study with special reference to Finnish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa. 1973b. Structural relationships in children's utterances: Syntactic or semantic? In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (197–213). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa. 1974. Learning the structure of causative verbs: A study in the relationship of cognitive, semantic, and syntactic development. Papers & Reports on Child Language Development [Stanford University] 8, 142–178.Google Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa. 1982a. Evaluating competing linguistic models with language acquisition data: Implications of developmental errors with causative verbs. Quaderni di Semantica 3, 5–66.Google Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa. 1982b. Reorganizational processes in lexical and syntactic development. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (319–346). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa. 1985a. Beyond communicative adequacy: From piecemeal knowledge to an integrated system in the child's acquisition of language. In Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children's language, vol. V (369–398). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa. 1985b. What shapes children's grammars? In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vol. II (1257–1319). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa. 1986. First steps in acquiring conditionals. In Traugott, E. C., Meulen, A., Reilly, J. S., & Ferguson, C. A. (eds.), On conditionals (285–307). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa. 1988. The “no negative evidence” problem: How do children avoid constructing an overly general grammar? In Hawkins, J. A. (ed.), Explaining language universals (71–101). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa. 1990. Mapping thematic roles onto syntactic functions: Are children helped by innate linking rules?Linguistics 28, 1253–1289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa. 1996a. Argument structure and learnability: Is a solution in sight? In Johnson, K., Juge, M. L., & Moxley, J. L. (eds.), Proceedings of the 22nd annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, Parasession on the role of learnability in grammatical theory (454–468). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa. 1996b. Learning how to structure space for language: A crosslinguistic perspective. In Bloom, P., Peterson, M. A., Nadel, L., & Garrett, M. F. (eds.), Language and space (385–436). Cambridge, MA: MI.Google Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa. 2005. Why can't you “open” a nut or “break” a noodle? Learning covert object categories in action word meanings. In Gershkoff-Stowe, L. & Rakison, D. H. (eds.), Building object categories in developmental time (209–243). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa, & Croft, William. 2008. The acquisition of the English causative. In Bowerman, M. & Brown, P. (eds.), Crosslinguistic perspectives on argument structure: Implications for learnability (279–308). New York: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Boyle, Mary K., & Gerken, LouAnn. 1997. The influence of lexical familiarity on children's function morpheme omissions: A nonmetrical effect?Journal of Memory & Language 36, 117–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braine, Martin D. S. 1963. On learning the grammatical order of words. Psychological Review 70, 323–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braine, Martin D. S. 1971. The acquisition of language in infant and child. In Reed, C. (ed.), The learning of language (7–95). New York: Appleton.Google Scholar
Braine, Martin D. S. 1976. Children's first word combinations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 41 (serial no. 164).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braine, Martin D. S.; Brody, Ruth E.; Fisch, Shalom M.; & Weisberger, Mara J. 1990. Can children use a verb without exposure to its argument structure?Journal of Child Language 17, 313–342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandone, Amanda C.; Pence, Khara L.; Golinkoff, Roberta M.; & Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy. 2007. Action speaks louder than words: Young children differentially weight perceptual, social, and linguistic cues to learn verbs. Child Development 78, 1322–1342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Branigan, George. 1976. Syllabic structure and the acquisition of consonants: The great conspiracy in word formation. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 5, 117–133.Google Scholar
Branigan, George. 1979. Some reasons why successive single word utterances are not. Journal of Child Language 6, 411–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braunwald, Susan R. 1995. Differences in the acquisition of early verbs: Evidence from diary data from sisters. In Tomasello, M. & Merriman, W. E. (eds.), Beyond names for things: Young children's acquisition of verbs (81–111). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bréal, Michel. 1897. Essai de sémantique. Paris: Hachette.Google Scholar
Brent, Michael R., & Cartwright, Timothy A. 1996. Distributional regularity and phonotactic constraints are useful for segmentation. Cognition 61, 93–125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bresson, François. 1977. Semantics, syntax and utterance: Determining a referent. International Journal of Psycholinguistics 4, 31–41.Google Scholar
Bresson, François; Bouvier, Nadine; Dannequin, Claudine; Depreux, J.; Hardy, Marianne; & Platone, Françoise. 1970. Quelques aspects du système des déterminants chez les enfants de l'école maternelle: Utilisation des articles défini et indéfini. 2 (3–40). Paris: Centre de Recherche de l'Education Spécialisé et de l'Adaptation Scolaire (Institut Pédagogique National).Google Scholar
Bretherton, Inge. 1984. Representing the social world in symbolic play: Reality and fantasy. In Bretherton, I. (ed.), Symbolic play: The development of social understanding (3–41). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, William F., & Stone, J. Brandon. 1975. Acquisition of spatial antonym pairs. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 19, 299–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brinkmann, Ursula. 1995. The locative alternation: Its structure and acquisition. PhD dissertation, Katholieke Universiteit Nijmegen.
Broca, Paul. 1856. Des aneurysmes et de leur traitement. Paris: Labe.Google Scholar
Broca, Paul. 1861. Remarques sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé, suivies d'une observation d'aphémie. Bulletin de la Société d'Anatomie 5, 33–357.Google Scholar
Broen, Patricia A. 1972. The verbal environment of the language-learning child. Monograph of the American Speech & Hearing Association 17.Google Scholar
Brooks, Patricia J.; Tomasello, Michael; Dodson, Kelly; & Lewis, Lawrence B. 1999. Young children's overgeneralizations with fixed transitivity verbs: The entrenchment hypothesis. Child Development, 70, 1325–1337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, H. Douglas. 1971. Children's comprehension of relativized English sentences. Child Development 42, 1923–1936.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, Penelope. 1998. Children's first verbs in Tzeltal: Evidence for an early verb category. Linguistics 36, 713–753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, Penelope, & Levinson, Stephen C. 1987. Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, Roger. 1957. Linguistic determinism and the part of speech. Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology 55, 1–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, Roger. 1958a. How shall a thing be called?Psychological Review 65, 14–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, Roger. 1958b. Words and things. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Brown, Roger. 1968. The development of wh questions in child speech. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 7, 279–290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, Roger. 1973. A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, Roger, & Bellugi, Ursula. 1964. Three processes in the child's acquisition of syntax. Harvard Educational Review 34, 133–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, Roger, & Hanlon, Camille. 1970. Derivational complexity and order of acquisition in child speech. In Hayes, J. R. (ed.), Cognition and the development of language (11–53). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bruner, Jerome S. 1975. The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language 2, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, Jerome S. 1983. Child's talk: Learning to use language. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Budwig, Nancy. 1989. The linguistic marking of agentivity and control in child language. Journal of Child Language 16, 263–284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Budwig, Nancy. 1990. The linguistic marking of nonprototypical agency: An exploration into children's use of passives. Linguistics 28, 1221–1252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Budwig, Nancy. 1995. A developmental-functionalist approach to child language. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bunta, Ferenc; Davidovich, Ingrid; & Ingram, David. 2006. The relationship between the phonological complexity of a bilingual child's words and those of the target languages. International Journal of Bilingualism 10, 71–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buresh, Jennifer S., & Woodward, Amanda L. 2007. Infants track goals within and across agents. Cognition 104, 287–314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buresh, Jennifer S.; Woodward, Amanda; & Brune, Camille W. 2006. The roots of verbs in prelinguistic action knowledge. In Hirsh-Pasek, K. & Golinkoff, R. M. (eds.), How children learn verbs (208–277). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Burling, Robbins. 1959. Language development of a Garo and English speaking child. Word 15, 45–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burns, Tracey C.; Yoshida, Katherine A.; Hill, Karen; & Werker, Janet F. 2007. The development of phonetic representation in bilingual and monolingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics 28, 455–474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, George, & Cochran, Edward. 1980. What minds have in common in space: A perceptual mechanism for joint reference in infancy. International Journal of Behavioural Development 3, 253–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, George, & Jarrett, Nicholas. 1991. What minds have in common in space: Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 9, 55–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 1985. Morphology: A study of the relation between meaning and form. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 1995. Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language & Cognitive Processes 10, 425–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 1998. The emergent lexicon. In Gruber, M. C., Higgins, D., Olson, K. S., & Wysocki, T. (eds.), Proceedings of the Chicago Linguistic Society 34: The Panels (421–435). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., & Slobin, Dan I. 1982. Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past tense. Language 58, 265–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callanan, Maureen A. 1985. How parents label objects for young children: The role of input in the acquisition of category hierarchies. Child Development 56, 508–523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callanan, Maureen A. 1989. Maternal speech strategies and children's acquisition of hierarchical category labels. Genetic Psychology 17 (2), 3–12.Google Scholar
Callanan, Maureen A. 1990. Parents' descriptions of objects: Potential data for children's inferences about category principles. Cognitive Development 5, 101–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callanan, Maureen, & Sabbagh, Mark A. 2004. Multiple labels for objects in conversations between young children: Parents' language and children's developing expectations about word meanings. Developmental Psychology 40, 746–763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, Robin N. 2006. Prescientific studies of language development. In Brown, K. (gen. ed.), Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics (2nd edn), vol. VI (391–394). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capirci, Olga; Contaldo, Annarita; Caselli, M. Cristina; & Volterra, Virginia. 2005. From action to language through gesture. Gesture 5, 155–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capirci, Olga; Iverson, Jana M.; Pizzuto, Elena; & Volterra, Virginia. 1996. Gestures and words during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of Child Language 23, 645–673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carey, Susan. 1985. Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Carey, Susan, & Bartlett, Elsa. 1978. Acquiring a single new word. Papers & Reports on Child Language Development [Stanford University] 15, 17–29.Google Scholar
Carmichael, L.; Hogan, H. P.; & Walter, A. A. 1932. An experimental study of the effect of language on the reproduction of visually perceived forms. Journal of Experimental Psychology 15, 73–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carter, Allyson, & Gerken, LouAnn. 2004. Do children's omissions leave traces?Journal of Child Language 31, 561–586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, Anne L. 1978. From sensori-motor vocalizations to words: A case study of the evolution of attention-directing communication in the second year. In Lock, A. (ed.), Action, gesture, and symbol: The emergence of language (309–349). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Carter, Anne L. 1979. The disappearance schema: Case study of a second-year communicative behavior. In Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. B. (eds.), Developmental pragmatics (131–156). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cartmill, Erica A., & Byrne, Richard W. 2007. Orangutans modify their gestural signaling according to their audience's comprehension. Current Biology 17, 1345–1348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cartwright, Timothy A., & Brent, Michael R. 1997. Syntactic categorization in early language acquisition: Formalizing the role of distributional analysis. Cognition 63, 121–170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casadio, Paola, & Caselli, M. Cristina. 1989. Il primo vocabulario del bambino: Gesti e parole a 14 mesi. Età Evolutiva 33, 32–42.Google Scholar
Caselli, M. Cristina. 1983. Gesti comunicativi e primi parole. Età Evolutiva 16, 36–51.Google Scholar
Cazden, Courtney B. 1968. The acquisition of noun and verb inflections. Child Development 39, 433–448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cazden, Courtney B.; John, Vera P.; & Hymes, Dell (eds.). 1972. Functions of language in the classroom. Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland Press.
Chafe, Wallace. 1970. Meaning and the structure of language. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Chambers, James C., & Tavuchis, Nicholas. 1976. Kids and kin: Children's understanding of American kin terms. Journal of Child Language 3, 63–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chambers, J. K. 1992. Dialect acquisition. Language 68, 673–705.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chambers, J. K. 2003. Sociolinguistic theory: Linguistic variation and social significance (2nd edn). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Cheung, Sik Lee. 1998. Causative verbs in child Cantonese. In Clark, E. V. (ed.), Proceedings of the 29th annual Child Language Research Forum 1997 (151–160). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Chmura-Klekotowa, Maria. 1970. Odbicie tendencji slowotwórczych jezyka polskiego w neologizmach dzieci [Reflection of derivational trends of the Polish language in children's neologisms]. Prace Filologiczne (Warsaw) 20, 153–159.Google Scholar
Choi, Soonja. 1999. Early development of verb structures and caregiver input in Korean: Two case studies. International Journal of Bilingualism 3, 241–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, Soonja. 2000. Caregiver input in English and Korean: Use of nouns and verbs in book-reading and toy-play contexts. Journal of Child Language 27, 69–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, Soonja. 2006. Influence of language-specific input on spatial cognition: Categories of containment. First Language 26, 207–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, Soonja, & Bowerman, Melissa. 1991. Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: The influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns. Cognition 41, 83–121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, Soonja, & Gopnik, Alison. 1995. Early acquisition of verbs in Korean: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Child Language 22, 497–529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, Soonja; McDonough, Laraine; Bowerman, Melissa; & Mandler, Jean M. 1999. Early sensitivity to language-specific spatial categories in English and Korean. Cognitive Development 14, 241–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Carol S. 1969. The acquisition of syntax in children from 5 to 10. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1965. Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1972. Language and mind (enlarged edn). New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Chouinard, Michelle M., & Clark, Eve V. 2003. Adult reformulations of child errors as negative evidence. Journal of Child Language 30, 637–669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christophe, Anne; Mehler, Jacques; & Sebastián-Gallés, Núria. 2001. Perception of prosodic boundary correlates by newborn infants. Infancy 2, 38–394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, Harald, & Almazan, Mayella. 1998. Syntax and morphology in Williams syndrome. Cognition 68, 167–198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clancy, Patricia M. 1985. The acquisition of Japanese. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vol. I (373–524). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Clancy, Patricia M. 1996. Referential strategies and the co-construction of argument structure in Korean acquisition. In Fox, B. (ed.), Studies in anaphora (33–68). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clancy, Patricia M. 2003. The lexicon in interaction: Developmental origins of preferred argument structure in Korean. In Du Bois, J. W., Kumpf, L., & Ashby, W. (eds.), Preferred argument structure: Grammar as architecture for function (81–108) Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1970. How young children describe events in time. In Flores d'Arcais, G. B. & Levelt, W. J. M. (eds.), Advances in psycholinguistics (275–293). Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1971. On the acquisition of before and after. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 10, 266–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1972. On the child's acquisition of antonyms in two semantic fields. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 11, 750–758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1973a. What's in a word? On the child's acquisition of semantics in his first language. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (65–110). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1973b. Nonlinguistic strategies and the acquisition of word meaning. Cognition 2, 161–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1973c. How children describe time and order. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds.), Studies of child language development (585–606). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1977. Universal categories: On the semantics of classifiers and children's early word meanings. In Juilland, A. (ed.), Linguistic studies offered to Joseph Greenberg (449–462). Saratoga, CA: Anma Libri.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1978a. Awareness of language: Some evidence from what children say and do. In Sinclair, A., Jarvella, R., & Levelt, W. J. M. (eds.), The child's conception of language (17–43). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1978b. From gesture to word: On the natural history of deixis in language acquisition. In Bruner, J. & Garton, A. (eds.), Human growth and development: Wolfson College lectures 1976 (95–120). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1978c. Discovering what words can do. In Farkas, D., Jacobsen, W. M., & Todrys, K. W. (eds.), Papers from the parasession on the lexicon (34–57). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1979. Building a vocabulary: Words for objects, actions, and relations. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (eds.), Language acquisition (149–160). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1980. Here's the top: Nonlinguistic strategies in the acquisition of orientational terms. Child Development 51, 329–338.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1981. Negative verbs in children's speech. In Klein, W. & Levelt, W. J. M. (eds.), Crossing the boundaries in linguistics (253–264). Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1982. Language change during language acquisition. In Lamb, M. E. & Brown, A. L. (eds.), Advances in developmental psychology, vol. II (173–197). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1983. Meanings and concepts. In Flavell, J. H. & Markman, E. M. (eds.), Handbook of child psychology, vol. III: Cognitive development (787–840). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1984. Acquiring compounds. In Alvarez, G., Brodie, B., & McCoy, T. (eds.), Proceeedings of the Eastern States Conference on Linguistics 1984 (181–190). Columbus: Department of Linguistics, Ohio State University.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1987. The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (1–33). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1988. On the logic of contrast. Journal of Child Language 15, 317–335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, Eve V. 1990. The pragmatics of contrast. Journal of Child Language 17, 417–431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, Eve V. 1993. The lexicon in acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1995. Later lexical development and word formation. In Fletcher, P. & MacWhinney, B. (eds.), The handbook of child language (393–412). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1996. Early verbs, event types, and inflections. In Johnson, C. E. & Gilbert, J. H. V. (eds.), Children's language, vol. IX (61–73). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1997. Conceptual perspective and lexical choice in acquisition. Cognition 64, 1–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, Eve V. 1998. Lexical structure and pragmatic directions in acquisition. In Gruber, M. C., Higgins, D., Olson, K. S., & Wysocki, T. (eds.), Papers from the 34th meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society: The Panels (437–446). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1999. Acquisition in the course of conversation. Studies in Linguistic Sciences (Special Issue, Forum Lectures 1999, LSA Summer Institute) 29, 1–18.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 2001. Emergent categories in first language acquisition. In Bowerman, M. & Levinson, S. C. (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (379–405). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 2002a. Making use of pragmatic inferences in the acquisition of meaning. In Beaver, D., Kaufmann, S., Clark, B., & Casillas, L. (eds.), The construction of meaning (45–58). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 2002b. Grounding and attention in language acquisition. In Andronis, M., Ball, C., Elston, H., & Neuvel, S. (eds.), Papers from the 37th meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, vol. I (95–116). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 2006. Color, reference, and expertise in language acquisition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 94, 339–343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, Eve V. 2007. Young children's uptake of new words in conversation. Language in Society 36, 157–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 2009. Adult offer, word-class, and child uptake in early lexical acquisition. First Language 00, in press.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V., & Berman, Ruth A. 1984. Structure and use in the acquisition of word-formation. Language 60, 547–590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V., & Berman, Ruth A. 1987. Types of linguistic knowledge: Interpreting and producing compound nouns. Journal of Child Language 14, 547–567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, Eve V., & Bernicot, Josie. 2008. Repetition as ratification: How parents and children place information in common ground. Journal of Child Language 35, 349–371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, Eve V., & Bowerman, Melissa. 1986. On the acquisition of final voiced stops. In Fishman, J. A. (ed.), The Fergusonian impact, vol. I: From phonology to society (51–68). Berlin and Amsterdam: Mouton/de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V., & Carpenter, Kathie L. 1989a. The notion of source in language acquisition. Language 65, 1–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V., & Carpenter, Kathie L. 1989b. On children's uses of from, by, and with in oblique noun phrases. Journal of Child Language 16, 349–364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, Eve V.; Carpenter, Kathie L.; & Deutsch, Werner. 1995. Reference states and reversals: Undoing actions with verbs. Journal of Child Language 22, 633–662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, Eve V., & Clark, Herbert H. 1979. When nouns surface as verbs. Language 55, 767–811.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V., & Cohen, Sophia R. 1984. Productivity and memory for newly-formed words. Journal of Child Language 11, 611–625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, Eve V., & Garnica, Olga K. 1974. Is he coming or going? On the acquisition of deictic verbs. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 13, 559–572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V.; Gelman, Susan A.; & Lane, Nancy M. 1985. Compound nouns and category structure in young children. Child Development 56, 84–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V., & Grossman, James B. 1998. Pragmatic directions and children's word learning. Journal of Child Language 25, 1–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, Eve V., & Hecht, Barbara F. 1982. Learning to coin agent and instrument nouns. Cognition 12, 1–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, Eve V.; Hecht, Barbara F.; & Mulford, Randa C. 1986. Acquiring complex compounds: Affixes and word order in English. Linguistics 24, 7–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V.; Neel-Gordon, Amy; & Johnson, Susan. 1993. Convention and contrast in the acquisition of verbs. Paper presented at the Sixth International Congress for the Study of Child Language, Trieste, Italy.
