Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-7drxs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T21:10:25.734Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Experiential Flexibility of Cultural Models: Kinship Knowledge and Networks among Individual Khasi (Meghalaya, N.E. India)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 November 2011

Monika Böck
Affiliation:
University of Cologne
Thomas Schweizer
Affiliation:
Universität zu Köln
Douglas R. White
Affiliation:
University of California, Irvine
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

For many years, social anthropologists have observed a gulf between the way peoples say kin should act toward one another – ideal standards or norms – and what they actually do…. What, then, to do conceptually about this apparent gulf between the ideal and the actual?

More than two decades ago, this question was asked by the late Roger M. Keesing in his textbook, Kin Groups and Social Structure (1975:122). He sketched out two lines of thinking, providing insights on how to deal with the problem: The first placed emphasis on “the actual,” focusing on individual interests and strategies for goal attainment and analyzing interpersonal relations and informal networks; the second line of thinking laid stress upon “the ideal,” investigating kinship in terms of people's own conceptual and symbolic world (1975:122–31).

In the following years the relationship between ideal standards or norms and actual kinship practices remained “a thorny question” (Barnard & Good 1984: 162). Whereas the first line of thinking has evidently produced some more food for thought (see, e.g., Bourdieu 1977; Holy 1986; Holy & Stuchlik 1983), the second does not appear to have had much to offer. On the one hand, symbolic analyses of kinship – especially those which followed and developed Schneider's approach (e.g., Delaney 1991; Inden & Nicholas 1977; Schneider 1968) – have been criticized for providing little evidence of links between allegedly culturally dominant indigenous concepts and symbols and specific “native voices.” The relationship between such cultural constructs and human action remains more or less unaddressed, and intracultural variations are neglected (Barnard & Good 1984:180; Starr 1994:229).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1998

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×