Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T08:13:53.519Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2018

Luiz Amaral
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Amherst
Marcus Maia
Affiliation:
Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro
Andrew Nevins
Affiliation:
University College London and Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro
Tom Roeper
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Amherst
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbeduto, L. and Rosenberg, S. (1985). Children’s knowledge of the presuppositions of know and other cognitive verbs. Journal of Child Language, 12(3), 621641.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aboh, E. (2004). The morphosyntax of complement-head sequences: clause structure and word order patterns in Kwa. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Adani, F. (2011). Rethinking the acquisition of relative clauses in Italian: towards a grammaticality based account. Journal of Child Language, 38(1), 141165.Google Scholar
Aguado, G. (2000). El desarrollo del lenguaje de 0 a 3 años. Madrid: CEPE.Google Scholar
Aikhenvald, A. Y. (2003). Evidentiality in typological perspective. In Aikhenvald, A. Y. and Dixon, R. M. W. (eds.), Studies in evidentiality. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 131.Google Scholar
Aikhenvald, A. Y. (2004). Evidentiality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aikhenvald, A. Y. (2012). Languages of the Amazon. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Aissen, J. and Perlmutter, D. (1983). Clause reduction in Spanish. In Perlmutter, D. (ed.), Studies in relational grammar 1. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 360403.Google Scholar
Alston, W. P. (2000). Illocutionary acts and sentence meanings. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Amaral, L. and Leandro, W. (2013). The acquisition of the relativizer ‘–uraz’ in Wapichana (and other recursive structures). Paper presented at Recursion in Brazilian Languages and Beyond 2013, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro.Google Scholar
Ambridge, B., Pine, J., and Lieven, E. (2013). Child language acquisition: why universal grammar does not help. Perspectives: Online Section of Language, 90(3), e53e90.Google Scholar
Anchieta, J. (1990). Arte da gramática da língua mais usada na costa do Brasil (fac-simile, 1595). São Paulo: Loyola.Google Scholar
Anderson, L. B. (1986). Evidentials, paths of change, and mental maps: typologically regular asymmetries. In Chafe, W. and Nichols, J. (eds.), Evidentiality: the linguistic coding of epistemology. Norwood: Ablex, 273312.Google Scholar
Arnauld, A. and Lancelot, C. (1660). Grammaire générale et raisonnée contenant les fondemens de l’art de parler, expliqués d’une manière claire et naturelle. (J. Rieux and B. Rollin, trans., 1975). The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Aronoff, M. and Fuhrhop, N. (2002). Restricting suffix combinations in German and English: closing suffixes and the monosuffix constraint. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 20(3), 451490.Google Scholar
Arsenijević, B. (2009). Clausal complementation as relativization. Lingua, 119(1), 3950.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arsenijević, B. and Hinzen, W. (2012). On the absence of X-within-X recursion in human grammar. Linguistic Inquiry, 43(3), 423440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Augusto, M. R. A., Corrêa, L. M. S., and Forster, R. (2012). An argument for DPs as phases in an integrated model of on-line computation: the immediate mapping of complex DPs with relative clauses. Revista Virtual de Estudos da Linguagem – ReVEL. Special issue, n. 6.Google Scholar
Backscheider, A. and Shatz, M. (1993). Children’s acquisition of the lexical domain of color. In Beals, K. et al. (eds.), What we think, what we mean, and how we say it: papers from the parasession on the correspondence of conceptual, semantic, and grammatical representations, CLS 29 (vol. 2). Chicago: The Chicago Linguistic Society, 1121.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1997). Human memory: theory and practice (revised edition). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Baker, M. (1988). Incorporation: a theory of grammatical function changing. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Baker, M. (1996). The polysynthesis parameter. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, M. (2001). The atoms of language. Charlotte: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bar-Hillel, Y. (1953). On recursive definitions in empirical science. Proceedings of 11th International Congress of Philosophy, 5, 160165.Google Scholar
Barnes, J. (1984). Evidentials in the Tuyuca verb. International Journal of American Linguistics, 50(3), 255271.Google Scholar
Barnes, J. (2006). Tucanoan languages. In Brown, K. (ed.), Encyclopedia of language and linguistics (2nd edition). Oxford: Elsevier, 130142.Google Scholar
Basilico, D. (1996). Head position and internally headed relative clauses. Language, 72(3), 498532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayer, J. (1999). Final complementizer in hybrid languages. Journal of Linguistics, 35(2), 233271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berwick, R. C. and Chomsky, N. (2015). Why only us: language and evolution. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Berwick, R. C., Friederici, A., Chomsky, N., and Bolhuis, J. J. (2013) Evolution brain and the nature of language. Trends in Cognitive Science, 17(2), 8998.Google Scholar