Clark, Eve V., & Nikitina, Tatiana. 2009. One vs. more than one: Antecedents to plurality in early language acquisition. Linguistics 47(1), in press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V., & Sengul, C. J. 1978. Strategies in the acquisition of deixis. Journal of Child Language 5, 457–475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V., & Svaib, Trisha A. 1997. Speaker perspective and reference in young children. First Language 17, 57–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V., & Wong, Andrew D.-W. 2002. Pragmatic directions about language use: Words and word meanings. Language in Society 31, 181–212.Google Scholar
Clark, Herbert H. 1973. Space, time, semantics, and the child. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (27–63). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Herbert H. 1996. Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Herbert H., & Marshall, Catherine R. 1981. Definite reference and mutual knowledge. In Joshi, A. K., Weber, B. L., & Sag, I. A. (eds.), Elements of discourse understanding (10–63). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, Ruth A., & Delia, Jesse G. 1976. The development of functional persuasive skills in childhood and early adolescence. Child Development 47, 1008–1014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen-Bacri, Jean. 1978. Langage et processus cognitifs: Interprétation de phrases complexes avec proposition relative chez l'enfant. La Linguistique 14, 89–110.Google Scholar
Colas, Annie. 1999. Introducing infants to referential events: A developmental study of material ostensive marking in French. Journal of Child Language 26, 113–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collis, Glyn M. 1977. Visual co-orientation and maternal speech. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction (355–375). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Collis, Glyn M., & Schaffer, H. Rudolph. 1975. Synchronization of visual attention in mother-infant pairs. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 16, 315–320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Compayré, Gabriel. 1896. L'évolution intellectuelle et morale de l'enfant (2nd edn). Paris: Hachette.Google Scholar
Cook-Gumperz, Jenny, & Corsaro, William A. 1977. Social-ecological constraints on children's communicative strategies. Sociology 11, 411–434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook-Gumperz, Jenny, & Scales, Barbara. 1996. Girls, boys, and just people: The interactional accomplishment of gender in the discourse of the nursery school. In Slobin, D. I., Gerhardt, J., Kyratzis, A., & Guo, J. (eds.), Social interaction, social context, and language: Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (513–527). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Corsaro, William A. 1977. The clarification request as a feature of adult interactive styles with young children. Language in Society 6, 183–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corsaro, William A., & Rizzo, Thomas A. 1990. Disputes in the peer culture of American and Italian nursery-school children. In Grimshaw, A. D. (ed.), Conflict talk: Sociolinguistic investigations of arguments in conversations (21–66). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Corson, David. 1995. Using English words. Boston: Kluwer Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Courtney, Susan M., & Ungerleider, Leslie G. 1997. What fMRI has taught us about human vision. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 7, 554–561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courtney, Susan M.; Ungerleider, Leslie G.; Keil, Katrina; & Haxby, James V. 1997. Transient and sustained activity in a distributed neural system for human working memory. Nature 386, 608–611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croft, William. 1991. Syntactic categories and grammatical relations. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Croft, William. 2000. Explaining language change: An evolutionary approach. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Crutchley, Alison. 2004. ‘If she had of shutted the cage, the rabbit wouldn't escape’: Past counterfactuals elicited from 6- to 11-year-old children. First Language 24, 209–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cruttenden, Alan. 1994. Phonetic and prosodic aspects of Baby Talk. In Gallaway, C. & Richards, B. J. (eds.), Input and interaction in language acquisition (135–152). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csibra, Gergely; Bíró, Szilvia; Koos, Orsolya; & Gergely, György. 2003. One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively. Cognitive Science 27, 111–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curtiss, Susan. 1977. Genie: A psycholinguistic study of a modern-day “wild child.”New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Curtiss, Susan. 1988. Abnormal language acquisition and the modularity of language. In Newmeyer, F. (ed.), Linguistics: The Cambridge survey, vol. II: Linguistic Theory: Extensions and implications (96–116). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cutting, James E., & Rosner, Burton S. 1974. Categories and boundaries in speech and music. Perception & Psychophysics 16, 564–570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dabrowska, Ewa. 2000. From formula to schema: The acquisition of English questions. Cognitive Linguistics 11, 83–102.Google Scholar
Daniels, Peter T., & Bright, William (eds.). 1996. The world's writing systems. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Davis, K. 1940. Extreme social isolation of a child. American Journal of Sociology 45, 554–565.Google Scholar
Davis, K. 1947. Final note on a case of extreme social isolation. American Journal of Sociology 52, 432–437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deák, Gedeon O., & Maratsos, Michael P. 1998. On having complex representations of things: Preschoolers use multiple labels for objects and people. Developmental Psychology 34, 224–240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boysson-Bardies, Benedicte, & Vihman, Marilyn M. 1991. Adaptation to language: Evidence from babbling and first words in four languages. Language 67, 297–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeCasper, Anthony J., & Fifer, William P. 1980. Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers' voices. Science 208, 1174–1176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeCasper, Anthony J., & Spence, Melanie J. 1986. Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns' perception of speech sounds. Infant Behavior & Development 9, 133–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deffebach, Kim P., & Adamson, Lauren B. 1994. Teaching referential and social-regulative words to toddlers: Mothers' use of metalingual language. First Language 14, 249–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houwer, Annick. 1990. The acquisition of two languages from birth: A case study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houwer, Annick. 1995. Bilingual language acquisition. In Fletcher, P. & MacWhinney, B. (eds.), The handbook of child language (219–250). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Houwer, Annick. 2007. Parental language input patterns and children's bilingual use. Applied Psycholinguistics 28, 411–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houwer, Annick; Bornstein, Marc H.; & Coster, Sandrine. 2006. Early understanding of two words for the same thing: A CDI study of lexical comprehension in infant bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism 10, 331–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemos, Claudia. 1981. Interactional processes in the child's construction of language. In Deutsch, W. (ed.), The child's construction of language (57–76). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
León, Lourdes. 1998. The emergent participant: Interactive patterns in the socialization of Tzotzil (Mayan) infants. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 8 (2), 131–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demuth, Katherine. 1986. Prompting routines in the language socialization of Basotho children. In Schieffelin, B. B. & Ochs, E. (eds.), Language socialization across cultures (51–79). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Demuth, Katherine. 1989. Maturation and the acquisition of the Sesotho passive. Language 65, 56–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demuth, Katherine. 1992. The acquisition of Sesotho. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vol. III (557–638). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Demuth, Katherine. 1994. On the underspecification of functional categories in early grammars. In Lust, B., Suñer, M., & Whitman, J. (eds.), Syntactic theory and first language acquisition: Crosslinguistic perspectives, vol. I (119–134). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Demuth, Katherine. 1996. The prosodic structure of early words. In Morgan, J. & Demuth, K. (eds.), From signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition (171–184). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dennis, Maureen. 1980. Capacity and strategy for syntactic comprehension after left or right hemidecortication. Brain & Language 10, 287–317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, Maureen. 1988. Language and the young damaged brain. In Boll, T. & Bryant, B. K. (eds.), Clinical neuropsychology and brain function: Research, measurement, and practice (89–123). New York: Guilford Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennis, Maureen. 2000. Childhood medical disorders and cognitive impairment: Biological risk, time, development, and reserve. In Yeates, K. O. & Ris, M. D. (eds.), Pediatric neuropsychology: Research, theory, and practice (3–22). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Dennis, Maureen, & Kohn, Bruno. 1975. Comprehension of syntax in infantile hemiplegics after cerebral hemidecortication: Left-hemisphere superiority. Brain & Language 2, 472–482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, Maureen;Lovett, Maureen; & Wiegel-Crump, Carole A. 1981. Written language acquisition after left or right hemidecortication in infancy. Brain & Language 12, 54–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, Maureen, & Whitaker, Harry. (1975). Language acquisition following hemidecortication: Linguistic superiority of left over the right hemisphere. Brain & Language 3, 404–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deuchar, Margaret, & Quay, Suzanne. 2000. Bilingual acquisition: Theoretical implications of a case study. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Villiers, Jill G. 1985. Learning how to use verbs: Lexical coding and the influence of input. Journal of Child Language 12, 587–595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Villiers, Jill G., & Villiers, Peter. 1973. A cross-sectional study of the acquisition of grammatical morphemes. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 2, 267–278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Villiers, Peter A., & Villiers, Jill G. 1979. Form and function in the development of sentence negation. Papers & Reports on Child Language Development [Stanford University] 17, 57–64.Google Scholar
Diesendruck, Gil, & Markson, Lori. 2001. Children's avoidance of lexical overlap: A pragmatic account. Developmental Psychology 27, 630–641.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diessel, Holger, & Tomasello, Michael. 2000. Why complement clauses do not include a that-complementizer in early child language. In Chang, S. S., Liaw, L., & Ruppenhofer, J. (eds.), Proceedings of the 25th annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (86–97). Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Dockrell, Julie. 1981. The child's acquisition of unfamiliar words: An experimental study. PhD dissertation, University of Stirling, United Kingdom.
Dockrell, Julie, & Campbell, Robin N. 1986. Lexical acquisition strategies in the preschool child. In Kuczaj II, S. A. & Barrett, M. D. (eds.), The development of word meaning: Progress in cognitive development research (121–154). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dockrell, Julie, & McShane, John. 1990. Young children's use of phrase structure and inflectional information in form-class assignments of novel nouns and verbs. First Language 10, 127–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dockrell, Julie, & Ralli, Mina. 1996. Strategies in lexical acquisition: Constraint or context? Poster, Developmental Section, meeting of the British Psychological Society, Oxford.
Dodd, Barbara. 1975. Children's understanding of their own phonological forms. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 27, 165–172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'Odorico, Laura, & Carrubi, Stefania. 2003. Prosodic characteristics of early multi-word utterances in Italian children. First Language 23, 97–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donaldson, Margaret, & Balfour, George. 1968. Less is more: A study of language comprehension in children. British Journal of Psychology 59, 461–471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donaldson, Margaret, & Wales, Roger J. 1970. On the acquisition of some relational terms. In Hayes, J. R. (ed.), Cognition and the development of language (235–268). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Donaldson, Morag L. 1986. Children's explanations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Döpke, Susanne. 1992. One parent one language: An interactional approach. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dor, Daniel. 1996. Representations, attitudes, and factivity evaluations. PhD dissertation, Stanford University.
Dowty, David. 1991. Thematic proto-roles and argument selection. Language 67, 547–619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dromi, Esther. 1987. Early lexical development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Drozd, Kenneth F. 1995. Child English pre-sentential negation as metalinguistic exclamatory sentence negation. Journal of Child Language 22, 583–610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Du Bois, John W. 1987. The discourse basis of ergativity. Language 63, 805–855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dumont, Robert V. 1972. Learning English and how to be silent: Studies in Sioux and Cherokee classrooms. In Cazden, C., John, V., & Hymes, D. (eds.), Functions of language in the classroom (344–369). New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
Dunn, Judy, & Shatz, Marilyn. 1989. Becoming a conversationalist despite (or because of) having an older sibling. Child Development 60, 399–410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ebeling, Karen S., & Gelman, Susan A. 1994. Children's use of context in interpreting “big” and “little.”Child Development 65, 1178–1192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ebeling, Karen S., & Gelman, Susan A. 1997. The influence of shape and representational status on children's naming. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Washington, DC.
Echols, Catharine H. 1993. A perceptually-based model of children's earliest productions. Cognition 46, 245–296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Echols, Catharine; Crowhurst, Megan J.; & Childers, Jane B. 1997. The perception of rhythmic units in speech by infants and adults. Journal of Memory & Language 36, 202–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Echols, Catharine H., & Newport, Elissa L. 1992. The role of stress and position in determining first words. Language Acquisition 2, 189–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckert, Penelope. 2000. Linguistic variation as social practice. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Eckert, Penelope, & McConnell-Ginet, Sally. 1992. Think practically and look locally: Language and gender as community-based practice. Annual Review of Anthropology 21, 461–490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, Derek, & Goodwin, Roger. 1986. Action words and pragmatic functions in early language. In Kuczaj II, S. A. & Barrett, M. D. (eds.), The development of word meaning: Progress in cognitive development research (257–273). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, Jon; Beckman, Mary E.; & Munson, Benjamin. 2004. The interaction between vocabulary size and phonotactic probability effects on children's production accuracy and fluency in nonword repetition. Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research 47, 421–436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, Mary Louise. 1974. Perception and production in child phonology: The testing of four hypotheses. Journal of Child Language 1, 205–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egger, Emile. 1887. Observations et réflexions sur le développement de l'intelligence et du langage chez les enfants. Paris: Picard.Google Scholar
Eimas, Peter D. 1974. Auditory and linguistic processing of cues for place of articulation by infants. Perception & Psychophysics 16, 513–521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eimas, Peter D.; Siqueland, Einar R.; Jusczyk, Peter W.; & Vigorito, James. 1971. Speech perception in infants. Science 209, 1140–1141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, Ann R., & Garvey, Catherine. 1981. Children's use of verbal strategies in resolving conflicts. Discourse Processes 4, 149–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elbers, Loekie. 1982. Operating principles of repetitive babbling: A cognitive continuity approach. Cognition 12, 45–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elbers, Loekie, & Ton, Josi. 1985. Play pen monologues: The interplay of words and babbles in the first words period. Journal of Child Language 12, 551–565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elbers, Loekie, & Wijnen, Frank. 1992. Effort, production skill, and language learning. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (eds.), Phonological development: Models, research, implications (337–368). Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Elkonin, D. B. 1971. Development of speech. In Zaporozhets, A. V. & Elkonin, D. B. (eds.), The psychology of pre-school children (111–185). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Elman, Jeffrey L. 1993. Learning and development in neural networks: The importance of starting small. Cognition 48, 71–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elman, Jeffrey L.; Bates, Elizabeth A.; Johnson, Mark H.; Karmiloff-Smith, Annette; Parisi, Domenico; & Plunkett, Kim. 1996. Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Elsen, Hilke. 1994. Phonological constraints and overextensions. First Language 14, 305–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elsen, Hilke. 1997. The acquisition of past participles: One or two mechanisms? In Tabri, A., Orfmann, A., & Paredi, T. (eds.), Models of inflection (134–151). Tübingen: Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Engel, Stephen A.; Rumelhart, David E.; Wandell, Brian A.; Lee, Adrian T.; Glover, Gary H.; Chichilnisky, Eduardo-José; & Shadlen, Michael N. 1994. FMRI of human visual cortex. Nature 369, 525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erbaugh, Mary S. 1992. The acquisition of Mandarin. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vol. III (373–455). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ervin, Susan. 1964. Imitation and structural change in children's language. In Lenneberg, E. H. (ed.), New directions in the study of language (163–189). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, Susan. 1970. Discourse agreement: How children answer questions. In Hayes, J. R. (ed.), Cognition and the development of language (79–107). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, Susan. 1973. Some strategies for the first two years. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (261–286). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, Susan. 1974. The comprehension and production of requests by children. Papers & Reports on Child Language Development [Stanford University] 8, 188–195.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, Susan. 1979. Children's verbal turn-taking. In Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. B. (eds.), Developmental pragmatics (391–414). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, Susan; Guo, Jiansheng; & Lampert, Martin. 1990. Politeness and persuasion in children's control acts. Journal of Pragmatics 14, 307–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Estes, Katharine G.; Evans, Julia L.; Alibabli, Martha W.; & Saffran, Jenny R. 2007. Can infants map meaning to newly segmented words? Psychological Science 18, 254–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Estigarribia, Bruno. 2007. Asking questions: Language variation and language acquisition. PhD dissertation, Stanford University.
Estigarribia, Bruno, & Clark, Eve V. 2007. Getting and maintaining attention in talk to young children. Journal of Child Language 34, 799–814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, Gary W.; Maxwell, Lorraine E.; & Hart, Betty. 1999. Parental language and verbal responsiveness to children in crowded homes. Developmental Psychology 35, 1020–1023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabbro, Franco. 2001. The bilingual brain: Cerebral representation of languages. Brain & Language 79, 211–222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fantini, Alvino E. 1985. Language acquisition of a bilingual child: A sociolinguistic perspective. San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.Google Scholar
Felix, Sascha. 1988. Universal Grammar in language acquisition. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 33, 367–393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, Larry; Dale, Philip S.; Reznick, J. Steven; Bates, Elizabeth; Thal, Donna J.; & Pethick, Steven J. 1994. Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59 (serial no. 242).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferguson, Charles A. 1977. Baby talk as a simplified register. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (209–235). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ferguson, Charles A. 1978. Learning to pronounce: The earliest stages of phonological development in the child. In Minifie, F. D. & Lloyd, L. L. (eds.), Communicative and cognitive abilities: Early behavioral assessment (273–297). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Ferguson, Charles A. 1994. Dialect, register, and genre: Working assumptions about conventionalization. In Biber, D. & Finegan, E. (eds.), Sociolinguistic perspectives on register (15–30). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ferguson, Charles A., & Farwell, Carol B. 1975. Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language 51, 419–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, Charles A.; Peizer, David B.; & Weeks, Thelma E. 1973. Model-and-replica phonological grammar of a child's first words. Lingua 31, 35–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, Charles A., & Slobin, Dan I. (eds.). 1973. Studies of child language development. New York: Holt Rinehart & Winston.
