Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-8zxtt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-15T01:36:14.160Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Memory development and learning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2011

Stefano Vicari
Affiliation:
Bambino Gesù Children's Hospital
Deny Menghini
Affiliation:
Bambino Gesù Children's Hospital
Jean-Adolphe Rondal
Affiliation:
Université de Liège, Belgium
Juan Perera
Affiliation:
Universitat de les Illes Balears, Palma de Mallorca
Donna Spiker
Affiliation:
Stanford Research Institute International
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Distinct cognitive profiles among individuals with mental retardation (MR) of different etiologies have recently been documented. Studies from different laboratories, for example, have demonstrated a complex neuropsychological profile in people with Down syndrome (DS), with atypical development in the cognitive and linguistic domains (for a review see Vicari et al., 2004; Vicari, 2006). However, a quite different pattern is often reported in other syndromes such as Williams syndrome (WS). This is another genetic condition, less frequent but equally characterized by MR and typified by a number of severe medical anomalies, such as facial dysmorphology and abnormalities of the cardiovascular system (Bellugi et al., 1999). Differently from DS, WS children often show marked impairment in certain spatial abilities (especially praxic–constructive) and relative preservation of both productive and receptive language, at least concerning the phonological elements (Vicari et al., 2004).

Within the neuropsychological approach to MR, the study of memory and learning is particularly relevant. In fact, altered development of the memory function can seriously interfere with adequate maturation of general intellectual abilities, and thus with the possibility of learning and modifying behavior on the basis of experience.