Biberauer, T., Holmberg, A., and Roberts, I. (2014). A syntactic universal and its consequences. Linguistic Inquiry, 45(2), 169225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickel, B. (2007). Typology in the 21st century: major current developments. Linguistic Typology, 11(1), 239251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bisetto, A. (2010). Recursiveness and Italian compounds. SKASE Journal of Theoretical Linguistics, 7(1), 1435Google Scholar
Bjorkman, B. M. (2011). A syntactic correlate of semantic asymmetries in clausal coordination. Presented at: 41st Annual Meeting of the North East Linguistic Society. October 22–24.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. M., Lightbown, P., and Hood, L. (1975). Structure and variation in child language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 40(2), 197.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Rispoli, M., Gartner, B., and Hafitz, J. (1989). Acquisition of complementation. Journal of Child Language, 16(1),101120Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (1990). Subjectless sentences in child language. Linguistic Inquiry, 21(4), 491504.Google Scholar
Bloomfield, L. (1933). Language. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Bobaljik, J. D. and Thráinsson, H. (1998). Two heads aren’t always better than one. Syntax 1(1), 3771.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booij, G. (2000). Inflection and derivation. In Booij, G. et al. (eds.), Morphology: an international handbook on inflection and word formation (vol. 1). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter, 360369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boole, G. (1854). An investigation of the laws of thought on which are founded the mathematical theories of logic and probabilities. London: Macmillan. Reprinted with corrections, New York: Dover Publications, 1958.Google Scholar
Bopp, F. (1816). Über das Conjugationssystem der Sanskritsprache in Vergleichung mit jenem der griechischen, lateinischen, persischen und germanischen Sprache. Frankfurt: Andreae, K. J. Windischmann. Reprinted Hildesheim: Olms, 1975.Google Scholar
Brown, C. and Hagoort, P. (1993). The processing nature of the N400: evidence from masked priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(1), 3444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, R. W. (1973). A first language. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, R. W. and Hanlon, C. (1970). Derivational complexity and order of acquisition in child speech. In Hayes, R. (ed.), Cognition and the development of language. New York: Wiley, 1121.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1982). Semantic development: state of the art. In Gleitman, L. and Wanner, E. (eds.), Language acquisition: state of the art. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 347389.Google Scholar
Carnie, A., Harley, H., and Pyatt, E. (2000). VSO order as raising out of IP? Some evidence from Old Irish. In Carnie, A. and Guilfoyle, E. (eds.), The syntax of verb initial languages. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 3960.Google Scholar
Carston, R. (1993). Conjunction, explanation and relevance. Lingua, 90(1), 2748.Google Scholar
Carston, R. (2002). Thoughts and utterances: the pragmatics of explicit communication. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Chafe, W. L. (1982). Integration and involvement in speaking, writing and oral literature. In Tannen, D. (ed.), Spoken and written language: exploring orality and literacy. Norwood: Ablex, 3553.Google Scholar
Chen-Main, J. (2005). Characteristics of Mandarin imperatives. In Brandstetter, C. and Rus, D. (eds.), Georgetown Working Papers in Linguistics 4. Washington, DC: Georgetown Dept of Linguistics, 115.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1955/1975). The logical structure of linguistic theory. Manuscript, Harvard University. New York and London: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1959). On certain formal properties of grammars. Information and Control, 2(2), 137167.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris Publications.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2001). Derivation by phase. In Kenstowicz, M. (ed.), Ken Hale: a life in language. Cambridge: MIT Press, 154.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2004). Beyond explanatory adequacy. In A. Belletti (ed.), Structures and Beyond. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 104–131.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2005). Three factors of language design. Linguistic Inquiry, 36(1), 122.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2007). Approaching UG from below. In Sauerland, U. and Gärtner, H. (eds.), Interfaces + recursion = language? Chomsky’s minimalism and the view from syntax-semantics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 130.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2008). On phases. In Freidin, R., Otero, C. P., and Zubizarreta, M. L. (eds.), Foundational issues in linguistic theory: essays in honor of Jean-Roger Vergnaud. Cambridge: MIT Press, 133166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2012). The science of language: interviews with James McGilvray. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2013). Problems of projection. Lingua, 130, 3349.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. and Lasnik, H. (1993). The theory of principles and parameters. In Jacobs, J. et al. (eds.), Syntax: an international handbook of contemporary research (vol. 1). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. 506569.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. and Miller, G. (1958). Finite state languages. Information and Control, 1(2), 91112.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. and Miller, G. (1963). Introduction to the formal analysis of natural languages. In Luce, R. D., Bush, R. R., and Galanter, E. (eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology (vol. 2). New York: Wiley, 269321.Google Scholar