Fernald, Anne. 1985. Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behavior & Development 8, 181–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, Anne. 1989. Intonation and communicative intent in mothers' speech to infants: Is the melody the message? Child Development 60, 1497–1510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernald, Anne. 1992. Meaningful melodies in mothers' speech to infants. In Papousek, H., Jürgens, U., & Papousek, M. (eds.), Nonverbal communication: Comparative and developmental approaches (262–282). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fernald, Anne, & Hurtado, Nereyda. 2006. Names in frames: infants interpret words in sentence frames faster than words in isolation. Developmental Science 9, F33–F40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernald, Anne, & Kuhl, Patricia K. 1987. Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech. Infant Behavior & Development 10, 279–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, Anne, & Mazzie, Claudia. 1991. Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Developmental Psychology 27, 209–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, Anne; Perfors, Amy; & Marchman, Virginia A. 2006. Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology 42, 98–116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernald, Anne, & Simon, Thomas. 1984. Expanded intonation contours in mothers' speech to newborns. Developmental Psychology 20, 104–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, Anne; Taeschner, Traute; Dunn, Judy; Papousek, Mechthild; Boysson-Bardies, Benedicte; & Fukui, Ikuko. 1989. A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers' and fathers' speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language 16, 477–501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferreiro, Emilia. 1971. Les relations temporelles dans le langage de l'enfant. Geneva: Droz.Google Scholar
Ferrier, Linda J. 1978. Some observations of error in context. In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (eds.), The development of communication (301–309). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Figueira, Rosa Attié. 1984. On the development of the expression of causativity: A syntactic hypothesis. Journal of Child Language 11, 109–127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fikkert, Paula. 1994. On the acquisition of prosodic structure. Leiden: Holland Institute of Linguistics.Google Scholar
Fillmore, Charles J. 1968. The case for case. In Bach, E. & Harms, R. T. (eds.), Universals of linguistic theory (1–90). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Fillmore, Charles J. 1979. On fluency. In Fillmore, C. J., Kempler, D., & Wang, W. S.-Y. (eds.), Individual differences in language ability and language behavior (85–101). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, Cynthia. 1996. Structural limits on verb mapping: The role of analogy in children's interpretations of sentences. Cognitive Psychology 31, 41–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, Cynthia; Klingler, Stacy L.; & Song, Hyun-joo. 2006. What does syntax say about space? 2-year-olds use sentence structure to learn new prepositions. Cognition 101, B19–B29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, Cynthia, & Tokura, Hisayo. 1995. The given-new contract in speech to infants. Journal of Memory & Language 34, 287–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fishman, Joshua A. 1971. Sociolinguistic perspective on the study of bilingualism. In Fishman, J. A., Cooper, R. L., & Ma, R. (eds.), Bilingualism in the barrio (3–10). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Fletcher, Paul. 1985. A child's learning of English. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Flynn, Valerie, & Masur, Elise F. 2007. Characteristics of maternal verbal style: Responsiveness and directiveness in two natural contexts. Journal of Child Language 34, 519–543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, Ivan. 1972. A propos de la génèse de la phrase enfantine. Lingua 30, 31–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forrester, Michael A. 1988. Young children's polyadic conversation monitoring skills. First Language 7, 145–158.Google Scholar
Foulkes, Paul; Docherty, Gerry J.; & Watt, Dominic. 2005. Phonological variation in child-directed speech. Language 81, 177–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fourcin, Adrian J. 1975. Speech perception in the absence of speech productive ability. In O'Connor, N. (ed.), Language, cognitive deficits, and retardation (33–43). London: Butterworth.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, Carol A. 1988. Differential shortening of repeated content words produced in various communicative contexts. Language & Speech 31, 307–319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fowler, Carol A., & Housum, Jonathan. 1987. Talkers' signaling of “new” and “old” words in speech and listeners' perception and use of the distinction. Journal of Memory & Language 26, 489–504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franco, Fabia, & Butterworth, George. 1996. Pointing and social awareness: Declaring and requesting in the second year. Journal of Child Language 23, 307–336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, Lucia A., & Nelson, Katherine. 1981. Temporal knowledge expressed in preschoolers' descriptions of familiar activities. Papers & Reports on Child Language Development [Stanford University] 20, 61–69.Google Scholar
Friederici, Angelika D., & Wessels, Jeanine M. I. 1993. Phonotactic knowledge of word boundaries and its use in infant speech-perception. Perception & Psychophysics 54, 287–295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedrich, Manuela, & Friederici, Angela D. 2005. Phonotactic knowledge and lexical–semantic processing in one-year-olds: Brain responses to words and nonsense words in picture contexts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17, 1785–1802.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fromkin, Victoria. 1971. The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Language 47, 27–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galantucci, Bruno. 2005. An experimental study of the emergence of human communication systems. Cognitive Science 29, 737–767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galati, Alexia, & Brennan, Susan E. (2006). Given-new attenuation effects in spoken discourse: For the speaker, or for the addressee? Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society, 47th annual meeting, Dallas, TX.
Gallagher, Tanya M. 1977. Revision behaviors in the speech of normal children developing language. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research 20, 303–318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ganea, Patricia A., & Saylor, Megan M. 2007. Infants' use of shared linguistic information to clarify ambiguous requests. Child Development 78, 493–502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garnica, Olga K. 1973. The development of phonemic speech perception. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (215–222). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garnica, Olga K. 1977. Some prosodic and paralinguistic features of speech to children. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (63–88). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Garrett, Merrill F. 1975. The analysis of sentence production. In Bower, G. H. (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, vol. IX (133–177). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Garvey, Catherine. 1975. Requests and responses in children's speech. Journal of Child Language 2, 41–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garvey, Catherine. 1977. Play with language. In Tizard, B. & Harvey, D. (eds.), Biology of play (74–99). London: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Garvey, Catherine. 1979. Contingent queries and their relations in discourse. In Ochs, E. & Schiefflin, B. B. (eds.), Developmental pragmatics (363–372). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Garvey, Catherine. 1984. Children's talk. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gathercole, Virginia C. Mueller; Sebastián, Eugenia; & Soto, Pilar. 1999. The early acquisition of Spanish verbal morphology: Across-the-board or piecemeal knowledge?International Journal of Bilingualism 3, 133–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, Susan A., & Coley, John D. 1990. The importance of knowing a dodo is a bird: Categories and inferences in two-year-olds. Developmental Psychology 26, 796–804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, Susan A.; Coley, John D.; Rosengren, Karl S.; Hartman, Erin; & Pappas, Athina. 1998a. Beyond labeling: The role of maternal input in the acquisition of richly structured categories. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 63 (serial no. 253).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, Susan A.; Croft, William; Fu, Panfang; Clausner, Timothy C.; & Gottfried, Gail. 1998b. Why is a pomegranate an apple? The role of shape, taxonomic relatedness, and prior lexical knowledge in children's overextensions. Journal of Child Language 25, 267–291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, Susan A., & Eveling, Karen S. 1998. Shape and representational status in children's early naming. Cognition 66, B35–B47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, Susan A., & Tardif, Twila. 1998. Acquisition of nouns and verbs in Mandarin and English. In Clark, E. V. (ed.), Proceedings of the 29th annual Child Language Research Forum 1997 (27–36). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Gelman, Susan A., & Taylor, Marjorie. 1984. How two-year-old children interpret proper and common names for unfamiliar objects. Child Development 55, 1535–1540.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genesee, Fred. 1989. Early bilingual development: One language or two? Journal of Child Language 16, 161–179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genesee, Fred. 2006. Bilingual first language acquisition in perspective. In McCardle, P. & Hoff, E. (eds.), Childhood bilingualism (45–67). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Genesee, Fred; Boivin, Isabelle; & Nicoladis, Elena. 1996. Talking with strangers: A study of bilingual children's communicative competence. Applied Psycholinguistics 17, 427–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Genesee, Fred; Nicoladis, Elena; & Paradis, Johanne. 1995. Language differentiation in early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language 22, 611–631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genesee, Fred; Tucker, G. Richard; & Lambert, Wallace E. 1976. Communication skills of bilingual children. Child Development 46, 1010–1014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, Dedre. 1982. Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In Kuczaj, S. A. (ed.), Language development, vol. II: Language, thought, and culture (301–334). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gentner, Dedre, & Boroditsky, Lera. 2001. Individuation, relativity and early word learning. In Bowerman, M. & Levinson, S. C. (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (215–256). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, Dedre, & Medina, José. 1998. Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition 65, 263–297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gergely, György; Bekkering, Harold; & Király, Ildikó. 2002. Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature 415, 755 (14 February 2002).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gergely, György, & Csibra, Gergely. 2003. Teleological reasoning in infancy: The naïve theory of rational action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7, 287–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerken, LouAnn. 1991. The metrical basis of children's subjectless sentences. Journal of Memory & Language 30, 431–451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerken, LouAnn. 1994a. A metrical template account of children's weak syllable omissions from multisyllabic words. Journal of Child Language 21, 565–584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerken, LouAnn. 1994b. Young children's representations of prosodic phonology: Evidence from English-speakers' weak syllable productions. Journal of Memory & Language 33, 19–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerken, LouAnn. 1996. Prosodic structure in young children's language production. Language 72, 683–712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerken, LouAnn; Landau, Barbara; & Remez, Robert E. 1990. Function morphemes in young children's speech perception and production. Developmental Psychology 26, 204–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerken, LouAnn, & McIntosh, Bonnie J. 1993. The interplay of function morphemes and prosody in early language. Developmental Psychology 29, 448–457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geschwind, Norman, & Levitsky, Walter. 1968. Human brain: Left-right asymmetries in temporal speech region. Science 161, 186–187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gesell, Arnold, & Thompson, Helen. 1934. Infant behavior: Its genesis and growth. New York: McGraw-Hill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giles, Howard; Coupland, Nikolas; & Coupland, Justine (eds.). 1991. Contexts of accommodation: Developments in applied sociolinguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Givón, Talmy. 1979. Understanding grammar. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gleitman, Lila R. 1981. Maturational determinants of language growth. Cognition 10, 103–114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gleitman, Lila R. 1990. The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition 1, 3–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goffman, Lisa; Schwartz, Richard G.; & Marton, Klara. 1996. Information level and young children's phonological accuracy. Journal of Child Language 23, 337–347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gogate, Lakshmi J.; Bahrick, Lorraine E.; & Watson, Jilayne D. 2000. A study of multimodal motherese: The role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. Child Development 71, 878–894.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, Adele E. 1995. Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, Adele E. 1999. The emergence of the semantics of argument structure constructions. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), The emergence of language (197–212). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goldfield, Beverly A., & Reznick, J. Steven. 1990. Early lexical acquisition: Rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt. Journal of Child Language 17, 171–183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldfield, Beverly A., & Reznick, J. Steven. 1992. Rapid change in lexical development in comprehension and production. Developmental Psychology 28, 406–413.Google Scholar
Goldfield, Beverly A., & Reznick, J. Steven. 1996. Measuring the vocabulary spurt: A reply to Mervis & Bertrand. Journal of Child Language 23, 241–246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, Susan; Goodrich, Whitney; Sauer, Eve; & Iverson, Jana. 2007. Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Developmental Science 10, 778–785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, Susan; Seligman, Martin E. P.; & Gelman, Rochel. 1976. Language in the two-year-old. Cognition 4, 189–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldwater, Sharon; Griffiths, Thomas L.; & Johnson, Mark. 2006. Contextual dependencies in unsupervised word segmentation. Proceedings of the 21st International Conference on Computational Linguistics and the 44th annual meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (673–680), Sydney, Australia.
Golinkoff, Roberta M. 1983. The preverbal negotiation of failed messages: Insights into the transition period. In Golinkoff, R. M. (ed.), The transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication (57–78). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, Roberta M. 1986. “I beg your pardon?”: The preverbal negotiation of failed messages. Journal of Child Language 13, 455–476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golinkoff, Roberta M.; Mervis, Carolyn B.; & Hirsh-Pasek, Kathryn. 1994. Early object labels: A case for a developmental lexical principles framework. Journal of Child Language 21, 125–155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, Judith C.; McDonough, Laraine; & Brown, Natasha B. 1998. The role of semantic context and memory in the acquisition of novel nouns. Child Development 69, 1330–1344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, Marjorie H. 1990. He-said-she-said: Talk as social organization among black children. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Goodwin, Marjorie H. 1993. Tactical uses of stories: Participation frameworks within boys' and girls' disputes. In Tannen, D. (ed.), Gender and conversational interaction (110–143). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goodwyn, Susan W., & Acredolo, Linda P. 1993. Symbolic gesture vs. word: Is there a modality advantage for onset of symbol use? Child Development 55, 903–910.Google Scholar
Gordon, Raymond G. (ed.). 2005. Ethnologue: Languages of the world (15th edn). Dallas, TX: SIL International.
Grassmann, Susanne, & Tomasello, Michael. 2007. Two-year-olds use primary sentence accent to learn new words. Journal of Child Language 34, 677–687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenberg, Joseph H. (ed.). 1963. Universals of language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Greenberg, Joseph H. 1966. Language universals. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Greenfield, Patricia M. 1979. Informativeness, presupposition, and semantic choice in single-word utterances. In Ochs, E. & Schiefflin, B. B. (eds.), Developmental pragmatics (159–166). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Greenfield, Patricia M., & Smith, Joshua. 1976. The structure of communication in early language development. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Grégoire, Antoine. 1939, 1947. L'apprentissage du langage, 2 vols. Liège: Librairie Droz.Google Scholar
Grevisse, Maurice. 1964. Le bon usage: Grammaire française avec des remarques sur la langue française d'aujourd'hui (8th edn). Paris: Hatier.Google Scholar
Grice, H. Paul. 1989. Studies in the way of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Grieser, DiAnne L., & Kuhl, Patricia K. 1988. Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features. Developmental Psychology 24, 14–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grieve, Robert; Hoogenraad, Robert; & Murray, Diarmid. 1977. On the young child's use of lexis and syntax in understanding locative instructions. Cognition 5, 235–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, Patrick D., & Atkinson, Martin. 1978. A ‘door’ to verbs. In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (eds.), The development of communication (311–319). London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Griffiths, Thomas L., & Kalish, Michael L. 2007. Language evolution by iterated learning with Bayesian agents. Cognitive Science 31, 441–480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grimm, Hannelore. 1973. Strukturanalytische Untersuchung der Kindersprache. Bern: Verlag Huber.Google Scholar
Grimm, Hannelore. 1975. Analysis of short-term dialogues in 5–7 year olds: Encoding of intentions and modifications of speech acts as a function of negative feedback. Paper presented at the Third International Child Language Symposium, London.
Grimm, Hannelore, & Schöler, Hans. 1975. Erlauben–befehlen–lassen: Wie gut verstehen kleine Kindere kausativierende Beziehungen? In Grimm, H., Schöler, H., & Wintermentel, M., Zur Entwicklung sprachlicher Strukturformen bein Kindern (100–120). Weinheim: Beltz.Google Scholar
Grimshaw, Jane. 1981. Form, function, and the language acquisition device. In Baker, C. L. & McCarthy, J. J. (eds.), The logical problem of language acquisition (165–182). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gropen, Jess; Pinker, Steven; Hollander, Michelle; & Goldberg, Richard. 1991. Syntax and semantics in the acquisition of locative verbs. Journal of Child Language 18, 115–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grosjean, François. 1982. Life with two languages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Grosjean, François. 1989. Neurolinguists, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person. Brain & Language 36, 3–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, Maurice. 1975. On the relations between syntax and semantics. In Keenan, E. L. (ed.), Formal semantics of natural language (389–405). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gruber, Jeffrey S. 1973. Correlations between the syntactic constructions of the child and the adult. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds.), Studies of child language development (440–445). New York: Holt Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Guerriero, A. M. Sonia; Oshima-Takane, Yuriko; & Kuriyama, Yoko. 2006. The development of referential choice in English and Japanese: A discourse-pragmatic perspective. Journal of Child Language 33, 823–857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guidetti, Michèle. 2002. The emergence of pragmatics: Forms and functions of conventional gestures in young French children. First Language 22, 265–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guillaume, Paul. 1927. Le développement des éléments formels dans le langage de l'enfant. Journal de Psychologie 24, 203–229.Google Scholar
Gumperz, John J. 1982. Discourse strategies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gvozdev, Alexandr N. 1961. Voprosy izucheniya detskoy rechi [The development of language in children]. Moscow: Akademii Pedagogicheskikh Nauk RSFSR.Google Scholar
Haiman, John (ed.). 1985. Iconicity in syntax. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRef
Hakuta, Kenji. 1981. Grammatical description versus configurational arrangement in language acquisition – The case of relative clauses in Japanese. Cognition 9, 197–236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hakuta, Kenji. 1986. Mirror of language: The debate on bilingualism. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hakuta, Kenji, & D'Andrea, Daniel. 1992. Some properties of bilingual maintenance and loss in Mexican background high-school students. Applied Psycholinguistics 13, 72–99.Google Scholar
Hall, D. Geoffrey, & Graham, Susan A. 1999. Lexical form class information guides word-to-object mapping in preschoolers. Child Development 70, 78–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, D. Geoffrey; Quantz, Darryl H.; & Persoage, Kelley A. 2000. Preschoolers' use of form class cues in word learning. Developmental Psychology 36, 449–462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halle, Tamara, & Shatz, Marilyn. 1994. Mothers' social regulatory language to young children in family settings. First Language 14, 83–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, Michael A. K. 1975. Learning how to mean – Explorations in the development of language. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Hare, Mary, & Elman, Jeffrey L. 1995. Learning and morphological change. Cognition 56, 61–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, Catherine L., & Shirai, Yasuhiro. 1997. Selecting past-tense forms for new words: What's meaning got to do with it? In Proceedings of the 19th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (295–300). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Harris, Margaret; Barlow-Brown, Fiona; & Chasin, Joan. 1995. The emergence of referential understanding: Pointing and the comprehension of object names. First Language 15, 19–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, Margaret; Jones, David; & Grant, Julia. 1984/1985. The social-interactional context of maternal speech to infants: An explanation for the event-bound nature of early word use? First Language 5, 89–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, Margaret; Yeeles, Caroline; Chasin, Joan; & Oakley, Yvonne. 1995. Symmetries and asymmetries in early lexical comprehension and production. Journal of Child Language 22, 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, Paul L.; German, Tim; & Mills, Patrick. 1996. Children's use of counterfactual thinking in causal reasoning. Cognition 61, 233–259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, Zellig S. 1955. From phoneme to morpheme. Language 31, 190–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, Betty, & Risley, Todd R. 1992. American parenting of language-learning children: Persisting differences in family-child interactions observed in natural home environments. Developmental Psychology 28, 1096–1105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, Betty, & Risley, Todd R. 1995. Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: H. Paul Brookes.Google Scholar
Haryu, Etsuko, & Imai, Mutsumi. 2002. Reorganizing the lexicon by learning a new word: Japanese children's interpretations of the meaning of a new word for a familiar artifact. Child Development 73, 1378–1391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haviland, Susan E., & Clark, Eve V. 1974. “This man's father is my father's son”: A study of the acquisition of English kin terms. Journal of Child Language 1, 23–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, John A. (ed.). 1988. Explaining language universals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hayes, J. Richard, & Clark, Herbert H. 1970. Experiments on the segmentation of an artificial speech analogue. In Hayes, J. R. (ed.), Cognition and the development of language (221–234). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Heath, Shirley Brice. 1983. Ways with words. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hecht, Barbara F. 1983. Situations and language: Children's use of plural allomorphs in familiar and unfamiliar settings. PhD dissertation, Stanford University.
