Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T17:59:20.726Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 September 2017

Andrew S. Gordon
Affiliation:
University of Southern California
Jerry R. Hobbs
Affiliation:
University of Southern California
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
A Formal Theory of Commonsense Psychology
How People Think People Think
, pp. 557 - 566
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Carlos E., Alchourrón, Peter, Gärdenfors, and David, Makinson. On the logic of theory change: Partial meet contraction and revision functions. Journal of Symbolic Logic, 50(2):510–530, 1985.Google Scholar
James, Allen and George, Ferguson. Actions and events in interval temporal logic. In O., Stock, editor, Spatial and Temporal Reasoning, pp. 205–245. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer Academic, 1997.
James F., Allen. Maintaining knowledge about temporal intervals. Communications of the ACM, 26(11):832–843, 1983.Google Scholar
James F., Allen and Henry A., Kautz. A model of naive temporal reasoning. In Jerry R., Hobbs and Robert C., Moore (eds.), Formal Theories of the Commonsense World, pp. 251–268. Norwood, NJ:Ablex, 1985.
Michael C., Anderson and Collin, Green. Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control. Nature, 410(6826):366–369, 2001.Google Scholar
Janet W., Astington. The Child's Discovery of the Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1993.
Angeliki, Athanasiadou and Elzbieta, Tabakowska. Speaking of Emotions: Conceptualisation and Expression. Berlin:Walter de Gruyter, 1998.
Jeremy, Avis and Paul L., Harris. Belief-desire reasoning among Baka children: Evidence for a universal conception of mind. Child Development, 62(3):460–467, 1991.Google Scholar
Emmon, Bach. On time, tense, and aspect: An essay in Englipagessh metaphysics. In P., Cole (ed.), Radical Pragmatics, pp. 63–81. New York:Academic Press, 1981.
Simon, Baron-Cohen, Alan M., Leslie, and Uta, Frith. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1):37–46, 1985.Google Scholar
Karen, Bartsch and Henry M., Wellman. Children Talk About the Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1995.
Lawrence, Birnbaum. Rigor mortis: A response to Nilsson's “Logic and Artificial Intelligence.” Artificial Intelligence, 47(1):57–77, 1991.Google Scholar
Paul, Bloom and Tim P., German. Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory of mind. Cognition, 77(1):B25–B31, 2000.Google Scholar
Ronen I., Brafman and Carmel, Domshlak. Preference handling—an introductory tutorial. AI Magazine, 30(1):58–86, 2009.Google Scholar
Michael E., Bratman. Intention, Plans, and Practical Reason. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1987.
Donald, Brown. Human Universals. New York:McGraw-Hill, 1991.
Jerome R., Busemeyer and Joseph G., Johnson. Computationalmodels of decision making. Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making, pp. 133–154. Malden, MA: Blackwell, 2004.
Josep, Call and Michael, Tomasello. A nonverbal false belief task: The performance of children and great apes. Child Development, 70(2):381–395, 1999.Google Scholar
Josep, Call and Michael, Tomasello. Does the chimpanzee have a theory ofmind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5):187–192, 2008.Google Scholar
David J., Chalmers. Foundations. In David J., Chalmers (ed.), Philosophy ofMind: Classical and Contemporary Readings, pp. 1–9. Oxford:Oxford University Press, 2002.
Paul M., Churchland. Eliminativematerialism and the propositional attitudes. The Journal of Philosophy, 78(2): 67–90, 1981.Google Scholar
Paul M., Churchland. Scientific Realism and the Plasticity of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1986.
Herbert H., Clark. Arenas of Language Use. Chicago:University of Chicago Press, 1992.
John, Clement. A conceptual model discussed by Galileo and used intuitively by physics students. In Dedre, Gentner and A., Stevens (eds.), Mental Models. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1983.
Paul, Cohen and Hector, Levesque. Intention is choice with commitment. Artificial Intelligence, 42:213–261, 1990.Google Scholar
Paul J., Cohen. Set Theory and the Continuum Hypothesis. New York: W. A. Benjamin, 1966.