Chung, S. (2005). What fronts? On the VP-raising account of verb-initial order. In Carnie, A., Harley, H., and Dooley-Colburg, S. (eds.), Verb first: studies in predicate initial languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 930.Google Scholar
Chung, S. (2006). Properties of VOS languages. In Everaert, M., van Riemsdijk, H., Goedemans, R., and Hollebrandse, B. (eds.), The Blackwell companion to syntax. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 685720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cinque, G. (2006a). Restructuring and functional heads. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cinque, G. (2006b). Two types of appositives. University of Venice Working Papers in Linguistics, 16, 756.Google Scholar
Clements, W. A. and Perner, J. (1994). Implicit understanding of belief. Cognitive Development, 9(4), 377395.Google Scholar
Clifton, C., Speer, S., and Abney, S. (1991). Parsing arguments: phrase structure and argument structure determinants of initial parsing decisions. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(2), 251271.Google Scholar
Cole, P. (1987) The structure of internally headed relative clauses. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 5(2) 277302.Google Scholar
Collins, C. and Branigan, P. (1997). Quotative inversion. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 15(1), 141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Comrie, B. (1983). Switch-reference in Huichol: a typological study. In Haiman, J. and Munro, P. (eds.), Switch-reference and universal grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 1737.Google Scholar
Condoravdi, C. and Lauer, S. (2010). Imperatives and public commitments. Handout of talk presented at the 11th Annual Semantics Fest. Stanford University.Google Scholar
Condoravdi, C. and Lauer, S. (2011). Performative verbs and performative acts. In Reich, I., Horch, E., and Pauly, D. (eds.), Sinn und Bedeutung 15, Saarbrücken: Saarland Unversity Press, 149164.Google Scholar
Condry, K. and Spelke, E. (2008). The development of language and abstract concepts: the case of natural number. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(1), 2238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coon, J. (2010). VOS as predicate-fronting in Chol. Lingua, 120, 345378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corballis, M. (2007). The uniqueness of human recursive thinking. American Scientist, 95(3), 240248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corballis, M. (2011). The recursive mind: the origins of human language, thought, and civilization. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Corrêa, L. M. S. (1995). An alternative account of children’s comprehension of relative clauses. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24(3), 183203.Google Scholar
Corrêa, L. M. S. and Augusto, M. R. A. (2007). Computação linguística no processamento on-line: soluções formais para a incorporação de uma derivação minimalista em modelos de processamento. Cadernos de Estudos Linguistics (UNICAMP), 49(2), 167183.Google Scholar
Corrêa, L. M. S. and Augusto, M. R. A. (2011). Possible loci of SLI from a both linguistic and psycholinguistic perspective. Lingua, 121(3), 476486.Google Scholar
Corrêa, L. M. S., Augusto, M. R. S. and Marcilese, M. (2009). Resumptive pronouns and passives in the production of object relative clauses: circumventing computational cost. Presentation. 22nd Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing. Davis, CA: March 26–28.Google Scholar
Cowart, W. and McDaniel, D. (2012). What kind of a thing is a coordinate? Qualitative comparison of agreement relations in coordinate and subordinate sentences. In Myers, J. (ed.), Grammar and evidence. Taipei: Language and Linguistics Monograph Series 48.Google Scholar
Cram, D. and Maat, J. (eds.) (2001). George Dalgarno on universal language: the art of signs (1661), the deaf and dumb man’s tutor (1680), and the unpublished papers. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cresswell, M. (1990). Entities and indices, No. 41 in studies in linguistics and philosophy. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Crnič, L. and Trinh, T. (2009). Embedding imperatives in English. In Riester, A. and Solstad, T. (eds.), Sinn und Bedeutung 13. Stuttgart: OPUS, 113127.Google Scholar
Crocker, M. W. (2009). Computational psycholinguistics. In Clark, A., Fox, C., and Lappin, L. (eds.), The handbook of computational linguistics and natural language processing. London: Blackwell, 482513.Google Scholar
Culicover, P. W. and Jackendoff, R. (1997). Semantic subordination despite syntactic coordination. Linguistic Inquiry, 28(2), 195217.Google Scholar
Culy, C. (1990). The syntax and semantics of internally headed relative clauses. Ph.D. Dissertation. Stanford University.Google Scholar
Davidson, D. (1968). On saying that. Synthese, 19(1–2), 130146.Google Scholar
Davis, H., Gillon, G., and Matthewson, L. (2014). How to investigate linguistic diversity: lessons from the Pacific Northwest. Language, 90(4), 180226Google Scholar
DeLancey, S. (1997). Mirativity: the grammatical marking of unexpected information. Linguistic Typology, 1(1), 3352.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1996). The intentional stance. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Di Sciullo, A. M. (2015). On the domain specificity of the human language faculty and the effects of principles of computational efficiency. Revista LinguíStica, 11(1), 5770.Google Scholar
Diesing, M. (1992). Indefinites. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Diesing, M. (1996). Semantic variables and object shift. In Epstein, S. D. and Thráinsson, H. (eds.), Studies in comparative Germanic syntax (vol. 2). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers, 6684.Google Scholar