Heibeck, Tracy H., & Markman, Ellen M. 1987. Word learning in children: An examination of fast mapping. Child Development 58, 1021–1034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hemsley, Gayle; Holm, Alison; & Dodd, Barbara. 2006. Diverse but not different: The lexical skills of two primary age bilingual groups in comparison to monolingual peers. International Journal of Bilingualism 10, 453–476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickmann, Maya. 1991. The development of discourse cohesion: Some functional and cross-linguistic issues. In Piéraut-Le-Bonniec, G. & Dolitsky, M. (eds.), Language bases, discourse bases: Some aspects of contemporary French-language psycholinguistics research (157–185). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickmann, Maya, & Hendriks, Henriëtte. 1999. Cohesion and anaphora in children's narratives: A comparison of English, French, German, and Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Child Language 26, 419–452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirsh-Pasek, Kathryn; Kemler Nelson, Deborah G.; Jusczyk, Peter W.; Cassidy, Kimberly W.; Druss, Benjamin; & Kennedy, Lori. 1987. Clauses are perceptual units for young infants. Cognition 26, 269–286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirsh-Pasek, Kathryn; Treiman, Rebecca; & Schneiderman, Maita. 1984. Brown & Hanlon revisited: Mothers' sensitivity to ungrammatical forms. Journal of Child Language 11, 81–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hochberg, Judith G. 1986. Children's judgments of transitivity errors. Journal of Child Language 13, 317–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, Erika. 2006. How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review 26, 55–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, Erika. 1991. Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development 62, 782–796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff-Ginsberg, Erika. 1998. The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children's language experience and language development. Applied Psycholinguistics 19, 603–629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, Erika, & Tardif, Twila. 1995. Socioeconomic status and parenting. In Bornstein, M. H. (ed.), Handbook of parenting, vol. II: Ecology and biology of parenting (161–188). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hoffman, Eva. 1989. Lost in translation: A life in a new language. Harmondsworth, Middlesex: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Hollich, George; Jusczyk, Peter W.; & Luce, Paul A. 2001. Lexical neighborhood effects on 17-month-old word learning. Proceedings of the 26th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (314–323). Boston, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Hollos, Marida, & Beeman, William. 1978. The development of directives among Norwegian and Hungarian children: An example of communicative style in culture. Language in Society 7, 345–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holowka, Siobhan; Brosseau-Lapré, Françoise; & Petitto, Laura. 2002. Semantic and conceptual knowledge underlying bilingual babies' first signs and words. Language Learning 52, 205–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hood, Bruce M.; Willen, J. Douglas; & Driver, Jon. 1998. Adults' eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science 9, 131–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hood, Lois, & Bloom, Lois. 1979. What, when, and how about why: A longitudinal study of early expressions of causality. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 44 (serial no. 181).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopper, Paul J., & Thompson, Sandra A. 1980. Transitivity in grammar and discourse. Language 56, 251–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horgan, Dianne. 1978. The development of the full passive. Journal of Child Language 5, 65–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horn, Laurence R. 1988. Morphology, pragmatics, and the un- verb. In Powers, J. & Jong, K. (eds.), Proceedings of the 5th Eastern States Conference on Linguistics. Columbus: Department of Linguistics, Ohio State University.Google Scholar
Hornby, Peter A. 1971. Surface structure and the topic-comment distinction: A developmental study. Child Development 42, 1975–1988.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hornby, Peter A., & Hass, Wilbur A. 1970. Use of contrastive stress by preschool children. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research 13, 395–399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hornstein, Norbert, & Lightfoot, David. 1981. Introduction. In Explanation in linguistics: The logical problem of language acquisition (9–31). London: Longman.Google Scholar
Horowitz, Frances D. (ed.). 1974. Visual attention, auditory stimulation, and language discrimination in young infants. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 39 (serial no. 158).
Hubel, David, & Wiesel, Torsten. 1970. The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens. Journal of Physiology 206, 419–436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hulk, Aafke, & Müller, Natascha. 2000. Bilingual first language acquisition at the interface between syntax and pragmatics. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition 3, 227–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, Janellen; Haight, Wendy; Bryk, Anthony; Seltzer, Michael; & Lyons, Thomas. 1991. Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology 17, 236–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, Janellen, & Smiley, Patricia. 1987. Early word meanings: The case of object names. Cognitive Psychology 19, 63–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, Janellen; Smiley, Patricia; & Charney, Rosalind. 1983. Emergence of action categories in the child: Evidence from verb meanings. Psychological Review 90, 72–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyams, Nina. 1986. Language acquisition and the theory of parameters. Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imai, Mutsumi; Haryu, Etsuko; & Okada, Hiroyuki. 2005. Mapping novel nouns and verbs onto dynamic action events: Are verb meanings easier to learn than noun meanings for Japanese children? Child Development 76, 340–355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ingham, Richard. 1992. The optional subject phenomenon in young children's English: A case study. Journal of Child Language 19, 133–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ingham, Richard. 1998. Tense without agreement in early clause structure. Language Acquisition 7, 51–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingram, David. 1971. Transitivity in child language. Language 47, 888–910.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingram, David. 1974. Phonological rules in young children. Journal of Child Language 1, 49–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Itard, Jean M. G. 1801. De l'education d'un homme sauvage ou des premiers développements physiques et moraux du jeune sauvage d'Aveyron. Paris: Gouyon. (Translated into English: G. Humphrey & M. Humphrey. 1932. The wild boy of Aveyron. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.)Google Scholar
Iverson, Jana M. 1999. How to get to the cafeteria: Gesture and speech in blind and sighted children's spatial description. Developmental Psychology 35, 1132–1142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, Jana M.; Capirci, Olga; & Caselli, M. Cristina. 1994. From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development 9, 23–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, Jana M.; Capirci, Olga; Longobardi, Emiddia; & Caselli, M. Cristina. 1999. Gesturing in mother-child interactions. Cognitive Development 14, 57–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, Jana M., & Goldin-Meadow, Susan. 2005. Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science 16, 368–371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackendoff, Ray S. 1983. Semantics and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jakobson, Roman. 1968. Child language, aphasia, and phonological universals. The Hague: Mouton. (Originally published in German, 1941.)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, Sharon L. 1978. Effect of listeners' age and situation on the politeness of children's directives. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 4, 307–317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaswal, Vikram K., & Malone, Lauren S. 2007. Turning believers into skeptics: 3-year-olds' sensitivity to cues to speaker credibility. Journal of Cognition & Development 8, 263–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jefferson, Gail. 1982. On exposed and embedded correction in conversation. Studium Linguisticum 14, 58–68.Google Scholar
Jespersen, Otto. 1922. Language: Its nature, development, and origin. London: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Jiang, Yang; Haxby, James V.; Martin, Alex; Ungerleider, Leslie G.; & Parasuraman, Raja. 2000. Complementary neural mechanisms for tracking items in human working memory. Science 287, 643–646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jisa, Harriet. 2000. Increasing cohesion in narratives: A developmental study of maintaining and reintroducing subjects in French. Linguistics 38, 591–620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, Carolyn E. 1981. Children's questions and the discovery of interrogative syntax. PhD dissertation, Stanford University.
Johnson, Carolyn E. 1983. The development of children's interrogatives: From formulas to rules. Papers & Reports on Child Language Development [Stanford University] 22, 108–115.
Johnson, Carolyn E. 2000. What you see is what you get: The importance of transcription for interpreting children's morphosyntactic development. In Menn, L. & Ratner, N. Bernstein (eds.), Methods for studying language production (181–204). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Johnson, Helen L. 1975. The meaning of before and after for preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 19, 88–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, Jacqueline S., & Newport, Elissa. 1989. Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology 21, 60–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, Jacqueline S., & Newport, Elissa. 1991. Critical period effects on universal properties of language: The status of subjacency on the acquisition of a second language. Cognition 39, 215–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, Susan C., & Carey, Susan. 1998. Knowledge enrichment and conceptual change in folkbiology: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognitive Psychology 37, 156–200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, Judith R., & Slobin, Dan I. 1979. The development of locative expressions in English, Italian, Serbo-Croatian and Turkish. Journal of Child Language 6, 529–545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jörg, Sabine, & Hörmann, Hans. 1978. The influence of general and specific verbal labels on the recognition of labeled and unlabeled parts of pictures. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 17, 445–454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Junker, Dörte A., & Stockman, Ida J. 2002. Expressive vocabulary of German-English bilingual toddlers. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 11, 381–394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, Peter W. 1992. Developing phonological categories from the speech signal. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (eds.), Phonological development: Models, research, implications (17–64). Parkton, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, Peter W. 1997. The discovery of spoken language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, Peter W., & Aslin, Richard N. 1995. Infants' detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology 29, 1–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jusczyk, Peter W.; Cutler, Anne; & Redanz, Nancy J. 1993. Infants' preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. Child Development 64, 675–687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jusczyk, Peter W.; Friederici, Angelika D.; Wessels, Jeanine M. I.; Svenkerud, Vigdis Y.; & Jusczyk, Ann M. 1993. Infants' sensitivity to segmental and prosodic characteristics of words in their native language. Journal of Memory & Language 32, 402–420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaper, Willem. 1980. The use of the past tense in games of pretend. Journal of Child Language 7, 213–215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, Annette. 1979. A functional approach to child language: A study of determiners and reference. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, Annette. 1981. The grammatical marking of thematic structure in the development of language production. In Deutsch, W. (ed.), The child's construction of language (121–147). New York and London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, Annette. 1998. Is atypical development necessarily a window on the normal mind/brain? The case of Williams syndrome. Developmental Science 1, 273–277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, Annette. 1999. Taking development seriously. Human Development 42, 325–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, Annette; Grant, Julia; Berthoud, Ioanna; Davies, Mark; Howlin, Patricia; & Udwin, Orlee. 1997. Language and Williams syndrome: How intact is “intact”?Child Development 68, 246–262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, Annette; Tyler, Lorraine K.; Voice, J. Kate; Sims, Kerry; Udwin, Orlee; Howlin, Patricia; & Davies, Mark. 1998. Linguistic dissociations in Williams syndrome: Evaluating receptive syntax in on-line and off-line tasks. Neuropsychologia 36, 343–351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Käsermann, Marie-Louise, & Foppa, Klaus. 1981. Some determinants of self-correction: An interactional study of Swiss-German. In Deutsch, W. (ed.), The child's construction of language (77–104). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Katz, Nancy; Baker, Erica; & Macnamara, John. 1974. What's in a name? On the child's acquisition of proper and common names. Child Development 45, 269–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kauschke, Christina; Lee, HaeWook; & Pae, Soyeong. 2007. Similarities and variation in noun and verb acquisition: A crosslinguistic study of children learning German, Korean, and Turkish. Language & Cognitive Processes 22, 1045–1072.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, Elinor O. 1974a. Norm-makers, norm-breakers: Uses of speech by men and women in a Malagasy community. In Baumann, J. & Sherzer, J. (eds.), Explorations in the ethnography of speaking (125–143). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Keenan, Elinor O. 1974b. Conversational competence in children. Journal of Child Language 1, 163–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, Elinor O., & Klein, Ewan. 1975. Coherency in children's discourse. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 4, 365–380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, Margaret, & Stoel-Gammon, Carol. 1997a. The acquisition of prosodic structure: An investigation of current accounts of children's prosodic development. Language 73, 113–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, Margaret, & Stoel-Gammon, Carol. 1997b. Truncation patterns in English-speaking children's word productions. Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research 40, 526–541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller-Cohen, Deborah. 1987. Context and strategy in acquiring temporal connectives. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 16, 165–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessel, Frank S. 1970. The role of syntax in children's comprehension from ages six to twelve. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 35 (serial no. 139).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ketrez, Fatma N., & Aksu-Koç, Ayhan. 2003. Acquisition of noun and verb categories in Turkish. In Özsoy, A. Sumru, Akar, D., Nakipoglu-Demiralp, M., Taylan, E. Erguvanlı, & Aksu-Koç, A. (eds.), Studies in Turkish linguistics (239–246). Istanbul: Bogazici University Press.Google Scholar
Kim, John J.; Marcus, Gary F.; Pinker, Steven; Hollander, Michelle; & Coppola, Marie. 1994. Sensitivity of children's inflection to grammatical structure. Journal of Child Language 21, 173–209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, Mikyong; McGregor, Karla K.; & Thompson, Cynthia K. 2000. Early lexical development in English- and Korean-speaking children: Language-general and language-specific patterns. Journal of Child Language 27, 225–254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiparsky, Paul, & Menn, Lise. 1977. On the acquisition of phonology. In Macnamara, J. (ed.), Language learning and thought (47–78). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kirk, Cecilia, & Demuth, Katherine. 2005. Asymmetries in the acquisition of word-initial and word-final consonant clusters. Journal of Child Language 2, 709–734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirk, Cecilia, & Demuth, Katherine. 2006. Accounting for variability in 2-year-olds' production of coda consonants. Journal of Learning & Development 2, 97–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klatzky, Roberta L.; Clark, Eve V.; & Macken, Marlys M. 1973. Asymmetries in the acquisition of polar adjectives: Linguistic or conceptual?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 16, 32–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klibanoff, Raquel S., & Waxman, Sandra R. 1999. Syntactic cues to word meaning: Initial expectations and the development of flexibility. In Greenhill, A., Littlefield, H., & Tano, C. (eds.), Proceedings of the 23rd annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (361–372). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Klima, Edward S., & Bellugi, Ursula. 1966. Syntactic regularities in the speech of children. In Lyons, J. & Wales, R. J. (eds.), Psycholinguistics papers (183–208). Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh Press.Google Scholar
Kluender, Keith R.; Diehl, Randy L.; & Killeen, Peter R. 1987. Japanese quail can learn phonetic categories. Science 237, 1195–1197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenig, Melissa A.; Clément, Fabrice; & Harris, Paul L. 2004. Trust in testimony: Children's use of true and false statements. Psychological Science 10, 694–698.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koenig, Melissa A., & Echols, Catharine H. 2003. Infants' understanding of false labeling events: The referential roles of words and the speakers who use them. Cognition 87, 179–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koenig, Melissa A., & Harris, Paul L. 2005. Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development 76, 1261–1277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohn, Bruno, & Dennis, Maureen. 1974. Selective impairments of visuo-spatial abilities in infantile hemiplegics after right cerebral hemidecortication. Neuropsychologia 12, 505–512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Köpcke, Klaus-Michael. 1998. The acquisition of plural marking in English and German revisited: Schemata versus rules. Journal of Child Language 25, 293–319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovac, Ceil, & Adamson, H. D. 1981. Variation theory and first language acquisition. In Sankoff, D. & Cedergren, H. (eds.), Variation omnibus (403–410). Edmonton, Alberta: Linguistic Research.Google Scholar
