Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-g78kv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-27T13:25:15.819Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

20 - Some Issues in Interpreting Neuroscientific Evidence

from IV - Evidence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 April 2021

Bartosz Brożek
Affiliation:
Jagiellonian University, Krakow
Jaap Hage
Affiliation:
Universiteit Maastricht, Netherlands
Nicole Vincent
Affiliation:
Macquarie University, Sydney
Get access

Summary

This paper comments on Jane Moriarty’s chapter on neuroimaging evidence in the US courts by presenting a couple of inferential pitfalls one may encounter in interpreting neuroscientific evidence in the context of law and trial.

First, a general issue of heterogeneity of neuroscientific data (structural vs. functional, individual vs. group) is discussed, which gives the background for revealing other problems. Secondly, an example of the multiplicity (cascade) of assumptions and inferences entangled in interpreting a simple CT scan in the legal context is presented. Finally, some specific inferential pitfalls are described (primarily the reverse inference issue, the group-to-individual problem, and the lingua franca problem) and a few more are briefly mentioned.

The paper suggests that, for now, the possibilities that neuroscience offers fact-finders are quite modest, and interpreting neuroscientific evidence in the context of law is a difficult business.

Type
Chapter
Information
Law and Mind
A Survey of Law and the Cognitive Sciences
, pp. 421 - 430
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andreassi, J. L. (2006). Psychophysiology: Human Behavior & Physiological Response, 5th ed. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Brett, M., Johnsrude, I. S., & Owen, A. M. (2002). The Problem of Functional Localization in the Human Brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3, 243249.Google Scholar
Brindley, T., & Giordano, J. (2014). Neuroimaging: Correlation, Validity, Value, and Admissibility: Daubert – and Reliability – Revisited. AJOB Neuroscience 5(2), 4850.Google Scholar
Brown, T., & Murphy, E. (2009). Through a Scanner Darkly: Functional Neuroimaging as Evidence of a Criminal Defendant’s Past Mental States. Stanford Law Review 62, 11191208.Google Scholar
Buckholtz, J. W., & Faigman, D. L. (2014). Promises, Promises for Neuroscience and Law. Current Biology 24(18), 861867.Google Scholar
Buckholtz, J., Reyna, V. F., & Slobogin, C. (2016). A Neuro-Legal Lingua Franca: Bridging Law and Neuroscience on the Issue of Self-Control. Mental Health Law & Policy Journal 5, 130 (Vanderbilt Public Law Research Paper No. 1632).Google Scholar
Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P., Mokrysz, C., et al.(2013). Power Failure: Why Small Sample Size Undermines the Reliability of Neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 14(5), 365376.Google Scholar
Caplan, L. (1984). The Insanity Defense and the Trial of John W. Hinckley, Jr. Boston, MA: David R. Godine.Google Scholar
Chambers, C. D., Feredoes, E., Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., & Etchells, P. (2014). Instead of “Playing the Game” It Is Time to Change the Rules: Registered Reports at AIMS Neuroscience and Beyond. AIMS Neuroscience 1(1), 417.Google Scholar
Clarke, J. W. (1990). On Being Mad or Merely Angry: John W. Hinckley Jr. and Other Dangerous People. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
De Boer, S. F., Olivier, B., Veening, J., & Koolhaas, J. M. (2015). The Neurobiology of Offensive Aggression: Revealing a Modular View. Physiology & Behavior 146, 111127.Google Scholar
Eklund, A., Nichols, T. E., & Knutsson, H. (2016). Cluster Failure: Why fMRI Inferences for Spatial Extent Have Inflated False-Positive Rates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113(28), 79007905.Google Scholar
Faigman, D. L., Monahan, J., & Slobogin, C. (2014). Group to Individual (G2i) Inference in Scientific Expert Testimony. The University of Chicago Law Review 81(2), 417480.Google Scholar
Farah, M. J., Hutchinson, J. B., Phelps, E. A., & Wagner, A. D. (2014). Functional MRI-Based Lie Detection: Scientific and Societal Challenges. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 15(2), 123131.Google Scholar
Feigenson, N. (2006). Brain Imaging and Courtroom Evidence: On the Admissibility and Persuasiveness of fMRI. International Journal of Law in Context 2(3), 233255.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2011). Neuroscience in the Courtroom. Scientific American 304(4), 5459.Google Scholar