Diesing, M. (1997). Yiddish VP order and the typology of object movement. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 15(2), 369427.Google Scholar
Diessel, H. (2004) The acquisition of complex sentences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Diessel, H. and Tomasello, M. (2001). The acquisition of finite complement clauses in English: a usage based approach to the development of grammatical constructions. Cognitive Linguistics, 12(2), 97141.Google Scholar
Dobson, R. (1980). Clause patterns in Kayabi. Brasília: Sociedade Internacional de Linguística (SIL).Google Scholar
Dobson, R. (1988). Aspectos da língua Kayabi. Brasília: SIL.Google Scholar
Dobson, R. (1990). Textos Kayabi. Brasília: Sociedade Internacional de Linguística (SIL).Google Scholar
Dobson, R. (1997). Gramática prática com exercícios da língua Kawaiwete. Brasília: Sociedade Internacional de Linguística (SIL).Google Scholar
Dobson, R. (2005). Aspectos da língua Kayabi. Cuiabá: Sociedade Internacional de Lingüística (SIL).Google Scholar
Dollaghan, C. and Campbell, T. (1998) Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 41(5), 11361146.Google Scholar
Dooley, R. (2006). Léxico Guarani, dialeto Mbyá com informações úteis para o ensino médio, a aprendizagem e a pesquisa linguística. Cuiabá: Sociedade Internacional de Lingüística.Google Scholar
Dooley, R. A. (2016). Léxico Guarani, Dialeto Mbyá – com informações úteis para o ensino médio, a aprendizagem e a pesquisa linguística. Brasília: Associação Internacional de Linguística/SIL Brasil.Google Scholar
Doron, E. (2000). VSO and left-conjunct agreement: Biblical Hebrew vs. modern Hebrew. In Carnie, A. and Guilfoyle, E. (eds.), The syntax of verb-initial languages. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 7596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duarte, F. B. (2005). Realização de traços de tempo nos D/NPs em Tenetehára. 53rd Seminar of Grupo de Estudos Linguísticos. UFSCAR, São Carlos.Google Scholar
Duarte, F. B. (2007). Studies in Tenetehára morphosyntax. Belo Horizonte, MG: Fale-UFMG Press.Google Scholar
Duarte, F. B. (2012). Tenetehára: a predicate fronting language. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 57(3) 359386.Google Scholar
Eisenbeiss, S. (2000). The acquisition of the DP in German. In Rizzi, L. and Friedemann, M. A. (eds.), The acquisition of syntax: studies in comparative developmental linguistics. London: Longman, 2762.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, S., Ukrainetz, T., Hsu, J., Kaderavek, J., Justice, L., and Gillam, R. (2008) Noun phrase elaboration in children’s spoken stories. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 39(2), 145157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epps, P. (2005). Areal diffusion and the development of evidentiality: evidence from Hup. Studies in Language, 29(3), 617649.Google Scholar
Evans, N. and Levinson, S. (2009). The myth of language universals: language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 429492.Google Scholar
Everett, D. (1986). Pirahã. In Derbyshire, D. C. and Pullum, G. K. (eds.), Handbook of Amazonian languages (vol. 1). Berlin: de Gruyter, 200325.Google Scholar
Everett, D. (1991). A língua Pirahã e a teoria da sintaxe: descrição, perspectivas e teoria. Campinas, São Paulo: Editora da UNICAMP.Google Scholar
Everett, D. (2005). Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã: another look at the design features of human language. Current Anthropology, 46(4), 621646.Google Scholar
Everett, D. (2008). Don’t sleep, there are snakes: life and language in the Amazonian jungle. New York: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Everett, D. (2009). Pirahã culture and grammar: a response to some criticisms. Language, 85(2), 405442.Google Scholar
Everett, D. (2012). Language: the cultural tool. New York: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Everett, K. (1990). Pirahã-English dictionary. Manuscript. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh.Google Scholar
Faller, M. (2002). Semantics and pragmatics of evidentials in Cuzco Quechua. Ph.D. Dissertation. Stanford University.Google Scholar
Faller, M. (2007). Evidentiality above and below speech acts. Manchester: University of Manchester.Google Scholar
Faller, M. (2011). A possible worlds semantics for Cuzco Quechua evidentials. In Li, N. and Lutz, D. (eds.), Semantics and Linguistic Theory (SALT) 20. Cornell University: CLC Publications, 660683.Google Scholar
Faria, A. (2004). Ordem oracional e movimento de clítico de segunda posição em Kayabi. Master’s Thesis. Unicamp.Google Scholar
Fedden, S. (2011). A grammar of Mian. (vol. 55). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Fernandez-Duque, D. (2009). Cognitive and neural underpinnings of syntactic complexity. In Givon, T. and Shibatani, M. (eds.), Syntactic complexity: diachrony, acquisition, neuro-cognition, evolution. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 433460.Google Scholar
Ferreiro, E., Othenin-Girard, C., Chipman, H. and Sinclair, H. (1976). How do children handle relative clauses? Archives de Psychologie, 44, 229266.Google Scholar
Finer, D. L. (1984). The formal grammar of switch-reference. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of Massachusetts Amherst.Google Scholar
Fitch, T. (2010). Three meanings of “recursion”: key distinctions for biolinguistics. In Larson, R., Déprez, V., and Yamakido, H. (eds.), The evolution of human language. New York: Cambridge University Press, 7390.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. and Garrett, M. F. (1967). Some syntactic determinants of sentential complexity. Perception and Psychophysics, 2(7), 289296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ford, M. (1983). A method of obtaining measures of local parsing complexity throughout sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 22(2), 203218.Google Scholar