Kroodsma, Donald E., & Pickert, Roberta. 1980. Environmentally dependent sensitive periods for avian vocal learning. Nature 288, 477–479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krott, Andrea, & Nicoladis, Elena. 2005. Large constituent families help children parse compounds. Journal of Child Language 32, 139–158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuczaj, Stan A., II. 1977. The acquisition of regular and irregular past tense forms. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 16, 589–600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuczaj, Stan A.. 1978. Children's judgments of grammatical and ungrammatical irregular past tense verbs. Child Development 49, 319–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuczaj, Stan A.. 1981. More on children's initial failures to relate specific acquisitions. Journal of Child Language 8, 485–487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuczaj, Stan A., II. 1983. Crib speech and language play. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuczaj, Stan A., II, & Daly, Mary J. 1979. The development of hypothetical reference in the speech of young children. Journal of Child Language 6, 563–579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuczaj, Stan A., II, & Maratsos, Michael P. 1975. On the acquisition of front, back, and side. Child Development 46, 202–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuczaj, Stan A., II, & Maratsos, Michael P. 1983. Initial verbs of yes-no questions: A different kind of grammatical category. Developmental Psychology 19, 440–444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, Patricia K. 1985. Methods in the study of infant speech perception. In Gottleib, G. & Krasnegor, N. (eds.), Measurement of audition and vision in the first year of postnatal life: A methodological overview (223–251). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Kuhl, Patricia K.; Andruski, Jean E.; Chistovich, I. A.; Chistovich, L. A.; Kozhevnikova, E. V.; Ryskina, V. L.; Stolyarova, E. I.; Sundberg, Ulla; & Lacersa, Francisco. 1997. Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science 277, 684–686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, Patricia K., & Miller, James D. 1975. Speech perception in the chinchilla: Voiced-voiceless distinction in alveolar plosive consonants. Science 190, 69–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, Patricia K., & Padden, Denise M. 1982. Enhanced discriminability at the phonetic boundaries for the voicing feature in macaques. Perception & Psychophysics 32, 542–550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Küntay, Aylin C. 2002. Development of the expression of indefiniteness: Presenting new referents in Turkish picture-series stories. Discourse Processes 33, 77–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Küntay, Aylin, & Slobin, Dan I. 1996. Listening to a Turkish mother: Some puzzles for acquisition. In Slobin, D. I., Gerhardt, J., Kyratzis, A., & Guo, J. (eds.), Social interaction, social context, and language: Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (265–286). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kyratzis, Amy. 1993. Pragmatic and discursive influences on the acquisition of subordination-coordination. In Clark, E. V. (ed.), Proceedings of the 25th annual Child Language Research Forum 1993 (324–332). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Labov, William, & Labov, Teresa. 1978. The phonetics of cat and mama. Language 54, 816–852.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laing, Emma;Butterworth, George;Ansari, Daniel; Gsödl, Marisa;Longli, Elena;Panagiotaki, Georgia;Paterson, Sarah; & Karmiloff-Smith, Annette. 2002. Atypical development of language and social communication in toddlers with Williams Syndrome. Developmental Science 5, 233–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambert, Wallace E., & Tucker, G. Richard. 1972. Bilingual education of children: The St. Lambert experiment. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Landau, Barbara;Smith, Linda; & Jones, Susan. 1998. Object shape, object function, and object name. Journal of Memory & Language 38, 1–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lane, Harlan. 1976. The wild boy of Aveyron. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lanza, Elizabeth. 1992. Can bilingual two-year-olds code-switch? Journal of Child Language 19, 633–658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lanza, Elizabeth. 1997. Language mixing in infant bilingualism: A sociolinguistic perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lawrence, Valerie W., & Shipley, Elizabeth F. 1996. Parental speech to middle- and working-class children from two racial groups in three settings. Applied Psycholinguistics 17, 233–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lempers, Jacques;Flavell, Eleanor L.; & Flavell, John H. 1977. The development in very young children of tacit knowledge concerning visual perception. Genetic Psychology Monographs 95, 3–53.Google ScholarPubMed
Lenneberg, Eric. 1967. Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Leonard, Laurence B. 1998. Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Leopold, Werner F. 1939–1949. Speech development of a bilingual child, 4 vols. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Leung, Eleanor H., & Rheingold, Harriet L. 1981. Development of pointing as social gesture. Developmental Psychology 17, 215–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, Clara C. 1994. The acquisition of place. Leiden: Holland Institute of Generative Linguistics Publications.Google Scholar
Levelt, Willem J. M. 1989. Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levin, Beth. 1993. English verb classes and alternations. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Levinson, Stephen C. 1983. Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, Stephen C. 2000. Presumptive meanings: The theory of generalized conversational implicature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levitt, Andrea G., & Utman, Jennifer G. A. 1992. From babbling towards the the sound systems of English and French: A longitudinal two-case study. Journal of Child Language 19, 19–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, David K. 1969. Convention: A philosophical study. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, John D., & Elman, Jeffrey L. 2008. Growth-related neural reorganization and the autism phenotype: A test of the hypothesis that altered brain growth leads to altered connectivity. Developmental Science 11, 135–155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, Ping, & MacWhinney, Brian. 1996. Cryptotype, overgeneralization, and competition: A connectionist model of the learning of English reversive prefixes. Connectionist Science 8, 3–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, Alvin M.; Cooper, Frank S.;Shankweiler, Donald P.; & Studdert-Kennedy, Michael G. 1967. Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review 74, 431–461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieven, Elena V. M. 1978. Turn-taking and pragmatics: Two issues in early child language. In Campbell, R. N. & Smith, P. T. (eds.), Recent advances in the psychology of language (215–236). London: Plenum.Google Scholar
Lieven, Elena V. M. 1997. Variation in a crosslinguistic context. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vol. V (199–263). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lieven, Elena;Behrens, Heike;Speares, Jennifer; & Tomasello, Michael. 2003. Early syntactic creativity: A usage-based approach. Journal of Child Language 30, 333–370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieven, Elena V. M.; Pine, Julian M.; & Baldwin, Gillian. 1997. Lexically-based learning and early grammatical development. Journal of Child Language 24, 187–219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindblom, Björn. 1992. Phonological units as adaptive emergents of lexical development. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (eds.), Phonological development: Models, research, implications (131–163). Parkton, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Lindner, Gustav. 1898. Aus dem Naturgarten der Kindersprache: Ein Beitrag zur kindlichen Sprach- und Geistentwicklung in den ersten vier Lebensjahren. Leipzig: Grieben.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, Ulf; Carpenter, Malinda; Henning, Anne; Striano, Tricia; & Tomasello, Michael. 2004. Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. Developmental Science 7, 297–307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liszkowski, Ulf; Carpenter, Malinda; Striano, Tricia; & Tomasello, Michael. 2006. 12- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. Journal of Cognition & Development 7, 173–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, John L. 1993. The child's path to spoken language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lodge, K. R. 1979. The use of the past tense in games of pretend. Journal of Child Language 6, 365–369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loftus, Elizabeth F. 1979. Eye-witness testimony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lord, Catherine. 1979. “Don't you fall me down”: Children's generalizations regarding cause and transitivity. Papers & Reports on Child Language Development [Stanford University] 17, 81–89.Google Scholar
Lust, Barbara. 1977. Conjunction reduction in child language. Journal of Child Language 4, 257–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lust, Barbara. 1994. Functional projection of CP and phrase structure parametrization: An argument for the Strong Continuity Hypothesis. In Lust, B., Suñer, M., & Whitman, J. (eds.), Syntactic theory and first language acquisition, vol. I: Heads, projections, and learnability (85–118). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Macaulay, Ronald K. S. 1997. Standards and variation in urban speech. Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCabe, Ann E.; Evely, Susan; Abramovitch, Rona; Corter, Carl M.; & Pepler, Debra J. 1983. Conditional statements in young children's spontaneous speech. Journal of Child Language 10, 253–258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maccoby, Eleanor E., & Bee, Helen L. 1965. Some speculations concerning the lag between perceiving and performing. Child Development 36, 367–377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCune, Lorraine; Vihman, Marilyn M.; Roug-Hellichius, Liselotte; Delery, Diane B.; & Gogate, Lakshmi. 1996. Grunt communication in human infants (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology 110, 27–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, Karla K. 1994. Article use in the spontaneous samples of children with specific language impairment: The importance of considering syntactic contexts. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 8, 153–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, Karla K.; Sheng, Li; & Smith, Bruce. 2005. The precocious two-year-old: Status of the lexicon and links to the grammar. Journal of Child Language 32, 563–585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macken, Marlys A. 1980. The child's lexical representation: The “puzzle-puddle-pickle” evidence. Journal of Linguistics 16, 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaughlin, Barry. 1984. Second language acquisition in childhood, vol. I: Preschool children (2nd edn). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McMurray, Bob. 2007. Defusing the childhood vocabulary explosion. Science 317, 631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maclay, Howard, & Osgood, Charles E. 1959. Hesitation phenomena in spontaneous English speech. Word 15, 19–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macnamara, John. 1982. Names for things: A study of human learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
McShane, John. 1980. Learning to talk. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McTear, Michael. 1978. Repetition in child language: Imitation or creation? In Campbell, R. N. & Smith, P. T. (eds.), Recent advances in the psychology of language (293–311). London: Plenum.Google Scholar
McTear, Michael. 1985. Children's conversation. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, Brian. 1978. The acquisition of morphophonology. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 43 (serial no. 174).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, Brian. 1985. Hungarian language acquisition as an exemplification of a general model of grammatical development. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vol. II (1069–1155). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B., & Snow, Catherine. 1985. The Child Language Data Exchange System. Journal of Child Language 12, 271–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B., & Snow, Catherine. 1990. The Child Language Data Exchange System: An update. Journal of Child Language 17, 457–472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McWilliam, Donna, & Howe, Christine. 2004. Enhancing pre-schoolers' reasoning skills: An intervention to optimise the use of justificatory speech acts during peer interaction. Language & Education 18, 504–524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maekawa, Junko, & Storkel, Holly L. 2006. Individual differences in the influence of phonological characteristics on expressive vocabulary development by young children. Journal of Child Language 33, 439–459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Major, D. R. 1906. First steps in mental growth: A series of studies in the psychology of infancy. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Malherbe, E. G. 1969. Comments on “How and when do persons become bilingual?” In Kelly, L. G. (ed.), Description and measurement of bilingualism (325–327). Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Mandler, Jean M., & McDonough, Laraine. 1993. Concept formation in infancy. Cognitive Development 8, 291–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maneva, Blagovesta, & Genesee, Fred. 2002. Bilingual babbling: Evidence for language differentiation in dual language acquisition. In Skarabela, B., Fish, S., & Do, A. H.-J. (eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Boston University Conference on Language Development (383–392). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Manning, Christopher D., & Schütze, Hinrich. 1999. Foundations of statistical natural language processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Maratsos, Michael P. 1973. Nonegocentric communication abilities in preschool children. Child Development 44, 697–700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maratsos, Michael P. 1974. Preschool children's use of definite and indefinite articles. Child Development 45, 446–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maratsos, Michael P. 1976. The use of definite and indefinite reference in young children: An experimental study in semantic acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Maratsos, Michael P. 1993. Artifactual overregularizations? In Clark, E. V. (ed.), Proceedings of the 24th annual Child Language Research Forum (139–148). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Maratsos, Michael P. 2000. More overregularizations after all: New data and discussion on Marcus, Pinker, Ullman, Hollander, Rosen, & Xu. Journal of Child Language 27, 183–212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maratsos, Michael P., & Chalkley, Mary Anne. 1980. The internal language of children's syntax: The ontogenesis and representation of syntactic categories. In Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children's language, vol. II (127–214). New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
Maratsos, Michael P.; Fox, Dana E. C.; Becker, Judith A.; & Chalkley, Mary Anne. 1985. Semantic restrictions on children's passives. Cognition 19, 167–191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchand, Hans. 1969. English word-formation (2nd rev. edn). Munich: C. H. Beck.Google Scholar
Marchman, Virginia, & Bates, Elizabeth. 1994. Continuity in lexical and morphological development: A test of the critical mass hypothesis. Journal of Child Language 21, 339–366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcos, Haydée. 1991. Reformulating requests at 18 months: Gestures, vocalizations, and words. First Language 11, 361–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcos, Haydée, & Kornhaber-Le Chanu, Mila. 1992. Learning how to insist and clarify in the second year: Reformulation of requests in different contexts. International Journal of Behavioral Development 15, 359–376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcos, Haydée; Ryckebusch, Céline; & Rabain-Jamin, Jacqueline. 2003. Adult responses to young children's communicative gestures: Joint achievement of speech acts. First Language 23, 213–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcus, Gary F. 1993. Negative evidence in language acquisition. Cognition 46, 53–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, Gary F.; Pinker, Steven; Ullman, Michael; Hollander, Michelle; Rosen, T. John; & Xu, Fei. 1992. Overregularization in language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 57 (serial no. 228).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marian, Viorica, & Spivey, Michael. 2003. Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within- and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition 6, 97–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, Viorica; Spivey, Michael; & Hirsch, Joy. 2003. Shared and separate systems in bilingual language processing: Converging evidence from eye-tracking and brain-imaging. Brain & Language 86, 70–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markman, Ellen M. 1987. How children constrain the possible meanings of words. In Neisser, U. (ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (255–287). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Markman, Ellen M. 1989. Categorization and naming in children: Problems of induction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Markman, Ellen M., & Hutchinson, Jean E. 1984. Children's sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic versus thematic relations. Cognitive Psychology 16, 1–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markman, Ellen M., & Wachtel, Gwyn F. 1988. Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology 20, 121–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martlew, Margaret; Connolly, Kevin; & McCleod, Christine. 1978. Language use, role and context in a five-year-old. Journal of Child Language 5, 81–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, M. K. 1942. Learning to speak after six and one-half years. Journal of Speech Disorders 7, 295–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masur, Elise F. 1997. Maternal labeling of novel and familiar objects: Implications for children's development of lexical constraints. Journal of Child Language 24, 427–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, Danielle; Lieven, Elena; Theakston, Anna; & Tomasello, Michael. 2006. The effect of perceptual availability and prior discourse on young children's use of referring expressions. Applied Psycholinguistics 27, 403–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, Danielle; Lieven, Elena; & Tomasello, Michael. 2007. How toddlers and preschoolers learn to uniquely identify referents for others: A training study. Child Development 78, 1744–1759.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattys, Sven L.; Jusczyk, Peter W.; Luce, Paul A.; & Morgan, James L. 1999. Phonotactic and prosodic effects on word segmentation in infants. Cognitive Psychology 38, 465–494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maye, Jessica; Werker, Janet F.; & Gerken, LouAnn. 2002. Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition 82, B101–B111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayer, Mercer. 1969. Frog, where are you?New York: Dial Press.Google Scholar
Mechelli, Andrea; Crinion, Jennifer T.; Noppeney, Uta; O'Doherty, John; Ashburner, John; Frackowiak, Richard S.; & Price, Cathy J. 2004. Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature 143, 757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehler, Jacques; Bertoncini, Josiane;Barrière, Michèle; & Jassik-Gerschenfeld, Dora. 1978. Infant recognition of mother's voice. Perception 7, 491–497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehler, Jacques.; Jusczyk, Peter W.; Lambertz, Ghislaine; Halsted, Nilofar; Bertoncini, Josiane; & Amiel-Tison, Claudine. 1988. A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition 29, 143–178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meisel, Jürgen (ed.). 1990. Two first languages: Early grammatical development in bilingual children. Dordrecht: Foris.CrossRef
Meisel, Jürgen. 1994. Code-switching in young bilingual children: The acquisition of grammatical constraints. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 16, 413–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meisel, Jürgen M. 2007. The weaker language in early child bilingualism: Acquiring a first language as a second language? Applied Psycholinguistics 28, 495–514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menn, Lise. 1971. Phonotactic rules in beginning speech. Lingua 26, 225–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merriman, William E., & Bowman, Laura L. 1989. The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 54 (serial no. 220).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mervis, Carolyn B. 1987. Child-basic object categories and early lexical development. In Neisser, U. (ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (201–233). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mervis, Carolyn B. 1999. Williams syndrome cognitive profile: Strengths, weaknesses, and interrelations among auditory short-term memory, language, and visuospatial constructive cognition. In Winograd, E. & Fivush, R. (eds.), Ecological approaches to cognition: Essays in honor of Ulric Neisser (193–227). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mervis, Carolyn B., & Greco, Carolyn. 1984. Parts and early conceptual development: Comment on Tversky & Hemenway. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 113, 194–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mervis, Carolyn B., & Johnson, Kathy E. 1991. Acquisition of the plural morpheme: A case study. Developmental Psychology 27, 222–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mervis, Carolyn B., & Long, Laurel M. 1987. Words refer to whole objects: Young children's interpretation of the referent of a novel word. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Baltimore, MD.
Messer, David J. 1978. The integration of mothers' referential speech with joint play. Child Development 49, 781–787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Messer, Stanley. 1967. Implicit phonology in children. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 6, 609–613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metraux, R. W. 1965. Study of bilingualism among children of U.S.-French parents. French Review 38, 650–665.Google Scholar
Mikes, Melanie. 1967. Acquisition des catégories grammaticales dans le langage de l'enfant. Enfance 20, 289–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, George A. 1967. Project grammarama. In The psychology of communication (125–187). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Miller, James D.; Weir, C.; Pastore, L.; Kelly, W. J.; & Dooling, Robert J. 1976. Discrimination and labeling of noise-buzz sequences with varying noise-lead times: An example of categorical perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 60, 410–417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, Peggy. 1982. Amy, Wendy, and Beth: Learning language in South Baltimore. Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Miller, Peggy. 1986. Teasing as language socialization in a white working-class community. In Schieffelin, B. B. & Ochs, E. (eds.), Language socialization across cultures (199–212). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Miller, Wick, & Ervin, Susan. 1964. The development of grammar in child language. In Bellugi, U. & Brown, R. (eds.), The acquisition of language (9–34). Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 29 (serial no. 92).