Philip R., Cohen and C. Raymond, Perrault. Elements of a plan-based theory of speech acts. Cognitive Science, 3:177–212, 1979.Google Scholar
Gregg Clinton, Collins. Plan Creation: Using Strategies as Blueprints. PhD thesis, Yale University, 1987.
Blandine, Courtois. Dictionnaires électroniques DELAF anglais et français. In C., Leclère, É., Laporte, M., Piot, and M., Silberztein (eds.), Lexique, Syntaxe et Lexique-Grammaire/ Syntax, Lexis and Lexicon-Grammar, Papers in Honor of Maurice Gross, pp. 113–123. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing, 2004.
William, Croft and D. A., Cruse. Cognitive Linguistics (Cambridge Textbooks in Linguistics). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2004.
Donald, Davidson. The logical form of action sentences. In N., Rescher (ed.), The Logic of Decision and Action, pp. 81–95. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press, 1967.
Ernest, Davis. Inferring ignorance from the locality of visual perception. In Proceedings of the Seventh National Conference on Artificial Intelligence, pp. 786–790. Palo Alto, CA: AAAI Press, 1988.
Ernest, Davis. Reasoning about hand–eye coordination. In Proceedings of the IJCAI-89 Workshop on Knowledge, Perception, and Planning. San Francisco, CA:Morgan Kaufmann, 1989.
Ernest, Davis. The kinematics of cutting solid objects. Annals of Mathematics and Artificial Intelligence, 9(3–4):253–305, 1993.Google Scholar
Ernest, Davis. Knowledge preconditions for plans. Journal of Logic and Computation, 4(5):721–766, 1994.Google Scholar
Ernest, Davis. Naive physics perplex. AI Magazine, 19(4):51, 1998.Google Scholar
Ernest, Davis. Knowledge and communication:A first-order theory. Artificial Intelligence, 166(1):81–139, 2005.Google Scholar
Ernest, Davis and Leora, Morgenstern. Introduction: Progress in formal commonsense reasoning. Artificial Intelligence, 153(1):1–12, 2004.Google Scholar
Ernest, Davis and Leora, Morgenstern. A first-order theory of communication and multiagent plans. Journal of Logic and Computation, 15(5):701–749, 2005.Google Scholar
Daniel C., Dennett. The Intentional Stance. Cambridge,MA: Bradford Books, 1989.
Daniel C., Dennett. Two contrasts: Folk craft versus folk science, and belief versus opinion. In John D., Greenwood (ed.), The Future of Folk Psychology: Intentionality and Cognitive Science, pp. 135–148. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1991.
René, Descartes. Meditations on first philosophy. In David J., Chalmers (ed.), Philosophy of Mind: Classical and Contemporary Readings, pp. 10–21. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2002.
Eric Andrew, Domeshek. Do the Right Thing:A Component Theory for Indexing Stories as Social Advice. PhD thesis, Yale University, 1992.
Jon, Doyle. A truth maintenance system. In G. F., Luger (ed.), Computational Intelligence, pp. 529–554. Palo Alto, CA: AAAI Press, 1995.
Rolf A., Eberle. A logic of believing, knowing, and inferring. Synthese, 26:356–382, 1974.Google Scholar
Albert, Einstein and Leopold, Infeld. The Evolution of Physics:The Growth of Ideas From Early Concepts to Relativity and Quanta. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1938.
Ronald, Fagin and Joseph Y., Halpern. Belief, awareness, and limited reasoning. Artificial Intelligence, 34:39–76, 1988.Google Scholar
Ronald, Fagin, Jeffrey D., Ullman, and Moshe Y., Vardi. On the semantics of updates in databases. In Proceedings of the 2nd ACM SIGACT-SIGMOD Symposium on Principles of Database Systems,pp. 352–365. New York: Association for Computer Machinery, 1983.