França, A. I., Lemle, M., Cagy, M., Constant, P., and Infantosi, A. F. C. I. (2004). Discriminating among different types of verb-complement merge in Brazilian Portuguese: an ERP study of morphosyntactic subprocesses. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17(6), 425437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franchetto, B. (2010). The ergativity effect in Kuikuro (Southern Carib, Brazil). In Gildea, S. and Queixalós, F.. (eds.), Ergativity in Amazonia. Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing, 121158.Google Scholar
Frank, R. (2006). Phase theory and tree adjoining grammar. Lingua, 116, 145202.Google Scholar
Frazier, L. (1985). Syntactic complexity. In Dowty, D., Karttunen, L., and Zwicky, A. (eds.), Natural language parsing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 129189.Google Scholar
Frege, G. (1892). Über Sinn und Bedeutung. Zeitschrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik, 100, 2550.Google Scholar
Friederici, A. D. and Brauer, J. (2009). Syntactic complexity in the brain. In Givón, T. and Shibatani, M. (eds.), Syntactic complexity: diachrony, acquisition, neuro-cognition, evolution. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 491506.Google Scholar
Friederici, A. D., Bahlmann, J., Friedrich, R., and Makuuchi, M. (2011). The neural basis of recursion and complex syntactic hierarchy. Biolinguistics, 5(1–2), 87104.Google Scholar
Friedmann, N. and Costa, J. (2010). The child heard a coordinated sentence and wondered: on children’s difficulty in understanding coordination and relative clauses with crossing dependencies, Lingua, 120(6), 15021515.Google Scholar
Friedmann, N., Belletti, A., and Rizzi, L. (2009). Relativized relatives: types of intervention in the acquisition of A-bar dependencies. Lingua, 119(1), 6788.Google Scholar
Fujimori, C. (2010). Acquisition of recursive possessives in Japanese. Master’s Thesis. University of Massachusetts Amherst.Google Scholar
Gayraud, F. and Martinie, B. (2008). Does structural complexity necessarily imply processing difficulty? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 37(1), 2131Google Scholar
Gentile, S. (2003). On the acquisition of left-branching recursive possessives. Bachelor’s Honors Thesis. University of Massachusetts Amherst.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68(1), 176.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. and Thomas, J. (1999). Memory limitations and structural forgetting: the perception of complex ungrammatical sentences as grammatical. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(3), 225248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. (1982). Logic vs. pragmatics, with human language as the referee: toward an empirically viable epistemology. Journal of Pragmatics, 6(2), 81133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. (2009). The genesis of syntactic complexity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, J. and Laks, B. (2016). Battle in the mind fields. Available at http://people.cs.uchicago.edu/~jagoldsm/battle/Google Scholar
Gomes, N. (2002). Observações sobre o Kayabi: a variação da ordem e os clíticos de 2a posição. Master’s Thesis. UFRJ.Google Scholar
Gomes, N. (2007). Clíticos, redobro e variação da ordem oracional em Kaiabi (Tupi-Guarani). Ph.D. Dissertation. UFRJ.Google Scholar
Gomez-Imbert, E. (2007). La vue ou l’ouïe: la modalité cognitive des langues Tukano orientales. In Guentchéva, Z. and Landaburu, J. (eds.), L’énonciation médiatisée II. Le traitement épistémologique de l’information: illustrations amérindiennes et caucasiennes. Leuven: Peeters, 6586.Google Scholar
Gomez-Imbert, E. (2011). La famille tukano. In Bonvini, E., Busuttil, J., and Peyraube, A. (eds.), Dictionnaire des langues du monde. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, 14541460.Google Scholar
González de Pérez, M. S. and Rodríguez de Montes, M. L. (eds.). (2000). Lenguas indígenas de Colombia: una visión descriptiva. Santa Fe, Bogotá: Instituto Caro y Cuervo.Google Scholar
Gorbet, L. (1976). Diegueño relative clauses. In Gorbet, L. P. (ed.) A grammar of diegueño nominals. New York: Garland Pub.Google Scholar
Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., and Johnson, M. (2001). Memory interference during language processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 27(6), 14111423.Google Scholar
Graffi, G. (2015). Some reflections on the notion of recursion. In Busà, M. G. and Gesuato, S. (eds.), Lingue e contesti: studi in onore di Alberto M. Mioni. Padova: Coop. Libraria Editrice Università di Padova, 447456.Google Scholar
Grano, T. (2012). Control and restructuring at the syntax-semantics interface. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Greenberg, J. H. (1963). Some universals of grammar with particular reference to the order of meaningful elements. In Greenberg, J. H. (ed.), Universals of language. London: MIT Press, 73113.Google Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In Cole, P. and Morgan, J. L. (eds.), Syntax and semantics. Vol. 3: speech acts. New York: Academic Press, 4158.Google Scholar
Grillo, N. (2009). Generalized minimality: feature impoverishment and comprehension deficits in agrammatism. Lingua, 119(10), 14261443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grimm, J. and Grimm, W. (1812–1815). Kinder- und Hausmärchen. Berlin: Realschulbuchhandlung.Google Scholar
Grodzinsky, Y. (1989). Agrammatic comprehension of relative clauses. Brain and Language, 37(3), 480499.Google Scholar
Gu, C. (2008). Structural ambiguity and AP/PP recursion in language acquisition. Manuscript. University of Massachusetts Amherst.Google Scholar