This chapter is dedicated to reviewing the neuropsychological literature and recent experimental studies on memory and learning development in people with DS, reporting their memory capacities and deficits. Consistent with a neuropsychological approach, distinct memory profiles can be traced to the characteristics of the DS brain development and architecture.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ashburner, J. & Friston, K. J. (2000). Voxel-based morphometry: the methods. NeuroImage, 11, 805–821.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, R. C. & Shiffrin, R. M. (1971). The control of short-term memory. Scientific American, 225, 82–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working Memory. London: Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D. & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. (ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 8, pp. 47–90. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U., Mills, D., Jerningan, T., Hickok, G., Galaburda, A. (1999). Linking cognition, brain structure, and brain function in Williams syndrome. In Tager-Flusberg, H. ed.), Neurodevelopmental Disorders, pp. 111–136. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Carlesimo, G. A., Marotta, L., Vicari, S. (1997). Long-term memory in mental retardation: evidence for a specific impairment in subjects with Down's syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 35, 71–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, R. S. (1995). Language development in children and adolescents with Down syndrome. In Fletcher, P. & MacWinney, B. (eds.), The Handbook of Child Language, pp. 641–663. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Della Sala, S. & Logie, R. H. (2002). Neuropsychological impairments of visual and spatial working memory. In Baddeley, A., Wilson, B., Kopelman, M. (eds.), Handbook of Memory Disorders, pp. 271 –292. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Fabbro, F., Alberti, A., Gagliardi, C., Borgatti, R. (2002). Differences in native and foreign language repetition task between subjects with Williams and Down syndromes. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 15, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, A. E. (1995). Linguistic variability in persons with Down syndrome. In Nadel, L. & Rosenthal, D. (eds.), Down Syndrome: Living and Learning in the Community, pp. 121–131. New York: Wiley-Liss.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working Memory and Language. Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Good, C. D., Johnsrude, I., Ashburner, J., et al. (2001). A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. NeuroImage, 14, 21–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hulme, C. & Mackenzie, S. (1992). Working Memory and Severe Learning Difficulties. Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ikeda, M. & Arai, Y. (2002). Longitudinal changes in brain CT scans and development of dementia in Down's syndrome. European Neurology, 47, 205–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., Hewes, A. K. (1999). Genetically dissociated components of working memory: evidence from Down and Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 37, 637–651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., Hewes, A. K. (2000). Verbal short-term memory deficits in Down syndrome: a consequence of problems in rehearsal? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(2), 233–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanno, K. & Ikeda, Y. (2002). Word-length effect in verbal short-term memory in individuals with Down's syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 46, 613–618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kesslak, J. P., Nagata, B. S., Lott, I., Nalcioglu, O. (1994). MRI analysis of age-related changes in the brains of individuals with DS. Neurology, 44, 1039–1045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krasuski, J. S., Alexander, G. E., Horowitz, B., Rapoport, S. I., Shapiro, M. B. (2002). Relation of medial temporal volumes to age and memory function in non-demented adults with Down's syndrome: implications for the prodromal phase of Alzheimer's disease. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 74–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laws, G. (2002). Working memory in children and adolescents with Down syndrome: evidence from a colour memory experiment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 353–364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laws, G. (2004). Contributions of phonological memory, language comprehension and hearing to the expressive language of adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laws, G. & Gunn, D. (2003). Phonological memory as a predictor of language comprehension in Down syndrome: a five-year follow-up study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 1–11.Google Scholar
Logie, R. H. (1995). Visuo-spatial Working Memory. Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Menghini, D., Costanzo, F., & Vicari, S. (in press). Relationship between brain and cognitive processes in Down syndrome. Behavior Genetics. DOI 10.1007/s10519-011-9448-3.
Mishkin, M. & Ungerleider, L. G. (1982). Contribution of striate inputs to the visuospatial functions of parieto-preoccipital cortex in monkeys. Behavioral Brain Research, 6, 57–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molinari, M., Leggio, M. G., Solida, A., et al. (1997). Cerebellum and procedural learning: evidence from focal cerebellar lesions. Brain, 120, 1753–1762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moscovitch, C., Kapur, S., Kohler, S., Houle, S. (1995). Distinct neural correlates of visual long-term memory for spatial location and object identity: a positron emission tomography study in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 92, 3721–3725.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nadel, L. (1999). Learning and memory in Down syndrome. In Rondal, J. A., Perera, J., Nadel, L. (eds.), Down Syndrome: A Review of Current Knowledge, pp. 133–142. London: Whurr.Google Scholar
Pennington, B. F., Moon, J., Edgin, J., Stedron, J., Nadel, L. (2003). The neuropsychology of Down syndrome: evidence for hippocampal dysfunction. Child Development, 74, 75–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinter, J. D., Eliez, S., Schmitt, J. E., Capone, G. T., Reiss, A. L. (2001). Neuroanatomy of Down's syndrome: a high-resolution MRI study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 1659–1665.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L. R. (1987). Memory and Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Teipel, S. J., Alexander, G. E., Schapiro, M. B., et al. (2004). Age-related cortical grey matter reductions in nondemented Down's syndrome adults determined by MRI with voxel-based morphometry. Brain, 127, 811–824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graaf, F. H., Jong, B. M., Maguire, R. P., Meiners, L. C., Leenders, K. L. (2004). Cerebral activation related to skills practice in a double serial reaction time task: striatal involvement in random-order sequence learning. Brain Research and Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 120–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vicari, S. (2006). Motor development and neuropsychological patterns in persons with Down syndrome. Behavior Genetics, 36, 355–364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vicari, S., Bates, E., Caselli, M. C., et al. (2004). Neuropsychological profile of Italians with Williams syndrome: an example of a dissociation between language and cognition?Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 862–876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vicari, S., Bellucci, S., Carlesimo, G. A. (2000). Implicit and explicit memory: a functional dissociation in persons with Down syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 38, 240–251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vicari, S., Bellucci, S., Carlesimo, G. A. (2001). Procedural learning deficit in children with Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 39, 665–677.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vicari, S., Bellucci, S., Carlesimo, G. A. (2003). Visual and spatial working memory dissociation: evidence from a genetic syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 45, 269–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vicari, S., Bellucci, S., Carlesimo, G. A. (2005). Visual and spatial long-term memory: differential pattern of impairments in Williams and Down syndromes. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 47, 305–311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vicari, S., Bellucci, S., Carlesimo, G. A. (2006). Evidence from two genetic syndromes for the independence of spatial and visual working memory. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 48, 126–131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vicari, S. & Carlesimo, G. A. (2002). Children with intellectual disabilities. In Baddeley, A., Wilson, B., Kopelman, M. (eds.), Handbook of Memory Disorders, pp. 501–518. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Vicari, S. & Carlesimo, G. A. (2006). Short-term memory deficits are not uniform in Down and Williams syndromes. Neuropsychological review, 16, 87–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vicari, S., Carlesimo, G. A., Caltagirone, C. (1995). Short-term memory in persons with intellectual disabilities and Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 39, 532–537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vicari, S., Verucci, L., Carlesimo, G. A. (2007). Implicit memory is independent from IQ and age but not from etiology: evidence from Down and Williams syndromes. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 51, 932–941.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, P. P. & Bellugi, U. (1994). Evidence from two genetic syndromes for dissociation between verbal and visual-spatial short-term memory. Journal of Clinical Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 317–322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wisniewski, K. E. (1990). Down syndrome children often have brain with maturation delay, retardation of growth, and cortical digenesis. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 7, 274–281.Google Scholar
White, N. S., Alkire, M. T., Haier, R. J. (2003). A voxel-based morphometric study of nondemented adults with Down syndrome. NeuroImage, 20, 393–403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×