Brian, Falkenhainer, Kenneth D., Forbus, and Dedre, Gentner. The structure-mapping engine: Algorithm and examples. Artificial Intelligence, 41(1):1–63, 1989.Google Scholar
Christiane, Fellbaum. WordNet. Wiley Online Library, 1998.
Richard E., Fikes and Nils J., Nilsson. Strips:Anew approach to the application of theorem proving to problem solving. Artificial Intelligence, 2(3-4):189–208, 1971.Google Scholar
Garth, Fletcher. The Scientific Credibility of Folk Psychology. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1995.
Jerry A, Fodor. Psychosemantics: The Problem of Meaning in the Philosophy of Mind. Cambridge, MA:MIT Press, 1987.
Jerry A., Fodor and Zenon W., Pylyshyn. Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis. Cognition, 28(1):3–71, 1988.Google Scholar
Dedre, Gentner. Structure-mapping:A theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science, 7(2):155–170, 1983.Google Scholar
Dedre, Gentner and Kenneth D., Forbus. MAC/FAC:Amodel of similarity-based retrieval. In Proceedings of the Thirteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Volume 504, p. 509. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1991.
Dedre, Gentner and Arthur B., Markman. Structure mapping in analogy and similarity. American Psychologist, 52(1):45, 1997.Google Scholar
Edmund L., Gettier. Is justified true belief knowledge? Analysis, 23(6):121–123, 1963.Google Scholar
Alvin I., Goldman. Interpretation psychologized. Mind & Language, 4(3):161–185, 1989.Google Scholar
Alison, Gopnik and Andrew N., Meltzoff.Words, Thoughts, and Theories. Cambridge,MA: MIT Press, 1997.
Alison, Gopnik and Henry M., Wellman. Why the child's theory of mind really is a theory. Mind & Language, 7(1-2):145–171, 1992.Google Scholar
Andrew S., Gordon. Strategies in analogous planning cases. In Proceedings, Twenty- Third Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 370–375. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 2001.
Andrew S., Gordon. The representation of planning strategies. Artificial Intelligence, 153(1):287–305, 2004a.Google Scholar
Andrew S., Gordon. Strategy Representation: An Analysis of Planning Knowledge. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2004b.
Andrew S., Gordon. Language evidence for changes in a theory of mind. In Michael A., Arbib (ed.), Action to Language via the Mirror Neuron System, pp. 374–393. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2006.
Andrew S., Gordon and Jerry R., Hobbs. Coverage and competency in formal theories: A commonsense theory of memory. In Proceedings of the 2003 AAAI Spring Symposium on Logical Formalizations of Commonsense Reasoning, pp. 24–26. Palo Alto,CA: AAAI Press, 2003.
Andrew S., Gordon and Jerry R., Hobbs. Formalizations of commonsense psychology. AI Magazine, 25:49–62, 2004.Google Scholar
Andrew S., Gordon and Jerry R., Hobbs. A commonsense theory of mind-body interaction. In Proceedings of the 2011 AAAI Spring Symposium on Logical Formalizations of Commonsense Reasoning, pp. 36–41. Palo Alto, CA: AAAI Press, 2011.
Andrew S., Gordon and Anish, Nair. Literary evidence for the cultural development of a theory of mind. In Proceedings of the 25th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 468–473. Boston, MA: The Cognitive Science Society, 2003.
Andrew S., Gordon and Anish, Nair. Expressions related to knowledge and belief in children's speech. In Proceedings of the 26th AnnualMeeting of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 476–481. Boston, MA: The Cognitive Science Society, 2004.
Andrew S., Gordon, Abe, Kazemzadeh, Anish, Nair, and Milena, Petrova. Recognizing expressions of commonsense psychology in English text. In Proceedings of the 41st Annual Meeting on Association for Computational Linguistics-Volume 1, pp. 208–215. Association for Computational Linguistics, 2003.