Haan, F. de (2001a). The place of inference within the evidential system. International Journal of American Linguistics, 67(2), 193219.Google Scholar
Haan, F. de (2001b). The cognitive basis of visual evidentials. In Cienki, A., Luka, B. J., and Smith, M. B. (eds.), Conceptual and discourse factors in linguistic structure. Stanford: CSLI Publications, 91106, 222.Google Scholar
Hale, K. (1975). Gaps in grammar and culture. In Kinkade, M. D., Hale, K. L., and Werner, O. (eds.), Linguistics and anthropology: in honor of C.F. Voegelin. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 295315.Google Scholar
Hale, K. (1976). The adjoined relative clause in Australia. In Dixon, R. (ed.), Grammatical categories in Australian languages. Canberra: Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies, 78105.Google Scholar
Hale, K. and Keyser, S. J. (1993). On argument structure and the lexical expression of syntactic relations. In Hale, K. and Keyser, S. J. (eds.), The view from building 20. Cambridge: MIT Press, 53109.Google Scholar
Halle, M. and Marantz, A. (1993). Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In Hale, K. and Keyser, S. J. (eds.), The view from building 20. Cambridge: MIT Press, 111176.Google Scholar
Hamburger, H. and Crain, S. (1982). Relative acquisition. In Kuczaj, S. (ed.), Language development. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, 245273.Google Scholar
Han, C. (1998). The structure and interpretation of imperatives. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Harder, P. (2010). Over the top – recursion as a functional option. In van der Hulst, H., Koster, J., and van Riemsdijk, H. (eds.), Recursion and human language. Berlin/New York: De Gruyter Mouton, 233244.Google Scholar
Harley, H. (2008). On the causative construction. In Miyagawa, S. and Saito, M. (eds.), Oxford handbook of Japanese linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2053.Google Scholar
Hauser, M. (2006). Moral minds: how nature designed a universal sense of right and wrong. New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., and Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298, 15691579.Google Scholar
Hawkins, J. A. (1994). A performance theory of order and constituency. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Healey, P. M (1966). Levels and chaining in Telefol sentences. In Wurm, S. A. (ed.), Pacific linguistics, series B, no. 5. Canberra: The Australian National University.Google Scholar
Heine, B. and Kuteva, T. (2005). Language contact and grammatical change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hernández Pina, F. (1984) Teorías psicolingüísticas y su aplicación a la adquisición del español como lengua materna. Madrid: Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
Hintz, D. J. and Hintz, D. M. (2014). The evidential category of mutual knowledge in Quechua. Lingua, 88109, 186187.Google Scholar
Hiraga, M. (2010). Acquisition of recursive verbal compound nouns. Manuscript. University of Massachusetts Amherst.Google Scholar
Hollebrandse, B. (2000). The acquisition of sequence of tense. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of Massachusetts Amherst.Google Scholar
Hollebrandse, B. (2007). A special case of wh-extraction in child language. Lingua, 117(11), 18971906.Google Scholar
Hollebrandse, B. (2010). Van enkelvoudige inbedding naar meervoudige inbedding: een tegelzetters-probleem? In Honor of Jan Koster. TABU 38ste jaargang 2009/2010. Groningen: University of Groningen.Google Scholar
Hollebrandse, B. and Roeper, T. (2007). Recursion and propositional exclusivity. Presentation at Recursion in Human Languages conference, April 27–29. Normal: University of Illinois.Google Scholar
Hollebrandse, B. and Roeper, T. (2014). Empirical results and formal approaches to recursion in acquisition. In Roeper, T. and Speas, M. (eds.), Recursion: complexity in cognition. Dordrecht: Springer, 179219.Google Scholar
Hollebrandse, B., Hobbs, K., de Villiers, J., and Roeper, T. (2008). Second order embedding and second order false belief. In Gavarró, A. and Freitas, M. J. (eds.), Proceedings of generative approaches to language acquisition. GALA 2007. Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Publishing, 270280.Google Scholar
Hollebrandse, B., van Hout, A., and Hendriks, P. (2014). Children’s first and second-order false-belief reasoning in a verbal and a low-verbal task. Synthese, 191(3), 321333.Google Scholar
Holmer, A. (2005). Seediq: Antisymmetry and final particles in a Formosan VOS language. In Carnie, A., Harley, H., and Dooley-Colburg, S. (eds.), Verb first: studies in predicate initial languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 175202.Google Scholar
Hornstein, N. (2009). A theory of syntax: minimal operations and Universal Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hualde, J. I. and Ortiz de Urbina, J. (2003). A grammar of Basque. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, Y., Spelke, E., and Snedeker, J. (2010). When is four far more than three? Children’s generalization of newly acquired number words. Psychological Science, 21(4), 600606.Google Scholar
Huang, Y., Spelke, E., and Snedeker, J. (2013). What exactly do numbers mean? Language Learning and Development, 9(2),105129.Google Scholar
Hulst, H. van der (2010). Re recursion. In van der Hulst, H., Koster, J., and van Riemsdijk, H. (eds.), Recursion and human language. Berlin/New York: De Gruyter Mouton, iliii.Google Scholar
Hunsicker, D. and Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). Hierarchical structure in a self-created communication system: building nominal constituents in homesign. Language, 88(4), 732763.Google Scholar