Mills, Debra L.; Coffey-Corina, Sharon; & Neville, Helen J. 1993. Language acquisition and cerebral specialization in 20-month-old infants. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 5, 317–334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mills, Debra L.; Coffey-Corina, Sharon; & Neville, Helen J. 1997. Language comprehension and cerebral specialization from 13 to 20 months. Developmental Neuropsychology 13, 397–445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills, Debra L.; Plunkett, Kim; Prat, Chantal; & Schafer, Graham. 2005. Watching the infant brain learn words: Effects of vocabulary size and experience. Cognitive Development 20, 19–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milroy, James, & Milroy, Lesley. 1991. Authority in language: Investigating language standardization. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Milroy, Lesley. 1980. Language and social networks. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Mintz, Toben H. 2002. Category induction from distributional cues in an artificial language. Memory & Cognition 30, 678–686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mintz, Toben H. 2003. Frequent frames as a cue for grammatical categories in child directed speech. Cognition 90, 91–117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mintz, Toben H.; Newport, Elissa L.; & Bever, Thomas G. 2002. The distributional structure of grammatical categories in speech to young children. Cognitive Science 26, 393–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moerk, Ernst L. 1983. The mother of Eve as a first language teacher. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Moeser, Shannon D., & Bregman, Albert S. 1972. The role of reference in the acquisition of a miniature artificial language. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 11, 759–769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moffitt, Alan R. 1971. Consonant cue perception by 20 and 24-week-old infants. Child Development 42, 717–731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moll, Henrike; Koring, Cornelia; Carpenter, Malinda; & Tomasello, Michael. 2006. Infants determine others' focus of attention by pragmatics and exclusion. Journal of Cognition & Development 7, 411–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moon, Christine; Cooper, Robin P.; & Fifer, William P. 1993. Two-day-olds prefer their native language. Infant Behavior & Development 16, 495–500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, Chris, & Dunham, Philip J. (eds.), 1995. Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Morgan, James L., & Saffran, Jenny R. 1995. Emerging integration of sequential and suprasegmental information in preverbal speech segmentation. Child Development 66, 911–936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, James L., & Travis, Lisa L. 1989. Limits on negative information in language input. Journal of Child Language 16, 531–552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morikawa, Hiromi. 1991. Acquisition of causatives in Japanese. Papers & Reports on Child Language Development [Stanford University] 30, 80–87.Google Scholar
Morimoto, Yukiko. 1999. Making words in two languages: A prosodic account of Japanese-English language mixing. International Journal of Bilingualism 3, 23–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, Colleen A., & Mervis, Carolyn B. 1999. Williams syndrome. In Goldstein, S. & Reynolds, C. R. (eds.), Handbook of neurodevelopmental and genetic disorders in children (555–590). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Morse, Philip A. 1972. The discrimination of speech and nonspeech stimuli in early infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 14, 477–492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morton, John, & Smith, Neilson V. 1974. Some ideas concerning the acquisition of phonology. In Problèmes actuels de psycholinguistique/Current problems in psycholinguistics (161–176). Paris: Editions du CNRS.Google Scholar
Mosier, Christine E., & Rogoff, Barbara. 1994. Infants' instrumental use of their mothers to achieve their goals. Child Development 65, 70–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mowrer, O. Hobart. 1960. Learning theory and symbolic processes. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muir, Darwin, & Hains, Sylvia. 1999. Young infants' perception of adult intentionality: Adult contingency and eye direction. In Rochat, P. (ed.), Early social cognition (155–187). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mulford, Randa C. 1983. On the acquisition of derivational morphology in Icelandic: Learning about -ari. Islenskt mál og almenn málfrae∂i 5, 105–125.Google Scholar
Muller, Eric; Hollien, Harry; & Murry, Thomas. 1974. Perceptual responses to infant crying – identification of cry types. Journal of Child Language 1, 89–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, Catherine M., & Messer, David J. 1977. Mothers, infants, and pointing: A study of a gesture. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies of mother-infant interaction (325–354). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Myers-Scotton, Carol. 1993a. Duelling languages: Grammatical structure in code-switching. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Myers-Scotton, Carol. 1993b. Social motivations for code-switching: Evidence from Africa. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nagy, William E., & Anderson, Richard C. 1984. How many words are there in printed school English? Reading Research Quarterly 19, 304–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Naigles, Letitia R., & Hoff-Ginsberg, Erika. 1995. Input to verb-learning: Evidence for the plausibility of syntactic bootstrapping. Developmental Psychology 31, 827–837.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Naigles, Letitia R., & Hoff-Ginsberg, Erika. 1998. Why are some verbs learned before other verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on children's early verb use. Journal of Child Language 25, 95–120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naigles, Letitia R., & Mayeux, Lara. 2000. Television as an incidental teacher. In Singer, D. & Singer, J. (eds.), Handbook of children and the media (135–153). New York: Sage.Google Scholar
Nakamura, Keiko. 2001. The acquisition of polite language by Japanese children. In Nelson, K. E. & Aksu-Koç, A. (eds.), Children's language, vol. X (93–112). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Namy, Laura L., & Waxman, Sandra R. 1998. Words and gestures: Infants' interpretations of different forms of symbolic reference. Child Development 69, 295–308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Namy, Laura L., & Waxman, Sandra R. 2000. Naming and exclaiming: Infants' sensitivity to naming contexts. Journal of Cognition & Development 1, 405–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nayer, Samantha L., & Graham, Susan A. 2006. Children's communicative strategies in novel and familiar word situations. First Language 26, 403–420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, Thierry; Bertoncini, Josiane; & Mehler, Jacques. 1998. Language discrimination by newborns: Toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perceptioon & Performance 24, 756–766.Google ScholarPubMed
Nazzi, Thierry; Jusczyk, Peter W.; & Johnson, Elizabeth K. 2000. Language discrimination by English-learning 5-month-olds: Effects of rhythm and familiarity. Journal of Memory & Language 43, 1–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, Katherine. 1973. Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 38 (serial no. 149).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, Katherine. 1975. The nominal shift in semantic-syntactic development. Cognitive Psychology 7, 461–479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, Katherine. 1988. Constraints on word learning? Cognitive Development 3, 221–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, Katherine (ed.). 1989. Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Nelson, Lauren, & Bauer, Harold R. 1991. Speech and language production at age 2: Evidence for tradeoffs between linguistic and phonetic processing. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research 34, 879–892.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neville, Helen J.; Coffey, Sharon A.; Lawson, Donald S.; Fischer, Andrew; Emmorey, Karen; & Bellugi, Ursula. 1997. Neural systems mediating American Sign Language: Effects of sensory experience and age of acquisition. Brain & Language 57, 285–308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newcombe, Nora, & Zaslow, Martha. 1981. Do 2 1/2-year-olds hint? A study of directive forms in the speech of 2 1/2-year-old children to adults. Discourse Processes 4, 239–252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, Jean E.; Lovett, Maureen W.; & Dennis, Maureen. 1986. The use of discourse analysis in neurolinguistics: Some findings from the narratives of hemidecorticate adolescents. Topics in Language Disorders 7, 31–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newport, Elissa L. 1988. Constraints on learning and their role in language acquisition: Studies of the acquisition of American Sign Language. Language Sciences 10, 147–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newport, Elissa L. 1990. Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science 14, 11–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newport, Elissa L. 1991. Contrasting conceptions of the critical period for language. In Carey, S. & Gelman, R. (eds.), The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition (111–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Newport, Elissa; Gleitman, Henry; & Gleitman, Lila R. 1977. “Mother, I'd rather do it myself”: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (109–149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nicolaci-da-Costa, Ana, & Harris, Margaret. 1983. Redundancy of syntactic information: An aid to young children's comprehension of sentential number. British Journal of Psychology 74, 343–352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicolaci-da-Costa, Ana, & Harris, Margaret. 1984/1985. Individual differences in the acquisition of number markers. First Language 5, 185–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, Elena, & Genesee, Fred. 1996. A longitudinal study of pragmatic differentiation in young bilingual children. Language Learning 46, 439–464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niemi, Jussi; Laine, Matti; & Tuominen, Juhani. 1994. Cognitive morphology in Finnish: Foundations of a new model. Language & Cognitive Processes 9, 423–446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ninio, Anat. 1999a. Pathbreaking verbs in syntactic development and the question of prototypical transitivity. Journal of Child Language 26, 619–653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ninio, Anat. 1999b. Model learning in syntax development. International Journal of Bilingualism 3, 111–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noveck, Ira A. 2001. When children are more logical than adults: Experimental investigations of scalar implicature. Cognition 78, 165–188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nunberg, Geoffrey. 1996. Transfers of meaning. In Pustejovsky, J. & Boguraev, B. (eds.), Lexical semantics: The problem of polysemy (109–132). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Ochs, Elinor. 1982. Talking to children in Western Samoa. Language in Society 11, 77–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ochs, Elinor. 1992. Indexing gender. In Duranti, A. & Goodwin, C. (eds.), Rethinking context (335–358). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ochs, Elinor, & Schieffelin, Bambi B. 1984. Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories. In Schweder, R. & Levin, R. (eds.), Culture theory: Essays in mind, self and emotion (276–320). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ochs, Elinor; Schieffelin, Bambi B.; & Platt, Martha L. 1979. Propositions across utterances and speakers. In Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. B. (eds.), Developmental pragmatics (251–268). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ochs, Elinor, & Taylor, Carolyn. 1995. The “father knows best” dynamic in dinnertime narratives. In Hall, K. & Bucholtz, M. (eds.), Gender articulated: Language and the socially constructed self (97–120). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
O'Connor, Mary Catherine. 1996. Managing the intermental: Classroom group discussion and the social context of learning. In Slobin, D. I., Gerhardt, J., Kyratzis, A., & Guo, J. (eds.), Social interaction, social context, and language: Esssays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (495–509). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
O'Grady, William. 1993. Functional categories and maturation: Data from Korean. Proceedings of the 5th Harvard International Symposium on Korean Linguistics (96–112). Seoul: Hanshin.Google Scholar
O'Grady, William. 1997. Syntactic development. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ogura, Tamiko; Dale, Philip S.; Yamashita, Yukie; Murase, Toshii; & Mahieu, Aki. 2006. The use of nouns and verbs by Japanese children and their caregivers in book-reading and toy-playing contexts. Journal of Child Language 33, 1–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oksaar, Els. 1970. Zum Spracherwerb des Kindes in Zweisprachiger Umgebung. Folia Linguistica 4, 330–358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oller, D. Kimbrough; Wieman, Leslie A.; Doyle, William J.; & Ross, Carol. 1976. Infant babbling and speech. Journal of Child Language 3, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olmsted, D. L. 1971. Out of the mouth of babes. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Omar, Margaret K. 1973. The acquisition of Egyptian Arabic as a native language. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Neill, Daniela K. 1996. Two-year-olds' sensitivity to the parent's knowledge when making requests. Child Development 67, 659–677.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orsolini, Margherita; Fanari, Rachele; & Bowles, Hugo. 1998. Acquiring regular and irregular inflection in a language with verb classes. Language & Cognitive Processes 13, 425–464.Google Scholar
Oviatt, Sharon L. 1980. The emerging ability to comprehend language: An experimental approach. Child Development 51, 97–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oviatt, Sharon L.; Levow, Gina-Anne; Moreton, Elliot; & MacEachern, Margaret. 1998. Modeling global and focal hyperarticulation during human-computer error resolution. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 104, 3080–3098.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paley, Vivian G. 1984. Boys and girls: Superheroes in the doll corner. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panneton Cooper, Robin, & Aslin, Richard. 1990. Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development 61, 1584–1595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papafragou, Anna, & Tantalou, Niki. 2004. The computation of implicatures by young children. Language Acquisition 12, 71–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, Johanne. 1996. Phonological differentiation in a bilingual child: Hildegard revisited. In Stringfellow, A., Cahana-Amitay, D., Hughes, E., & Zukowski, A. (eds.), Proceedings of the 20th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (528–539). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Paradis, Johanne, & Genesee, Fred. 1996. Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: Autonomous or independent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition 18, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, Johanne, & Genesee, Fred. 1997. On continuity and the emergence of functional categories in bilingual first-language acquisition. Language Acquisition 6, 91–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pasquini, Elisabeth S.; Corriveau, Kathleen H.; Koenig, Melissa A.; & Harris, Paul L. 2007. Preschoolers monitor the relative accuracy of informants. Developmental Psychology 43, 1216–1226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, Hermann. 1898. Principien der Sprachgeschichte (3rd edn). Halle: Max Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Pavlovitch, Milivoie. 1920. Le langage enfantin: Acquisition du serbe et du français par un enfant serbe. Paris: Champion.Google Scholar
Payne, Arvilla C. 1980. Factors controlling the acquisition of the Philadelphia dialect by out-of-state children. In Labov, W. (ed.), Locating language in time and space (143–178). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Peters, Ann M. 1983. The units of language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Peters, Ann M. 1985. Language segmentation: Operating principles for the perception and analysis of language. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vol. II (1029–1067). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Peters, Ann M., & Menn, Lise. 1993. False starts and filler syllables: Ways to learn grammatical morphemes. Language 69, 742–777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petretic, Patricia A., & Tweney, Ryan D. 1977. Does comprehension precede production? The development of children's responses to telegraphic sentences of varying grammatical adequacy. Journal of Child Language 4, 201–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Philips, Susan U. 1972. Participant structures and communicative competence: Warm Springs children in community and classroom. In Cazden, C., John, V., & Hymes, D. (eds.), Functions of language in the classroom (370–394). New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
Philips, Susan U. 1976. Some sources of cultural variability in the regulation of talk. Language in Society 5, 81–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, Juliet R. 1973. Syntax and vocabulary of mothers' speech to young children: Age and sex comparisons. Child Development 44, 182–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, Jean. 1952. Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood [La formation du symbole chez l'enfant]. (Translated by Gattegno, C. & Hodgson, F. M..) New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Piché, Gene L.; Rubin, Donald L.; & Michlin, Michael L. 1978. Age and social class in children's use of persuasive communicative appeals. Child Development 49, 773–780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pine, Julian M., & Lieven, Elena V. M. 1993. Re-analysing rote-learned phrases: Individual differences in the transition to multi-word speech. Journal of Child Language 20, 551–571.Google Scholar
Pine, Julian M., & Lieven, Elena V. M. 1997. Slot and frame patterns and the development of the determiner category. Applied Psycholinguistics 18, 123–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pine, Julian M.; Lieven, Elena V. M.; & Rowland, Caroline F. 1998. Comparing different models of the development of the English verb category. Linguistics 36, 807–830.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pine, Julian M., & Martindale, Helen. 1996. Syntactic categories in the speech of young children: The case of the determiner. Journal of Child Language 23, 369–395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, Steven. 1984. Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven. 1989. Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of argument structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven. 1999. Words and rules. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven; Lebeaux, David S.; & Frost, Loren A. 1987. Productivity and constraints in the acquisition of the passive. Cognition 26, 195–267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, Steven; Ullman, Michael; McClelland, James L.; & Patterson, Karalyn. 2002. The past-tense debate. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6, 456–474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pléh, Csaba. 1998. Early spatial case markers in Hungarian children. In Clark, E. V. (ed.), Proceedings of the 29th annual Child Language Research Forum 1997 (211–219). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Plunkett, Kim. 1993. Lexical segmentation and vocabulary growth in early language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 20, 43–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plunkett, Kim, & Marchman, Virginia. 1993. From rote learning to system building: Acquiring verb morphology in children and connectionist nets. Cognition 48, 21–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plunkett, Kim, & Nakisa, Ramin C. 1997. A connectionist model of the Arabic plural system. Language & Cognitive Processes 12, 807–836.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poplack, Shana. 1980. Sometimes I'll start a sentence in Spanish y termino en español: Toward a typology of code-switching. Linguistics 18, 581–618.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Postma, Albert. 2000. Detection of errors during speech production: A review of speech monitoring models. Cognition 77, 97–132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poulin-Dubois, Dianne, & Goodz, Naomi. 2001. Language differentiation in bilingual infants: Evidence form babbling. In Cenoz, J. & Genesee, F. (eds.), Trends in bilingual acquisition (95–106). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, Diane; Graham, Susan A.; & Riddle, Andrea S. 1995. Salient object parts and infants' acquisition of novel object words. First Language 15, 301–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Povinelli, Daniel J.; Reaux, James E.; Bierschwale, Donna T.; Allain, Ashley D.; & Simon, Bridgett B. 1997. Exploitation of pointing as a referential gesture in young children, but not adolescent chimpanzees. Cognitive Development 12, 423–461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pratt, Michael W.; Kerig, Patricia; Cowan, Philip A.; & Pape Cowan, Carolyn. 1988. Mothers and fathers teaching 3-year-olds: Authoritative parenting and adult scaffolding of young children's learning. Developmental Psychology 24, 832–839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preissler, Melissa A., & Carey, Susan. 2004. Do both pictures and words function as symbols for 18- and 24-month-old children? Journal of Cognition & Development 5, 185–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preyer, Wilhelm. 1882. Die Seele des Kindes: Beobachtungen über die geistige Entwicklung des Menschen in den ersten Lebensjahren. Leipzig: Schäfer.Google Scholar
Prince, Ellen. 1981. Toward a taxonomy of given-new information. In Cole, P. (ed.), Radical pragmatics (223–255). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Prudden, Shannnon M.; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Golinkoff, Roberta M.; & Hennon, Elizabeth A. 2006. The birth of words: Ten-month-olds learn words through perceptual salience. Child Development 77, 266–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pullum, Geoffrey K. 1996. Learnability, hyperlearning, and the poverty of the stimulus. In Johnson, J., Juge, M. L., & Moxley, J. L. (eds.), Proceedings of the 22nd annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society: Parasession on the role of learnability in grammatical theory (498–513). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Pullum, Geoffrey K., & Scholz, Barbara C. 2002. Empirical assessment of stimulus poverty arguments. The Linguistic Review 19 (1–2).Google Scholar
Purcell, April K. 1984. Code shifting Hawaiian style: Children's accommodations along a decreolizing continuum. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 46, 71–86.Google Scholar
Pye, Clifton. 1986. Quiché Mayan speech to children. Language 59, 583–604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quay, Suzanne. 1995. The bilingual lexicon: Implications for studies of language choice. Journal of Child Language 22, 369–387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quine, Willard v. O. 1960. Word and object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Radford, Andrew. 1990. Syntactic theory and the acquisition of English syntax. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Ramscar, Michael. 2002. The role of meaning in inflection: Why the past tense doesn't require a rule. Cognitive Psychology 45, 45–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ravid, Dorit D. 1995. Language change in child and adult Hebrew. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Read, Barbara K., & Cherry, Louise J. 1978. Preschool children's production of directive forms. Discourse Processes 1, 233–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redford, Melissa A., & Miikkulainen, Risto. 2007. Effects of acquisition rate on emergent structure in phonological development. Language 83, 737–769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redlinger, Wendy E., & Park, Tschang-zin. 1980. Language mixing in young bilinguals. Journal of Child Language 7, 337–352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reeder, Kenneth. 1980. The emergence of illocutionary skills. Journal of Child Language 7, 13–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Réger, Zita. 1986. The functions of imitation in child language. Applied Psycholinguistics 7, 323–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reilly, Judy S. 1982. The acquisition of conditionals in English. PhD dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.
Reilly, Judy S. 1986. The acquisition of temporals and conditionals. In Traugott, E. C., Meulen, A., Reilly, J. S., & Ferguson, C. A. (eds.), On conditionals (309–331). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, Leslie A. 1980. Overextension in early language development. Journal of Child Language 7, 321–335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rheingold, Harriet L.; Hay, Dale F.; & West, Meredith J. 1976. Sharing in the second year of life. Child Development 47, 1148–1158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, Mabel; Huston, Aletha; Truglio, Rosemarie; & Wright, John C. 1990. Words from “Sesame Street”: Learning vocabulary while viewing. Developmental Psychology 26, 421–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rivera-Gaxiola, Martitza; Klarman, Lindsay; Garcia-Sierra, Adrian; & Kuhl, Patricia K. 2005. Neural patterns to speech and vocabulary growth in American infants. NeuroReport 16, 495–498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzi, Luigi. 1994. Early null subjects and root null subjects. In Lust, B., Hermon, G., & Kornfilt, J. (eds.), Syntactic theory and first language acquisition: Cross-linguistic perspectives, vol. II: Binding dependencies (249–272). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Robb, Michael P.; Bauer, Harold R.; & Tyler, Ann A. 1994. A quantitative analysis of the single-word stage. First Language 14, 37–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, Joanne E.; Burchinal, Margaret; & Durham, Meghan. 1999. Parents' report of vocabulary and grammatical development of African American preschoolers: Child and environmental associations. Child Development 70, 92–106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, Julie. 1997. Acquisition of variable rules: A study of (-t, -d) deletion in preschool children. Journal of Child Language 24, 351–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, Julie, & Labov, William. 1995. Learning to talk Philadelphian. Language Variation & Change 7, 101–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robertson, Kirsten, & Murachver, Tamar. 2003. Children's accommodation to gendered language styles. Journal of Language & Social Psychology 22, 321–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, Byron F., & Mervis, Carolyn B. 1998. Disentangling early language development: Modeling lexical and grammatical acquisition using an extension of case-study methodology. Developmental Psychology 34, 363–375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodrigo, Maria J.; González, Angela; Vega, Manuel; Muñetón-Ayala, Mercedes; & Rodriguez, Guacimara. 2004. From gestural to verbal deixis: A longitudinal study with Spanish infants and toddlers. First Language 24, 71–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodriguez-Fornells, Antoni; Rotte, Michael; Heinze, Hans-Jochen; Nösselt, Tömme; & Münte, Thomas F. 2002. Brain potential and functional MRI evidence for how to handle two languages with one brain. Nature 415, 1026–1029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, Don. 1978. Information about word-meaning in the speech of parents to young children. In Campbell, R. N. & Smith, P. T. (eds.), Recent advances in the psychology of language (187–198). London: Plenum.Google Scholar
Rome-Flanders, Tibie; Cronk, Carolyn; & Gourde, Christine. 1995. Maternal scaffolding in mother-infant games and its relationship to language development: A longitudinal study. First Language 15, 339–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ronjat, Jules. 1913. Le développement du langage observé chez un enfant bilingue. Paris: Champion.Google Scholar
Rosch, Eleanor. 1973. On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (111–144). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rousseau, Jean-Jacques. 1992. Emile, ou de l'éducation [1792]. Classiques Garnier. Paris: Bordas.Google Scholar
Rowe, Meredith L. 2000. Pointing and talk by low-income mothers and their 14-month-old children. First Language 20, 305–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowland, Caroline F., & Pine, Julian M. 2000. Subject-auxiliary inversion errors and wh-question acquisition: “What do children know?”Journal of Child Language 27, 157–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozendaal, Margot I., & Baker, Anne E. 2008. A cross-linguistic investigation of the acquisition of the pragmatics of indefinite and definite reference in two-year-olds. Journal of Child Language 35, 773–808.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruke-Dravina, Velta. 1973. On the emergence of inflection in child language: A contribution based on Latvian speech data. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds.), Studies of child language development (252–267). New York: Holt Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, David E., & McClelland, Jay L. 1986. On learning the past tenses of English verbs. In Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L. (eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Exploration in the microstructure of cognition, vol. II: Psychological and biological models (216–271). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sachs, Jacqueline. 1979. Topic selection in parent-child discourse. Discourse Processes 2, 145–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sachs, Jacqueline; Bard, Barbara; & Johnson, Marie L. 1981. Language learning with restricted input: Case studies of two hearing children of deaf parents. Applied Psycholinguistics 1, 33–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sachs, Jacqueline; Brown, Robert; & Salerno, Raffaela A. 1976. Adults' speech to children. In Raffler-Engel, W. & LeBrun, Y. (eds.), Baby talk and infant speech (special issue). Neurolinguistics 5, 240–245.