Robert M., Gordon. Folk psychology as simulation. Mind & Language, 1(2):158–171, 1986.Google Scholar
Delia, Graff. Shifting sands:An interest-relative theory of vagueness. Philosophical Topics, 28(1):45–81, 2000.Google Scholar
Jonathan, Gratch. Emile: Marshalling passions in training and education. In Proceedings of the Fourth International Conference on Autonomous Agents, pp. 325–332. New York: ACM, 2000.
John D., Greenwood, ed. The Future of Folk Psychology: Intentionality and Cognitive Science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1991.
Jean, Harkins and Anna, Wierzbicka. Emotions in Crosslinguistic Perspective, Volume 17. Berlin:Walter de Gruyter, 2001.
Paul L., Harris. Children and Emotion: The Development of Psychological Understanding. Oxford: Basil Blackwell, 1989.
Patrick J, Hayes. Naive physics I: Ontology for liquids. In Jerry R., Hobbs and Robert C., Moore (eds.), Formal Theories of the Commonsense World, pp. 71–108. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, 1985.
Jane, Heal. Replication and functionalism. In J., Butterfield (ed.), Language, Mind, and Logic. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1986.
Fritz, Heider. The Psychology of InterpersonalRelations. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1958.
Fritz, Heider and Marianne, Simmel. An experimental study of apparent behavior. The American Journal of Psychology, 57(2):243–259, 1944.Google Scholar
Jaakko, Hintikka. Knowledge and Belief. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press, 1962.
Thomas, Hobbes. Leviathan. www.gutenberg.org, 1651.
Jerry R., Hobbs. Coherence and coreference. Cognitive Science, 3:67–90, 1979.Google Scholar
Jerry R., Hobbs. An improper treatment of quantification in ordinary English. In Proceedings of the 21st Annual Meeting on Association for Computational Linguistics, pp. 57–63. Association for Computational Linguistics, 1983.
Jerry R., Hobbs. Ontological promiscuity. In Proceedings of the 23rd Annual Meeting on Association for Computational Linguistics, pp. 60–69. Association for Computational Linguistics, 1985.
Jerry R., Hobbs. Monotone decreasing quantifiers in a scope-free logical form. In K. van, Deemter and S., Peters (eds.), Semantic Ambiguity and Underspecification, pp. 55–76. Stanford, CA, CSLI Publications, 1995.
Jerry R., Hobbs. The logical notation: Ontological promiscuity, 1998. www.isi.edu/ hobbs/disinf-tc.html. Chapter 2 of Discourse and Inference.
Jerry R., Hobbs. Half orders of magnitude. In Papers from the KR-2000 Workshop on Semantic Approximation, Granularity, and Vagueness, pp. 28–38, 2000.Google Scholar
Jerry R., Hobbs. An ontology of information structure. In Proceedings of the 7th International Symposium on Logical Formalizations of Commonsense Reasoning, Corfu, Greece, pp. 99–106, 2005a. Available at http://commonsensereasoning.org/2005/Google Scholar
Jerry R., Hobbs. Toward a useful concept of causality for lexical semantics. Journal of Semantics, 22(2):181–209, 2005b.Google Scholar
Jerry R., Hobbs and Andrew S., Gordon. Encoding knowledge of commonsense psychology. In 7th International Symposium on Logical.org/2005/ Formalizations of Commonsense Reasoning, May 22–24, 2005, Corfu, Greece, 2005. Available at http:// commonsensereasoning
Jerry R., Hobbs and Andrew S., Gordon. The deep lexical semantics of emotions. In Workshop on Sentiment Analysis: Emotion, Metaphor, Ontology and Terminology (EMOT-08), 6th International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC-08), Marrakech, Morocco, 2008. Available at www.lrec-conf.org/proceedings/lrec2008/
Jerry R., Hobbs and Andrew S., Gordon. Goals in a formal theory of commonsense psychology. In Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Formal Ontology in Information Systems (FOIS-2010), Toronto, Canada, pp. 59–72. Amsterdam: IOS Press, 2010.