Hunyadi, L. (2010) Cognitive grouping and recursion in prosody. In van der Hulst, H. (ed.), Recursion and human language. Berlin/New York: De Gruyter Mouton, 343370.Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (1913). Logical investigations (published as Logische Untersuchungen, 2nd edition). Halle: M. Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Jacobsen, W. (1967). Switch-reference in Hokan-Coahuiltec. In Hymes, D. H. and Bittle, W. E. (eds.), Studies in southwestern ethnolinguistics. The Hague: Mouton, 238263.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1941). Kindersprache, Aphasie und allgemeine Lautgesetze. Uppsala: Almqvist and Wiksell.Google Scholar
Jakubowicz, C. (2003). Computational complexity and the acquisition of functional categories by French-speaking children with SLI. Linguistics, 41(2), 175211.Google Scholar
Jakubowicz, C. (2011). Measuring derivational complexity: new evidence from typically developing and SLI learners of L1 French. Lingua, 121(3), 339351.Google Scholar
Joshi, A. (2014). Recursion in language: is it indirectly constrained? In Roeper, T. and Speas, M. (eds.), Recursion: complexity in cognition. New York: Springer, 139158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, J. and Postal, P. (1964). An integrated theory of linguistic descriptions. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kaufman, A. and Kaufman, S. (2004). Kaufman assessment battery for children (2nd edition). San Antonio, TX: Pearson.Google Scholar
Kaufmann, M. (2011). Interpreting imperatives. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Kaufmann, M. (2014). Embedded imperatives across languages: Too rare to expect, too frequent to ban. Colloquium, Department of Linguistics, SUNY Stony Brook.Google Scholar
Kayne, R. S. (1994). The antisymmetry of syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Keenan, E. (1976). Remarkable subjects in Malagasy. In Li, C. N. (ed.), Subject and topic. New York: Academic Press, 246301.Google Scholar
Kendall, M. B. (1974). Relative clause formation and topicalization in Yavapai. International Journal of American Linguistics, 40(2), 89101.Google Scholar
Kidd, E., Stewart, T., and Serratrice, L. (2009). Children do not overcome lexical biases where adults do: the role of the referential scene in garden-path recovery. Journal of Child Language, 38(1), 222234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, A. and Gilley, P. (2013). Neural mechanisms of rapid sensitivity to syntactic anomaly. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 45.Google Scholar
Kimball, J. (1973). Seven principles of surface structure parsing in natural language. Cognition, 2(1), 1547.Google Scholar
Klem, M., Melby-Lervag, M., Hagtvet, B., Lyster, S., Gustafsson, J., and Hulme, C. (2015). Sentence repetition is a measure of children’s language skills rather than working memory limitations. Developmental Sciences, 18(1), 146154.Google Scholar
Koster, J. (2000). Recursion and the lexicon. In Van der Hulst, H. (ed.), Recursion and human language. New York: De Gruyter, 285298.Google Scholar
Krapova, I. (2009). Bulgarian relative and factive clauses with an invariant complementizer. Lingua, 120(5),12401272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kratzer, A. (2013). Modality and the semantics of embedding. Slides from presentation at the Amsterdam Colloquium, December 2013.Google Scholar
Krifka, M. (2014). Embedding illocutionary acts. In Roeper, T. and Speas, M. (eds.), Recursion: complexity in cognition (Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics 43). Berlin: Springer, 125155.Google Scholar
Kutas, M. and Hillyard, S. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203205.Google Scholar
Kutas, M. and Hillyard, S. (1984). Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature, 307(5947), 161163.Google Scholar
Labelle, M. (1990). Predication, wh-movement and the development of relative clauses. Language Acquisition, 1(1), 99119.Google Scholar
Ladd, D. R. (1986). Intonational phrasing: the case for recursive prosodic structure. Phonology Yearbook, 3, 311340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladd, D. R. (1988). Declination ‘reset’ and the hierarchical organization of utterances. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 84(2), 530544.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1986). Frame semantic control of the coordinate structure constraint. Paper from the 21st Annual Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, Parasession on Pragmatics and Grammatical Theory. Available at: https://georgelakoff.files.wordpress.com/2011/04/frame-semantic-control-of-the-coordinate-structure-constraint-lakoff-1986.pdfGoogle Scholar
Landau, B. and Gleitman, L. R. (1985). Language and experience: evidence from the blind child. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Landau, I. (2000). Elements of control: structure and meaning in infinitival constructions. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands.Google Scholar
Landau, I. (2004). The scale of finiteness and the calculus of control. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 22(4): 811877.Google Scholar
Lander, Y. A. and Letuchiy, A. B. (2010). Kinds of recursion in Adyghe morphology. In van der Hulst, H. (ed.), Recursion and human language. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, 263284.Google Scholar
Landin, D. (1984). An outline of the syntactic structure of Karitiana sentences. In Dooley, R. (ed.), Estudos sobre línguas Tupi do Brasil. Brasilia: SIL, 219254.Google Scholar
Langdon, M. and Munro, P. (1979). Subject and (switch-)reference in Yuman. Folia Linguistica, 13, 321344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langendoen, T., McDaniel, D., and Langsam, Y. (1989). Preposition-phrase attachment in noun phrases. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18(6), 533548.Google Scholar
Larson, R. K. (1988). On the double object construction. Linguistic Inquiry, 19(3), 335391.Google Scholar