Sachs, Jacqueline, & Devin, Judith. 1976. Young children's use of age-appropriate speech styles in social interaction and role playing. Journal of Child Language 3, 81–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sachs, Jacqueline, & Johnson, Marie. 1976. Language development in a hearing child of deaf parents. In Raffler-Engel, W. & LeBrun, Y. (eds.), Baby talk and infant speech (special issue). Neurolinguistics 5, 246–252.
Saffran, Jenny R.; Aslin, Richard N.; & Newport, Elissa L. 1996. Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science 274, 1926–1928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saffran, Jenny R.; Johnson, Elizabeth K.; Aslin, Richard N.; & Newport, Elissa L. 1999. Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Cognition 70, 27–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saffran, Jenny R.; Newport, Elissa L.; & Aslin, Richard N. 1996. Word segmentation: The role of distributional cues. Journal of Memory & Language 35, 606–621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salazar Orvig, Anne; Hassan, Rouba; Leber-Marin, Jocelyne; Marcos, Haydée; Morgenstern, Aliyah; & Pares, Jacques. 2006. Peut-on parler d'anaphore chez le jeune enfant? Le cas des pronoms de 3e. personne. Langages 163, 10–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sampson, Geoffrey. 1987. Writing systems (2nd edn). London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Sandler, Wendy; Meir, Irit; Padden, Carol; & Aronoff, Mark. 2005. The emergence of grammar in a new sign language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102(7), 2661–2665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandra, Dominiek, & Taft, Marcus (eds.). 1994. Morphological structure, lexical representations, and lexical access. Language & Cognitive Processes 9, 225–472.
Santelmann, Lynn M., & Jusczyk, Peter W. 1998. Sensitivity to discontinuous dependencies in language learners: Evidence for limitations in processing space. Cognition 69, 105–134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sapir, Edward. 1924. The grammarian and his language. American Mercury 1, 149–155. (Reprinted in D. G. Mandelbaum (ed.), Selected writings of Edward Sapir in language, culture, and personality. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press, 1958.)Google Scholar
Savage, Susan L., & Au, Terry K.-F. 1996. What word learners do when input contradicts the mutual exclusivity assumption. Child Development 67, 3120–3134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saville-Troike, Muriel. 1988. Private speech: Evidence for second language learning strategies during the “silent” period. Journal of Child Language 15, 567–590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sawyer, R. Keith. 1997. Pretend play as improvisation. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Saxton, Matthew. 1997. The contrast theory of negative input. Journal of Child Language 24, 139–161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saxton, Matthew; Kulcsar, Bela; Marshall, Greer; & Rupra, Mandeep. 1998. Longer-term effects of corrective input: An experimental approach. Journal of Child Language 25, 701–721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saylor, Megan M.; Baird, Jodie A.; & Gallerani, Catherine. 2006. Telling others what's new: Preschoolers' adherence to the given-new contract. Journal of Cognition & Development 7, 341–379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saylor, Megan M., & Sabbagh, Mark A. 2004. Different kinds of information affect word learning in the preschool years: The case of part-term learning. Child Development 75, 395–408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saylor, Megan M.; Sabbagh, Mark A.; & Baldwin, Dare A. 2002. Children use whole-part juxtaposition as a pragmatic cue to word meaning. Developmental Psychology 38, 993–1003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schelletter, Christina. 2002. The effect of form similarity on bilingual children's lexical development. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition 5, 93–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schieffelin, Bambi B. 1979. Getting it together: An ethnographic approach to the study of the development of communicative competence. In Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. B. (eds.), Developmental pragmatics (73–108). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schlesinger, Itzak M. 1974. Relational concepts underlying language. In Scheifelbusch, R. L. & Lloyd, L. L. (eds.), Language perspectives – acquisition, retardation, and intervention (129–151). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Schlesinger, Itzak M. 1995. Cognitive space and linguistic case: Semantic and syntactic categories in English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, Chris L. 1996. Scrutinizing reference: How gesture and speech are coordinated in mother-child interaction. Journal of Child Language 23, 279–305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scholz, Barbara C., & Pullum, Geoffrey K. 2006. Irrational nativist exuberance. In Stainton, R. (ed.), Contemporary debates in cognitive science (59–80). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Schütze, Hinrich. 1993. Part-of-speech induction from scratch. In Proceedings of the 31st annual meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (251–258). Morristown, NJ: Association of Computational Linguistics.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schütze, Hinrich. 1994. A connectionist model of verb subcategorization. In Proceedings of the 16th annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (784–788).
Schwartz, Richard G., & Leonard, Laurence B. 1982. Do children pick and choose? Journal of Child Language 9, 319–336.Google ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, Richard G.; Leonard, Laurence B.; Loeb, Diane M. F.; & Swanson, Lori A. 1987. Attempted sounds are sometimes not: An expanded view of phonological selection and avoidance. Journal of Child Language 14, 411–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scollon, Ronald. 1976. Conversations with a one year old: A case study of the developmental foundation of syntax. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Scollon, Ronald. 1979. A real early stage: An unzippered condensation of a dissertation on child language. In Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. B. (eds.), Developmental pragmatics (215–227). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Searle, John R. 1975. A taxonomy of illocutionary acts. In Gunderson, K. (ed.), Minnesota studies in the philosophy of language (344–369). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Sebastián-Gallés, Núria, & Bosch, Laura. 2002. Building phonotactic knowledge in bilinguals: Role of early exposure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance 28, 974–989.Google ScholarPubMed
Sebastián-Gallés, Núria; Echeverría, Sagrario; & Bosch, Laura. 2005. The influence of early exposure on lexical representation: Comparing early and simultaneous bilinguals. Journal of Memory & Language 52, 240–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidl, Amanda, & Johnson, Elizabeth K. 2006. Infant word segmentation revisited: Edge alignment facilitates target extraction. Developmental Science 9, 565–573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Senghas, Ann, & Coppola, Marie. 2001. Children creating language: How Nicaraguan sign language acquired a spatial grammar. Psychological Science 12, 323–328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Senghas, Ann; Kita, Sotaro; Özyürek, Aslı. 2004. Children creating core properties of language: Evidence from an emerging Sign Language in Nicaragua. Science 305, 1779–1782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Serratrice, Ludovica. 2005. The role of discourse pragmatics in the acquisition of subjects in Italian. Applied Psycholinguistics 26, 437–462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shady, Michele, & Gerken, LouAnn. 1999. Grammatical and caregiver cues in early sentence comprehension. Journal of Child Language 26, 163–175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shatz, Marilyn. 1978a. Children's comprehension of their mothers' question-directions. Journal of Child Language 5, 39–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shatz, Marilyn. 1978b. On the development of communicative understanding: An early strategy for interpreting and responding to messages. Cognitive Psychology 10, 271–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shatz, Marilyn. 1983. Communication. In Flavell, J. H. & Markman, E. M. (eds.), Handbook of child psychology, vol. III: Cognitive development (4th edn) (841–889). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Shatz, Marilyn, & Gelman, Rochel. 1973. The development of communication skills: Modifications in the speech of young children as a function of listener. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 38 (serial no. 152).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shatz, Marilyn, & O'Reilly, Anne W. 1990. Conversation or communicative skill? A reassessment of two-year-olds' behavior in miscommunication episodes. Journal of Child Language 17, 131–146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheldon, Amy. 1974. The role of parallel function in the acquisition of relative clauses in English. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 13, 272–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheldon, Amy. 1990. Pickle fights: Gendered talk in preschool disputes. Discourse Processes 13, 3–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheldon, Amy, & Rohleder, Lisa. 1996. Sharing the same world, telling different stories: Gender differences in co-constructed pretend narratives. In Slobin, D. I., Gerhardt, J., Kyratzis, A., & Guo, J. (eds.), Social interaction, social context, and language: Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (613–632). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sherzer, Joel. 1973. Nonverbal and verbal deixis: The pointed lip gesture among the San Blas Cuna. Language in Society 2, 117–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shi, Rushen; Morgan, James L.; & Allopenna, Paul. 1998. Phonological and acoustic bases for earliest grammatical category assignment: A cross-linguistic perspective. Journal of Child Language 25, 169–201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shibatani, Masayoshi (ed.). 1976. Syntax and semantics, vol. VI: The grammar of causative constructions. New York: Academic Press.
Shipley, Elizabeth F., & Kuhn, Ivy F. 1983. A constraint on comparisons: Equally detailed alternatives. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 35, 195–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shipley, Elizabeth F.; Smith, Carlota S.; & Gleitman, Lila R. 1969. A study in the acquisition of language: Free responses to commands. Language 45, 322–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shirai, Yasuhiro. 1997. Is regularization determined by semantics, or grammar, or both? Comments on Kim, Marcus, Pinker, Hollander, & Coppola 1994. Journal of Child Language 24, 494–501.Google ScholarPubMed
Shirai, Yasuhiro, & Andersen, Roger W. 1995. The acquisition of tense-aspect morphology: A prototype account. Language 71, 743–762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Short-Meyerson, Katherine J., & Abbeduto, Leonard J. 1997. Preschoolers' communication during scripted interactions. Journal of Child Language 24, 469–493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shvachkin, N. Kh. 1973. The development of phonemic perception in early childhood. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds.), Studies of child language development (92–127). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Shwe, Helen, & Markman, Ellen M. 1997. Young children's appreciation of the mental impact of their communicative signals. Developmental Psychology 33, 630–636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegal, Michael. 1997. Knowing children: Experiments in conversation and cognition (2nd edn). Hove, Sussex: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Siegal, Michael; Waters, Lorraine J.; & Dinwiddy, L. Simon. 1988. Misleading children: Causal attributions for inconsistency under repeated questioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 45, 438–456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinclair, John McH., & Coulthard, Robert M. 1975. Towards an analysis of discourse. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Singh, Joseph A. L., & Zingg, Robert M. 1939. Wolf-children and feral man. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Sledd, James. 1988. Product in process: From ambiguities of Standard English to issues that divide us. College English 50, 168–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1970. Universals of grammatical development in children. In d'Arcais, G. B. Flores & Levelt, W. J. M. (eds.), Advances in psycholinguistics (174–186). Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1973. Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds.), Studies of child language development (175–208). New York: Holt Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1978. A case study of early language awareness. In Sinclair, A., Jarvella, R. J., & Levelt, W. J. M. (eds.), The child's conception of language (45–54). New York: Springer Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1979. The role of language in language acquisition. Invited Address, 50th Annual Meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Philadelphia.
Slobin, Dan I. 1981. The origins of grammatical encoding of events. In Deutsch, W. (ed.), The child's construction of language (185–199). London and New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. (ed.). 1985a. The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vols. I–II. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1985b. Crosslinguistic evidence for the Language-Making Capacity. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vol. II (1157–1249). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. (ed.). 1992. The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vol. III. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Slobin, Dan I. 1994. Passives and alternatives in children's narratives in English, Spanish, German, and Turkish. In Fox, B. & Hopper, P. J. (eds.), Voice: Form and function (341–364). Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1996. From “thought and language” to “thinking for speaking.” In Gumperz, J. J. & Levinson, S. C. (eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity (70–96). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. (ed.). 1997. The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vols. IV–V. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Slobin, Dan I. 2001a. Form/function relations: How do children find out what they are? In Bowerman, M. & Levinson, S. C. (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (406–449). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 2001b. The child learns to think for speaking: Puzzles of crosslinguistic diversity in form-meaning mappings. Master lecture, biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, MN.
Slobin, Dan I., & Bever, Thomas G. 1982. Children use canonical sentence schemas: A crosslinguistic study of word order and inflections. Cognition 12, 229–265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slobin, Dan I., & Welsh, Charles. 1973. Elicited imitation as a research tool in developmental psycholinguistics. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds.), Studies of child language development (485–497). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Smith, Bruce L.; McGregor, Karla K.; & Demille, Darcie. 2006. Phonological development in lexically precocious 2-year-olds. Applied Psycholinguistics 27, 355–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Jennifer; Durham, Mercedes; & Fortune, Liane. 2007. “Mam, my trousers is fa'in doon!”: Community, caregiver, and child in the acquisition of variation in a Scottish dialect. Language Variation & Change 19, 63–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Madorah E. 1935. A study of the speech of eight bilingual children of the same family. Child Development 6, 19–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Neilson V. 1973. The acquisition of phonology: A case study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Smoczyńska, Magdalena. 1985. The acquisition of Polish. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vol. I (595–686). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Snow, Catherine. 1972. Mothers' speech to children learning language. Child Development 43, 549–565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, Catherine. 1977. The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language 4, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, Catherine. 1978. The conversational context of language acquisition. In Campbell, R. N. & Smith, P. T. (eds.), Recent advances in the psychology of language (253–269). London: Plenum.Google Scholar
Snow, Catherine; Arlman-Rupp, A.; Hassing, Y.; Jobse, J.; Joosten, J.; & Vorster, J. 1976. Mothers' speech in three social classes. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 5, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, Catherine, & Hoefnagel-Hohle, Marian. 1978. The critical period for language acquisition. Child Development 49, 1114–1128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snyder, Alice D. 1914. Notes on the talk of a two-and-a-half year old boy. Pedagogical Seminary 21, 412–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Song, Hyun-joo, & Fisher, Cynthia. 2005. Who's “she”? Discourse prominence influences preschoolers' comprehension of pronouns. Journal of Memory & Language 52, 29–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sosa, Anna V., & Stoel-Gammon, Carol. 2006. Patterns of intra-word phonological variability during the second year of life. Journal of Child Language 33, 31–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spelke, Elizabeth S.; Gutheil, Grant; & Walle, Gretchen. 1995. The development of object perception. In Kosslyn, S. M. & Osherson, D. N. (eds.), Visual cognition: An invitation to cognitive science, vol. II (297–330). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Spence, Melanie J., & DeCasper, Anthony J. 1987. Prenatal experience with low-frequency maternal-voice sounds influences neonatal perception of maternal voice samples. Infant Behavior & Development 10, 133–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sridhar, Shikaripur N., & Sridhar, Kamal K. 1980. The syntax and psycholinguistics of bilingual code switching. Canadian Journal of Psychology 34, 407–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steels, Luc. 2006. Experiments on the emergence of human communication. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10, 347–349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Clara, & Stern, William. 1928. Die Kindersprache: Eine psychologische und sprachtheoretische Untersuchung (4th rev. edn; 1st edn, 1907). Leipzig: Barth. (Reprinted Darmstadt: Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft, 1965.)Google Scholar
Stern, Daniel N. 1977. The first relationship: Mother and infant. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Stern, Daniel N. 1985. The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Stern, Daniel N.; Spieker, Susan; Barnett, R. K.; & MacKain, Kristine. 1983. The prosody of maternal speech: Infant age and context related changes. Journal of Child Language 10, 1–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, Tassos, & Karmiloff-Smith, Annette. 1997. Word learning in a special population: Do individuals with Williams syndrome obey lexical constraints?Journal of Child Language 24, 737–765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storkel, Holly L. 2002. Restructuring of similarity neighborhoods in the developing mental lexicon. Journal of Child Language 29, 251–274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, Sidney (ed.). 1982. U-shaped behavioral growth. New York: Academic Press.
Strohner, Hans, & Nelson, Keith E. 1974. The young child's development of sentence comprehension: Influence of event probability, nonverbal context, syntactic form, and strategies. Child Development 45, 567–576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strömqvist, Sven. 1984. Make-believe through words: A linguistic study of children's play with a doll's house. PhD dissertation, University of Göteborg, Sweden.