Jerry R., Hobbs and Andrew S., Gordon. Axiomatizing complex concepts from fundamentals (invited paper). In Conference on Intelligent Text Processing and Computational Linguistics (CICLing 2014), Kathmandu, Nepal, pp. 351–365. Heidelberg, Germany: Springer, 2014.
Jerry R., Hobbs and Andrew, Kehler. A theory of parallelism and the case of vp ellipsis. In Proceedings, 35th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics, Madrid, Spain, July 1997, pp. 394–401. San Francisco: Morgan Kaufmann, 1997.
Jerry R., Hobbs and Vladik, Kreinovich. Optimal choice of granularity in commonsense estimation:Why half-orders of magnitude. In IFSAWorld Congress and 20th NAFIPS International Conference, 2001. Joint 9th, Volume 3, pp. 1343–1348. IEEE, 2001.
Jerry R., Hobbs and Rutu, Mulkar-Mehta. Toward as formal theory of information structure. In Bernd-Olaf, Küppers, Udo, Hahn, and Stefan, Artmann (eds.), Evolution of Semantic Systems, pp. 101–126. Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 2013.
Jerry R., Hobbs and Feng, Pan. An ontology of time for the semantic web. ACM Transactions on Asian Language Information Processing (TALIP), 3(1):66–85, 2004.
Jerry R., Hobbs, Mark E., Stickel, Douglas E., Appelt, and Paul, Martin. Interpretation as abduction. Artificial Intelligence, 63(1–2):69–142, October 1993.Google Scholar
Jerry R., Hobbs, Alicia, Sagae, and Suzanne, Wertheim. Toward a commonsense theory of microsociology: Interpersonal relationships. In Proceedings of the Seventh International Conference (FOIS 2012), Graz, Austria, pp. 249–262. Amsterdam: IOS Press, 2012.
Keith J., Holyoak and Paul, Thagard. Analogical mapping by constraint satisfaction. Cognitive Science, 13(3):295–355, 1989.Google Scholar
Thomas H., Huxley. On the hypothesis that animals are automata, and its history. Fortnightly Review, 16:555–580, 1874.Google Scholar
International Organization for Standardization. Common logic (CL):A framework for a family of logic-based languages. ISO/IEC IS, 24707, 2007.
William, James. The Principles of Psychology. New York:Henry Holt and Company, 1890.
Julian, Jaynes. The Origin of Consciousness in the Breakdown of the Bicameral Brain. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin, 1976.
Edward E., Jones and Keith E., Davis. A theory of correspondent inferences: From acts to dispositions. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 2:219–66, 1965.Google Scholar
Edward E., Jones and Victor A., Harris. The attribution of attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 3(1):1–24, 1967.Google Scholar
Edward E., Jones and Richard E., Nisbett. The Actor and the Observer:Divergent Perceptions of the Causes of Behavior. Morristown, NJ:General Learning Press, 1971.
Eric, Jones. The Flexible Use of Abstract Knowledge in Planning. PhD thesis, Yale University, 1992.
Alex, Kass. Developing Creative Hypotheses by Adapting Explanations. PhD thesis, Yale University, 1990.
Daniel, Kayser and Farid, Nouioua. From the textual description of an accident to its causes. Artificial Intelligence, 173(12):1154–1193, 2009.Google Scholar
Harold H., Kelley. Attribution theory in social psychology. In Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.University of Nebraska Press, pp. 192–238, 1967.Google Scholar
Ami, Klin, Warren, Jones, Robert, Schultz, and Fred, Volkmar. The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition:Lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of theRoyal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 358(1430):345–360, 2003.Google Scholar
Kurt, Konolige and Martha E., Pollack. A representationalist theory of intention. In International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence, pp. 390–395. San Francisco:Morgan Kaufmann, 1993.
George, Lakoff and Mark, Johnson. Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 2008.