Gilmore, R. O., Diaz, M. T., Wyble, B. A., & Yarkoni, T. (2017). Progress Toward Openness, Transparency, and Reproducibility in Cognitive Neuroscience. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1396(1), 518.Google Scholar
Goldstone, R. L., Pestilli, F., & Börner, K. (2015). Self-Portraits of the Brain: Cognitive Science, Data Visualization, and Communicating Brain Structure and Function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19(8), 462474.Google Scholar
Hans, V. P., & Slater, D. (1983). John Hinckley, Jr. and the insanity defense: The public’s verdict. Public Opinion Quarterly 47(2), 202212.Google Scholar
Knight, R. T. (2007). Neural Networks Debunk Phrenology. Science 316(5831), 15781579.Google Scholar
Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. Q. (1998). Brain Plasticity and Behavior. Annual Review of Psychology 49(1), 4364.Google Scholar
Loftus, E. F. (2005). Planting Misinformation in the Human Mind: A 30-Year Investigation of the Malleability of Memory. Learning & Memory 12(4), 361366.Google Scholar
Loftus, E. F. (2018). Eyewitness Science and the Legal System. Annual Review of Law and Social Science 14, 110.Google Scholar
McCabe, D. P., & Castel, A. D. (2008). Seeing Is Believing: The Effect of Brain Images on Judgments of Scientific Reasoning. Cognition 107(1), 343352.Google Scholar
McCabe, D. P., Castel, A. D., & Rhodes, M. G. (2011). The Influence of fMRI Lie Detection Evidence on Juror Decision-Making. Behavioral Sciences & the Law 29(4), 566577.Google Scholar
Moriarty, J. C. (2008). Flickering Admissibility: Neuroimaging Evidence in the US Courts. Behavioral Sciences & the Law 26(1), 2649.Google Scholar
Moriarty, J. C., & Langleben, D. D. (2017). Who Speaks for Neuroscience? Neuroimaging Evidence and Courtroom Expertise. Case Western Reserve Law Review 68(3), 783804.Google Scholar
Moriarty, J. C., Langleben, D. D., & Provenzale, J. M. (2013). Brain Trauma, PET Scans and Forensic Complexity. Behavioral Sciences & the Law 31(6), 702720.Google Scholar
Murphy, E. (2016). Neuroscience and the Civil/Criminal Daubert Divide. Fordham Law Review 85, 619639.Google Scholar
Parsons, L. M. (2001). Integrating Cognitive Psychology, Neurology and Neuroimaging. Acta Psychologica 107(1–3), 155181.Google Scholar
Pearl, J., & Mackenzie, D. (2018). The Book of Why: The New Science of Cause and Effect. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Plant, R. R., & Quinlan, P. T. (2013). Could Millisecond Timing Errors in Commonly Used Equipment Be a Cause of Replication Failure in Some Neuroscience Studies? Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 13(3), 598614.Google Scholar
Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Can Cognitive Processes Be Inferred from Neuroimaging Data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(2), 5963.Google Scholar
Poldrack, R. A., Baker, C. I., Durnez, J., et al.(2017). Scanning the Horizon: Towards Transparent and Reproducible Neuroimaging Research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 18(2), 115126.Google Scholar
Ross, L. (1977). The Intuitive Psychologist and His Shortcomings: Distortions in the Attribution Process. In Berkowitz, L. (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 10. New York: Academic Press. pp. 173220.Google Scholar
Rushing, S. E. (2014). The Admissibility of Brain Scans in Criminal Trials: The Case of Positron Emission Tomography. Court Review 50, 6269.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., & Loftus, E. F. (2013). Memory and Law: What Can Cognitive Neuroscience Contribute? Nature Neuroscience 16(2), 119123.Google Scholar
Schauer, F. (2010). Can Bad Science Be Good Evidence? Neuroscience, Lie Detection, and Beyond. Cornell Law Review 95(6), 11911220.Google Scholar
Schleim, S., & Roiser, J. P. (2009). fMRI in Translation: The Challenges Facing Real-World Applications. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 3, 63. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.09.063.2009Google Scholar
Schum, D. A., & Martin, A. W. (1982). Formal and Empirical Research on Cascaded Inference in Jurisprudence. Law & Society Review 17(1), 105152.Google Scholar
Slovenko, R. (1982). The Insanity Defense in the Wake of the Hinckley Trial. Rutgers Law Journal 14, 373.Google Scholar
Smaldino, P. E., & McElreath, R. (2016). The Natural Selection of Bad Science. Royal Society Open Science 3(9), 160384. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.160384Google Scholar
Turner, B. O., Paul, E. J., Miller, M. B., & Barbey, A. K. (2018). Small Sample Sizes Reduce the Replicability of Task-Based fMRI Studies. Communications Biology 1(1), 110.Google Scholar
Ward, N. S. (2005). Neural Plasticity and Recovery of Function. Progress in Brain Research 150, 527535.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×