Stromswold, Karin. 1989. How conservative are children? Evidence from auxiliary errors. Papers & Reports on Child Language Development [Stanford University] 28, 148–155.Google Scholar
Stubbs, Michael. 1976. Language, schools, and classrooms. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, Michael. 1987. The phoneme as a perceptuomotor structure. In Allport, A., MacKay, D., Prinz, W., & Scheerer, E. (eds.), Language, perception and production (67–84). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sully, James. 1896. Studies of childhood. New York: Appleton.Google Scholar
Svartvik, Jan. 1966. On voice in the English verb. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Swingley, Daniel. 2005. 11-month-olds' knowledge of how familiar words sound. Developmental Science 8, 432–443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swingley, Daniel, & Aslin, Richard N. 2000. Spoken word recognition and lexical representation in very young children. Cognition 76, 147–166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swingley, Daniel, & Aslin, Richard N. 2002. Lexical neighborhoods and the word-form representations of 14-month-olds. Psychological Science 13, 480–484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swingley, Daniel, & Fernald, Anne. 2002. Recognition of words referring to present and absent objects by 24-month-olds. Journal of Memory & Language 46, 39–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swingley, Daniel; Pinto, John P.; & Fernald, Anne. 1999. Continuous processing in word recognition at 24 months. Cognition 71, 73–108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tabouret-Keller, Andrée. 1964. L'acquisition du langage parlé chez un petit enfant en milieu bilingue. In Ajuriaguerra, J.et al. (eds.), Problèmes de psycholinguistique (205–219). Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Taeschner, Traute. 1983. The sun is feminine. Berlin and New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, Helen. 1982. The development of relative clauses in child speech. Papers & Reports on Child Language Development [Stanford University] 21, 104–111.Google Scholar
Taine, Hippolyte. 1870. Note sur l'acquisition du langage chez les enfants et dans l'espèce humaine. In De l'intelligence. Paris: Hachette.Google Scholar
Talmy, Leonard. 1985. Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms. In Shopen, T. E. (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description, vol. III: Grammatical categories and the lexicon (57–149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, Michael K. 1988. Psycholinguistics: An overview. In Newmeyer, F. J. (ed.), Linguistics: The Cambridge survey, vol. III: Language: Psychological and biological aspects (1–37). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tardif, Twila. 1996. Nouns are not always learned before verbs: Evidence from Mandarin speakers' early vocabularies. Developmental Psychology 32, 492–504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tardif, Twila; Shatz, Marilyn; & Naigles, Letitia. 1997. Caregiver speech and children's use of nouns versus verbs: A comparison of English, Italian, and Mandarin. Journal of Child Language 24, 535–565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Templin, Mildred C. 1957. Certain language skills in children: Their development and interrelationships. Institute of Child Welfare Monographs 26. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Thatcher, Robert W., & John, E. Roy. 1977. Functional neuroscience, vol. I: Foundations of cognitive processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Theakston, Anna L.; Lieven, Elena V. M.; Pine, Julian M.; & Rowland, Caroline F. 2001. The role of performance limitations in the acquisition of verb-argument structure: An alternative account. Journal of Child Language 28, 127–152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, Esther, & Smith, Linda B. 1994. A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thierry, Guillaume; Vihman, Marilyn; & Roberts, Mark. 2003. Familiar words capture the attention of 11-month-olds in less than 250 ms. NeuroReport 14, 2307–2310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thoermer, Claudia, & Sodian, Beate. 2001. Preverbal infants' understanding of referential gestures. First Language 21, 245–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, Michael S. C.; Grant, Julia; Barham, Zita; Gsödl, Marisa; Laing, Emma; Lakusta, Laura; Tyler, Lorraine K.; Grice, Sarah; Paterson, Sarah; & Karmiloff-Smith, Annette. 2001. Past tense formation in Williams syndrome. Language & Cognitive Processes 16, 143–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, Michael S. C., & Johnson, Mark H. 2006. The computational modeling of sensitive periods. Developmental Psychobiology 48, 337–344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, Michael S. C., & Karmiloff-Smith, Annette. 2003. Modeling language acquisition in atypical phenotypes. Psychological Review 110, 647–682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, Sandra A. 2002. “Object” complements and conversation: Towards a realistic account. Studies in Language 26, 125–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomson, Jean R., & Chapman, Robin S. 1977. “Who is daddy?” revisited: The status of two-year-olds' over-extended words in use and comprehension. Journal of Child Language 4, 359–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. 1992. First verbs: A case study of early grammatical development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. 1995. Joint attention as social cognition. In Moore, C. & Dunham, P. J. (eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. 2000. Do young children have adult syntactic competence?Cognition 74, 209–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, & Akhtar, Nameera. 1995. Two-year-olds use pragmatic cues to differentiate reference to objects and actions. Cognitive Development 10, 201–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael; Akhtar, Nameera; Dodson, Kelly; & Rekau, Laura. 1997. Differential productivity in young children's use of nouns and verbs. Journal of Child Language 24, 373–387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, Michael, & Barton, Michelle E. 1994. Learning words in nonostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology 30, 639–650.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, & Brooks, Patricia J. 1999. Early syntactic development: A construction grammar approach. In Barrett, M. (ed.), The development of language (161–189). Hove, Sussex: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael; Call, Josep; & Gluckman, Andrea. 1997. Comprehension of novel communicative signs by apes and human children. Child Development 68, 1067–1080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, Michael; Farrar, Michael J.; & Dines, Jennifer. 1984. Children's speech revisions for a familiar and an unfamiliar adult. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research 27, 359–363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, Michael, & Kruger, Ann C. 1992. Joint attention on actions: Acquiring verbs in ostensive and non-ostensive contexts. Journal of Child Language 19, 311–334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, Michael; Strosberg, Randi; & Akhtar, Nameera. 1996. Eighteen-month-old children learn words in non-ostensive contexts. Journal of Child Language 23, 157–176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trehub, Sandra E. 1973. Infants' sensitivity to vowel and tonal contrasts. Developmental Psychology 9, 91–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1977. Descriptive analyses of infant communication behaviour. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction (227–270). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tversky, Amos. 1977. Features of similarity. Psychological Review 84, 327–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, Barbara G., & Hemenway, Katherine. 1984. Objects, parts, and categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 113, 169–193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyack, Dorothy L., & Ingram, David. 1977. Children's production and comprehension of questions. Journal of Child Language 4, 211–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, Lorraine K.; Karmiloff-Smith, Annette; Voice, J. Kate; Stevens, Tassos; Grant, Julia; Udwin, Orlee; Davies, Mark; & Howlin, Patricia. 1997. Do individuals with Williams syndrome have bizarre semantics? Evidence for lexical organization using an on-line task. Cortex 33, 515–527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ungerleider, Leslie G. 1995. Functional brain imaging studies of cortical mechanisms for memory. Science 270, 769–775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
,U.S. Census 2000 Brief. 2003, October. Language use and English-speaking ability: 2000. U.S. Census Bureau, U.S. Department of Commerce.Google Scholar
Valian, Virginia. 1986. Syntactic categories in the speech of young children. Developmental Psychology 22, 562–579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valian, Virginia. 1991. Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children. Cognition 40, 21–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valian, Virginia, & Casey, Lyman. 2003. Young children's acquisition of wh-questions: The role of structured input. Journal of Child Language 30, 117–143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valian, Virginia; Hoeffner, James; & Aubry, Stephanie. 1996. Young children's imitation of sentence subjects: Evidence of processing limitations. Developmental Psychology 32, 153–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wal, Sjoukje. 1996. Negative polarity items and negation: Tandem acquisition. Groningen: Dissertations in Linguistics 17, University of Groningen.
Geert, Paul. 1991. A dynamic systems model of cognitive and language growth. Psychological Review 98, 3–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velten, H. V. 1943. The growth of phonemic and lexical patterns in infant language. Language 19, 281–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vendler, Zeno. 1967. Linguistics in philosophy. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Veneziano, Edy. 1985. “Replying” to mothers' questions: A way to lexical acquisition. Journal of Pragmatics 9, 433–452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veneziano, Edy. 1988. Vocal-verbal interaction and the construction of early lexical knowledge. In Smith, M. D. & Locke, J. L. (eds.), The emergent lexicon: The child's development of a linguistic vocabulary (109–147). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Veneziano, Edy, & Sinclair, Hermine. 2000. The changing status of “filler syllables” on the way to grammatical morphemes. Journal of Child Language 27, 461–500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veneziano, Edy; Sinclair, Hermine; & Berthoud, Ioanna. 1990. From one word to two words: Repetition patterns on the way to structured speech. Journal of Child Language 17, 633–650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhoeven, Ludo. 2007. Early bilingualism, language transfer, and phonological awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics 28, 425–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verkuyl, Henk J. 1993. A theory of aspectuality: The interaction between temporal and atemporal structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, Marilyn M. 1980. Formulas in first and second language acquisition. Papers & Reports on Child Language Development [Stanford University] 18, 75–92.Google Scholar
Vihman, Marilyn M. 1982. The acquisition of morphology by a bilingual child: The whole-word approach. Applied Psycholinguistics 3, 141–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, Marilyn M. 1985. Language differentiation by the bilingual infant. Journal of Child Language 12, 297–324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vihman, Marilyn M. 1996. Phonological development: The origins of language in the child. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Vihman, Marilyn M.; Thierry, Guillaume; Lum, Jarrad; Keren-Portnoy, Tamar; & Martin, Pam. 2007. Onset of word form recognition in English, Welsh, and English-Welsh bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics 28, 475–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vogel, Irene. 1975. One system or two? An analysis of a two-year-old Romanian-English bilingual's phonology. Papers & Reports on Child Language Development [Stanford University] 9, 43–62.Google Scholar
Volterra, Virginia. 1972. Il “no”: Prime fasi dello sviluppo della negazione nel linguaggio infantile. Archivio de Psicologia, Neurologia e Psichiatria 33, 16–53.Google Scholar
Volterra, Virginia, & Taeschner, Traute. 1978. The acquisition and development of language by a bilingual child. Journal of Child Language 5, 311–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, Dale; Greenwood, Charles; Hart, Betty; & Carta, Judith. 1994. Prediction of school outcomes based on early language production and socioeconomic factors. Child Development 65, 606–621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, Marilyn A. 1996. Inferring acceptance and rejection in dialog by default rules of inference. Language & Speech 39, 265–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wannemacher, Jill T., & Ryan, Mary L. 1978. Less is not more: A study of children's comprehension of less in various task contexts. Child Development 49, 660–668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warden, David A. 1976. The influence of context on children's use of identifying expressions and references. British Journal of Psychology 67, 101–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warden, David A. 1981. Learning to identify referents. British Journal of Psychology 72, 93–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waterson, Natalie. 1971. Child phonology: A prosodic view. Journal of Linguistics 7, 179–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson-Gegeo, Karen A., & Gegeo, David W. 1986a. The social world of Kwara'ae children: Acquisition of language and values. In Cook-Gumperz, J., Corsaro, W. A., & Streeck, J. (eds.), Children's worlds and children's language (107–127). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Watson-Gegeo, Karen A., & Gegeo, David W. 1986b. Calling-out and repeating routines in Kwara'ae children's language socialization. In Schieffelin, B. B. & Ochs, E. (eds.), Language socialization across cultures (17–50). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Watt, W. C. (ed.). 1994. Writing systems and cognition: Perspectives from psychology, physiology, linguistics, and semiotics. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRef
Waxman, Sandra R., & Booth, Amy E. 2001. Seeing pink elephants: Fourteen-month-olds' interpretations of novel nouns and adjectives. Cognitive Psychology 43, 217–242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waxman, Sandra R., & Hatch, Thomas. 1992. Beyond the basics: Preschool children label objects flexibly at multiple hierarchical levels. Journal of Child Language 19, 153–166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waxman, Sandra R., & Senghas, Ann. 1992. Relations among word meanings in early lexical development. Developmental Psychology 28, 862–873.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weber-Fox, Christine M., & Neville, Helen J. 1996. Maturational constraints on functional specializations for language processing: ERP and behavioral evidence in bilingual speakers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 8, 231–256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinreich, Uriel. 1953. Languages in contact. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Weir, Ruth. 1962. Language in the crib. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Weisenburger, Janet L. 1976. A choice of words: Two-year-old speech from a situational point of view. Journal of Child Language 3, 275–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, Deborah M., & Sachs, Jacqueline. 1991. Persuasive strategies used by preschool children. Discourse Processes 14, 55–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weissenborn, Jürgen. 1986. Learning how to become an interlocutor: The verbal negotiation of common frames of reference and actions in dyads of 7–14 year old children. In Cook-Gumperz, J., Corsaro, W. A., & Streeck, J. (eds.), Children's worlds and children's language (377–404). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Wellman, Henry M., & Lempers, Jacques D. 1977. The naturalistic communicative abilities of two-year-olds. Child Development 48, 1052–1057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, Gordon. 1985. Language development in the pre-school years. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wells, Gordon, & Mongomery, Martin. 1981. Adult-child interaction at home and at school. In French, P. & Maclure, M. (eds.), Adult-child conversation (210–243). London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Werker, Janet F., & Lalonde, Chris E. 1988. Cross-language speech perception: Initial capabilities and developmental change. Developmental Psychology 24, 672–683.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, Janet F., & McLeod, Peter J. 1989. Infant preference for both male- and female-infant-directed talk: A developmental study of attentional and affective responsiveness. Canadian Journal of Higher Education 19, 29–41.Google Scholar
Werker, Janet F.; Pegg, Judith E.; & McLeod, Peter J. 1994. A cross-language investigation of infant preference for infant-directed communication. Infant Behavior & Development 17, 323–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, Janet F., & Tees, Richard C. 1984. Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior & Development 7, 49–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werner, Heinz, & Kaplan, Bernard. 1963. Symbol formation: An organismic-developmental approach to language and the expression of thought. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wernicke, Carl. 1874. Der aphasische Symptomencomplex: Eine psychologische Studie auf anatomischer Basis. Breslan: Cohn & Weigert.Google Scholar
White, Lydia, & Genesee, Fred. 1996. How native is near-native? The issue of ultimate attainment in adult second language acquisition. Second Language Research 12, 238–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wieman, Leslie A. 1976. Stress patterns of early child language. Journal of Child Language 3, 283–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wigglesworth, Gillian. 1997. Children's individual approaches to the organization of narrative. Journal of Child Language 24, 279–309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilcox, Stephen, & Palermo, David S. 1974/1975. “In,” “on,” and “under” revisited. Cognition 3, 245–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiley, Angela. 1997. Religious affiliation as a source of variation in child rearing values and parental regulation of young children. Mind, Culture, & Activity 4, 86–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkins, David. 2003. Why pointing with the index finger is not a universal (in socio-cultural and semiotic terms). In Kita, S. (ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (171–215). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, Louise C.; Calculator, Steven; & Dollaghan, Christine. 1982. Ya wanna trade – Just for awhile: Children's requests and responses to peers. Discourse Processes 5, 161–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willes, Mary. 1981. Learning to take part in classroom interaction. In French, P. & Maclure, M. (eds.), Adult-child conversation (73–95). London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Winitz, Harris, & Irwin, Orvis C. 1958. Syllabic and phonetic structure of infants' early words. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research 1, 250–256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Witelson, Sandra F., & Pallie, W. 1973. Left hemisphere specialization for language in the newborn. Brain 96, 641–646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittek, Angelika, & Tomasello, Michael. 2005a. German-speaking children's productivity with syntactic constructions and case morphology: Local cues act locally. First Language 25, 103–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittek, Angelika, & Tomasello, Michael. 2005b. Young children's sensitivity to listener knowledge and perceptual context in choosing referring expressions. Applied Psycholinguistics 26, 541–558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong Fillmore, Lily. 1979. Individual differences in second language acquisition. In Fillmore, C. J., Kempler, D., & Wang, W. S.-Y. (eds.), Individual differences in language ability and language behavior (203–228). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong Fillmore, Lily. 1991. When learning a second language means losing the first. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 6, 323–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong Fillmore, Lily. 1996. What happens when languages are lost? An essay on language assimilation and cultural identity. In Slobin, D. I., Gerhardt, J., Kyratzis, A., & Guo, J. (eds.), Social interaction, social context, and language: Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (435–446). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Woodward, Amanda L. 1992. The role of the whole object assumption in early word learning. PhD dissertation, Stanford University.
Woodward, Amanda L. 2003. Infants' developing understanding of the link between looker and object. Developmental Science 6, 297–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodward, Amanda L., & Guajardo, Jose J. 2002. Infants' understanding of the point gesture as an object-directed action. Cognitive Development 17, 1061–1084.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodward, Amanda L., & Markman, Ellen M. 1991. Constraints on learning as default assumptions: Comments on Merriman & Bowman's “The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning.”Developmental Review 14, 57–77.Google Scholar
Xu, Fei, & Tenenbaum, Joshua B. 2007a. Word learning as Bayesian inference. Psychological Review 114, 245–272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, Fei, & Tenenbaum, Joshua B. 2007b. Sensitivity to sampling in Bayesian word learning. Developmental Science 10, 288–297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yip, Virginia, & Matthews, Stephen. 2007. The bilingual child: Early development and language contact. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Youssef, Valerie. 1991a. The acquisition of varilingual competence. English World-Wide 12, 87–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Youssef, Valerie. 1991b. Variation as a feature of language acquisition in the Trinidad context. Language Variation & Change 3, 75–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Youssef, Valerie. 1993. Children's linguistic choices: Audience design and societal norms. Language in Society 22, 257–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yumitani, Chutatip Chiraporn. 1998. The acquisition of the causative alternation in Thai. In Clark, E. V. (ed.), Proceedings of the 29th Child Language Research Forum 1997 (141–149). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Zager, David. 1981. A real time process model of morphological change. PhD dissertation, State University of New York, Buffalo.
Zajonc, Robert B. 1976. Family configuration and intelligence. Science 192, 227–236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zajonc, Robert B., & Mullally, Patricia R. 1997. Birth order: Reconciling conflicting effects. American Psychologist (July), 685–699.
Zangl, Renate, & Mills, Debra L. 2007. Increased brain activity to infant-directed speech in 6- and 13-month-old infants. Infancy 11, 31–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zentella, Ana Celia. 1997. Growing up bilingual: Puerto Rican children in New York. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Zukow, Patricia G. 1986. The relationship between interaction with the caregiver and the emergence of play activities during the one-word period. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 4, 223–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zukow-Goldring, Patricia, & Rader, Nancy de V. 2001. Perceiving referring actions. Developmental Science 4, 28–30.Google Scholar
Zurer Pearson, Barbara. 2007. Social factors in childhood bilingualism in the United States. Applied Psycholinguistics 28, 399–410.Google Scholar
Zurer Pearson, Barbara; Fernández, Silvia C.; & Oller, D. Kimbrough. 1995. Cross-language synonyms in the lexicons of bilingual infants: One language or two? Journal of Child Language 22, 345–368.Google Scholar
Zwitserlood, Pienie; Kellerman, Eric; Klein, Wolfgang; Liang, James; & Perdue, Clive. 2002. The first minutes of foreign-language exposure. Unpublished MS, Department of Psychology, University of Münster.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Eve V. Clark, Stanford University, California
  • Book: First Language Acquisition
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511806698.022
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Eve V. Clark, Stanford University, California
  • Book: First Language Acquisition
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511806698.022
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Eve V. Clark, Stanford University, California
  • Book: First Language Acquisition
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511806698.022
Available formats
×