Ellen J., Langer. The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32 (2):311, 1975.Google Scholar
David, Leake. Evaluating Explanations. PhD thesis, Yale University, 1990.
Douglas B., Lenat. Cyc:A large-scale investment in knowledge infrastructure. Communications of the ACM, 38(11):33–38, 1995.Google Scholar
Douglas B., Lenat and Ramanathan V., Guha. Building Large Knowledge Bases. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley, 1990.
Hector J., Levesque. A logic of implicit and explicit belief. In Proceedings of the National Conference on Artificial Intelligence (AAAI'84), pp. 198–202. Palo Alto, CA: AAAI Press, 1984.
Beth, Levin. English Verb Classes and Alternations:A Preliminary Investigation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1993.
David K., Lewis. Counterfactuals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1973.
Angeline, Lillard. Ethnopsychologies: cultural variations in theories of mind. Psychological Bulletin, 123(1):3, 1998.Google Scholar
Brian, MacWhinney. The CHILDES project: The Database, Volume 2. New York: Psychology Press, 2000.
Bertram F., Malle. The actor-observer asymmetry in attribution: A (surprising) metaanalysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6):895, 2006.Google Scholar
John C., McCarthy. Programs with common sense. In Proceedings of the Teddington Conference on the Mechanization of Thought Processes, pp. 75–91, London: Her Majesty's Stationary Office, 1959.
John C., McCarthy. Epistemological problems of artificial intelligence. In Proceedings, International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence, pp. 1038–1044. San Francisco: Morgan Kaufmann, August 1977.
John C., McCarthy. Ascribing mental qualities to machines. In Martin, Ringle (ed.), Philosophical Perspectives in Artificial Intelligence. Harvester Press, 1979.
John C., McCarthy. Circumscription –Aform of non-monotonic reasoning. Artificial Intelligence, 13:27–39, April 1980.Google Scholar
John C., McCarthy and Patrick J., Hayes. Some philosophical problems fromthe standpoint of artificial intelligence. Readings in Artificial Intelligence, pp. 431–450, 1969.Google Scholar
L. Thorne, McCarty. Ownership:Acase study in the representation of legal concepts. Artificial Intelligence and Law, 10(1–3):135–161, 2002.Google Scholar
Michael, McCloskey. Naive theories of motion. In Dedre, Gentner and A., Stevens (eds.), Mental Models. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1983.
Charles, Moore. Daniel H. Burnham, Architect, Planner of Cities, Volume 1. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1921.
Leora, Morgenstern. Mid-sized axiomatizations of commonsense problems:A case study in egg cracking. Studia Logica, 67(3):333–384, 2001.Google Scholar
Leora, Morgenstern. A first-order axiomitization of the Surprise Birthday Present problem: Preliminary report. In Seventh International Symposium on Logical Formalizations of Commonsense Reasoning (Commonsense-2005), Corfu, Greece, 2005. Available at http://commonsensereasoning.org/2005/
Adam, Morton. Frames of mind: Constraints on the common-sense conception of the mental, 1980.
Bernhard, Nebel. Base revision operations and schemes: Representation, semantics and complexity. In Proceedings of the 11th European Conference on Artificial Intelligence (ECAI'94), pp. 341–345. Hoboken,NJ: JohnWiley & Sons, 1994.
Shaun, Nichols, Stephen, Stich, Alan, Leslie, and David, Klein. Varieties of off-line simulation. In Theories of Theories of Mind, pp. 39–74. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1996.
Nils J., Nilsson. Logic and artificial intelligence. Artificial Intelligence, 47(1–3):31–56, 1991.Google Scholar
Andrew, Ortony, Gerald L., Clore, and Allan, Collins. The Cognitive Structure of Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1990.
Christopher C., Owens. Indexing and Retrieving Abstract Planning Knowledge. PhD thesis, Yale University, 1990.
Charles Sanders, Peirce. Abduction and induction. In Justus, Buchler (ed.), Philosophical Writings of Peirce, pp. 150–156. New York: Dover Books, 1955.
Elizabeth, Pennisi. Social animals prove their smarts. Science, 312(5781):1734–1738, 2006.Google Scholar
Pavlos, Peppas. Belief revision. In Frank van, Harmelen, Vladimir, Lifschitz, and Bruce, Porter (eds.), Handbook of Knowledge Representation. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 2008.
John, Perry and Jon, Barwise. Situations and Attitudes. Cambridge,MA: MIT Press, 1983.
Jean, Piaget. The Construction of Reality in the Child. New York: Basic Books, 1954.
Martin, Pickering and Nick, Chater. Why cognitive science is not formalized folk psychology. Minds and Machines, 5(3):309–337, 1995.Google Scholar
Ullin T., Place. Is consciousness a brain process? British Journal of Psychology, 47:44–50, 1956.Google Scholar
Carl, Pollard and Ivan A., Sag. Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar. Chicago:University of Chicago Press, 1994.
David, Premack and Guy, Woodruff. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(04):515–526, 1978.Google Scholar
Hilary, Putnam. The nature of mental states. In Art, Mind, and Religion, pp. 37–48. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press, 1973.
ZenonWalter, Pylyshyn. Computation and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1984.
Ashwin, Ram.Question-driven Understanding:An Integrated Theory of Story Understanding, Memory, and Learning. PhD thesis, Yale University, 1989.
Anand S., Rao and Michael P., Georgeff. Modeling rational agents within a BDIarchitecture. In Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Principles of KnowledgeRepresentation andReasoning,pp. 473–484. Burlington,MA:Morgan Kaufmann, 1991.
Byron, Reeves and Clifford, Nass. The Media Equation: How People Treat Computers, Television, and New Media Like People and Places. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications, 1996.
Raymond, Reiter. Knowledge in Action: Logical Foundations for Specifying and Implementing Dynamical Systems. Cambridge,MA: MIT Press, 2001.
Ivor, Richards. The Philosophy of Rhetoric. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1937.
Gilbert, Ryle. Concept of Mind. London: Hutchinson, 1949.
Peter, Salovey and Daisy, Grewal. The science of emotional intelligence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(6):281–285, 2005.Google Scholar
Kathryn E., Sanders. A logic for emotions: A basis for reasoning about commonsense psychological knowledge. Technical report, Brown University, Providence, RI, 1989.
Roger C., Schank. Explanation Patterns: Understanding Mechanically and Creatively. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1986.
Roger C., Schank and Robert P., Abelson. Scripts, Plans, Goals and Understanding: An Introduction into Human Knowledge Structures. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1977.
Roger C., Schank and Andrew, Fano. A thematic hierarchy for indexing stories in social domains. Technical Report 29, Institute for the Learning Sciences, Northwestern University, 1992.
Roger C., Schank, Alex, Kass, and Christopher K., Riesbeck (eds.). Inside Case-based Explanation. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1994.
Klaus R., Scherer. Emotion and emotional competence: Conceptual and theoretical issues for modeling agents. In A Blueprint for Affective Computing:A Sourcebook and Manual, Affective Sciences. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2010.
Yoav, Shoham. Nonmonotonic reasoning and causation. Cognitive Science, 14(2):213–252. Amsterdam: IOS Press, 1990.
Max, Silberztein. Text indexation with INTEX. Computers and the Humanities, 33(3):265– 280, 1999.Google Scholar
Munindar P., Singh and Nicholas M., Asher. A logic of intentions and beliefs. Journal of Philosophical Logic, 22(5):513–544, 1993.Google Scholar
John Jamieson Carswell, Smart. Sensations and brain processes. Philosophical Review, 68:141–156, 1959.Google Scholar
Jan, Smedslund. What is measured by a psychological measure? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 29(3-4):148–151, 1988.Google Scholar
Jan, Smedslund. The pseudoempirical in psychology and the case for psychologic. Psychological Inquiry, 2(4):325–338, 1991.Google Scholar
Jan, Smedslund. The Structure of Psychological Common Sense. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1997.
Jan, Smedslund. From hypothesis-testing psychology to procedure-testing psychologic. Review of General Psychology, 6(1):51, 2002.Google Scholar
Bruno, Snell. The Discovery of the Mind. The Greek Origins of European Thought. Oxford, 1953. Translated by Thomas Gustav Rosenmeyer.
Stephen P., Stich. From Folk Psychology to Cognitive Science: The Case Against Belief. Cambridge, MA:MIT Press, 1983.
Reid, Swanson and Andrew S., Gordon. Automated commonsense reasoning about human memory. In AAAI Spring Symposium:Metacognition in Computation, pp. 114–119. Palo Alto, CA: AAAI Press, 2005.
Helen, Tager-Flusberg. Evaluating the theory-of-mind hypothesis of autism. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6):311–315, 2007.Google Scholar
Kentaro, Toyama and Drew, McDermott. An interview with drewmcdermott. Crossroads, 3(1):3–4, 1996.Google Scholar
Alan M., Turing. Computing machinery and intelligence. Mind, LIX(236):433–460, 1950.Google Scholar
Amos, Tversky. Features of similarity. Psychological Review, 84(4):327, 1977.Google Scholar
Johan van, Bentham, Oliver, Roy, and Patrick, Girard. Everything else being equal: A modal logic for ceteris paribus preferences. Journal of Philosophical Logic, 38(1):83– 125, 2009.Google Scholar
Giambattista, Vico. The New Science of Giambattista Vico. Ithaca,NY: Cornell University Press, 1744. Translated by T., Bergin and M., Frisch (1968).
Eduard von, Hartmann. Philosophy of the Unconscious (1869). Translated by William Chatterton, Coupland. New York: Harcourt, 1931.
George Henrik Von, Wright. The Logic of Preference. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press, 1963.
Henry, Wellman. The Child's Theory of Mind. Learning, Development, and Conceptual Change. Cambridge,MA: MIT Press, 1990.
Henry M., Wellman, David, Cross, and Julanne, Watson. Meta-analysis of theory-ofmind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3):655–684, 2001.Google Scholar
Lancelot Law, Whyte. The Unconscious Before Freud. New York: St. Martin's Press, 1978.
Janyce, Wiebe, Theresa, Wilson, and Claire, Cardie. Annotating expressions of opinions and emotions in language. Language Resources and Evaluation, 39(2-3):165–210, 2005.Google Scholar
Anna, Wierzbicka. Emotions Across Languages and Cultures: Diversity and Universals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1999.
Heinz, Wimmer and Josef, Perner. Beliefs about beliefs:Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1):103–128, 1983.Google Scholar
Marianne, Winslett. Updating Logical Databases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1991.
Ludwig, Wittgenstein. Tractatus Logico-Philosophicus. London: Routledge&Kegan Paul, 1974. Translated by D. F., Pears and B. F., McGuinness.
Christian, Wolff. Preliminary Discourse on Philosophy in General. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill, 1963. Translated by R. J., Blackwell. Originally published in 1728.
Michael J., Wooldridge. Reasoning about Rational Agents. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2000.
Shali, Wu and Boaz, Keysar. The effect of culture on perspective taking. Psychological Science, 18(7):600–606, 2007.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Andrew S. Gordon, University of Southern California, Jerry R. Hobbs, University of Southern California
  • Book: A Formal Theory of Commonsense Psychology
  • Online publication: 01 September 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316584705.052
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Andrew S. Gordon, University of Southern California, Jerry R. Hobbs, University of Southern California
  • Book: A Formal Theory of Commonsense Psychology
  • Online publication: 01 September 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316584705.052
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Andrew S. Gordon, University of Southern California, Jerry R. Hobbs, University of Southern California
  • Book: A Formal Theory of Commonsense Psychology
  • Online publication: 01 September 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316584705.052
Available formats
×