Lasnik, H. (2011). What kind of computing device is the human language faculty? In Di Scullo, A. M. and Boecks, C. (eds.), The biolinguistic enterprise. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 354365.Google Scholar
Lau, E., Holcomb, P., and Kupperberg, G. (2012). Dissociating N400 effects of prediction from association in single-word contexts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(3), 484502.Google Scholar
Lau, E., Phillips, C., and Poeppel, D. (2008). A cortical network for semantics: (de)constructing the N400. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(12), 920933.Google Scholar
Lauer, S. (2013). Towards a dynamic pragmatics. Ph.D. Dissertation. Stanford University.Google Scholar
Le Corre, M. and Carey, S. (2007). One, two, three, four, nothing more: an investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles. Cognition, 105(2), 395438.Google Scholar
Leandro, W. and Amaral, L. (2014). The interpretation of multiple embedded genitive constructions by Wapichana and English speakers. Linguistica, 10(2), 149162.Google Scholar
Lebeaux, D. (1988). Language acquisition and the form of the grammar. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of Massachusetts Amherst.Google Scholar
Lebeaux, D. (2000). Language acquisition and the form of the grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Legate, J. A., Pesetsky, D., and Yang, C. (2014). Recursive misrepresentations: a reply to Levinson (2013). Language, 90(2), 515528.Google Scholar
Lehrdahl, F. and Jackendoff, R. (1983). A generative theory of tonal music. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Leibniz, G. (1666). De arte combinatoria (On the art of combination).Google Scholar
Leite, Y. F. (1990). Para uma tipologia ativa do Tapirapé. Cadernos de Estudos Lingüísticos, 18, 3756.Google Scholar
Leivada, E. (2015). X-within-X structures and the nature of categories. Biolinguistics, 9, 5073.Google Scholar
Lemos Barbosa, P. (1956). Curso de Tupi Antigo. Rio de Janeiro: Livraria São José.Google Scholar
Levin, N. S. and Prince, E. F. (1986). Gapping and causal implicature. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 19(3), 351364.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. (2013). Recursion in pragmatics. Language, 89(1), 149162.Google Scholar
Lightfoot, D. (1979). Principles of diachronic syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lima, S. and Santos, G. (2008). Levantamento Sociolinguístico no Baixo Xingu. Manuscript. Instituto Socioambiental (ISA).Google Scholar
Lima, S., Kayabi, P., Kaiabi, M., and Kaiabi, M. (in press). Gramática pedagógica da língua Kawaiwete. Rio de Janeiro: Museu do Índio/ UNESCO.Google Scholar
Limbach, M. and Adone, D. (2010). Language acquisition of recursive possessives in English. Proceedings of Boston University Conference on Language Development (BUCLD), 34, 281290.Google Scholar
Lobina, D. and García-Albea, J. (2009). Recursion and cognitive science: data structures and mechanisms. In Taatgen, N. A. and van Rijn, H. (eds.), Proceedings of the 31th annual conference of the cognitive science society, 13471352.Google Scholar
Lobina, D. J. (2014). Probing recursion. Cognitive Processing, 15(4), 435450.Google Scholar
Lynch, J. D. (1978). A grammar of Lenakel. In Pacific linguistics, series B, no. 55. Canberra: The Australian National University.Google Scholar
Lynch, J. D. (1983). Switch-reference in Lenakel. In Haiman, J. and Munro, P. (eds.), Switch-reference and universal grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 209221.Google Scholar
MacDonald, L. (1990). A grammar of Tauya. Berlin: de Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk (3rd edition). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Maia, M. (2007). Evidentiality processes in Karajá. In Guentchéva, Z. and Landaburu, J. (eds.), L’énonciation médiatisée II. Le traitement épistémologique de l’information: illustrations amérindiennes et caucasiennes. Leuven: Peeters, 293308.Google Scholar
Maia, M. (2012). Recursividade e coordenação de PPs. Manuscript. Rio de Janeiro: Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro.Google Scholar
Maia, M. (2015). Sintaxe experimental. In Othero, G. and Kenedy, E. (eds.), Sintaxe, sintaxes. São Paulo: Editora Contexto, 5172.Google Scholar
Malone, T. (1988). The origin and development of Tuyuca evidentials. International Journal of American Linguistics, 54(2), 119140.Google Scholar
Marantz, A. (1997). No escape from syntax: don’t try morphological analysis in the privacy of your own lexicon. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 4(2), 201225.Google Scholar
Marcilese, M. (2011). Sobre o papel da língua no desenvolvimento de habilidades cognitivas superiores: representação, recursividade e cognição numérica. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation. Pontíficia Universidade Católica do Rio de Janeiro.Google Scholar
Marcilese, M., Corrêa, L. M. S., and Augusto, M. R. A. (2013). Recursive pre-nominal modifiers interpretation in language acquisition. In Stavrakaki, S., Lalioti, M., and Konstantinopoulou, P. (eds.), Advances in language acquisition (1st edition). Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing, 138146.Google Scholar
Marcilese, M., Villarinho, C., Augusto, M. R. A., and Corrêa, L. M. S. (2009). Cognitive and linguistic demands in first and second order false belief tasks. Poster presentation at Recursion: structural complexity in language and cognition. Amherst.Google Scholar
Massam, D. (2000). VSO and VOS: aspects of Niuean word order. In Carnie, A. and Guilfoyle, E. (eds.), The syntax of verb initial language. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 97116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Massam, D. (2005). Predicate fronting and lexical category in Niuean. In Carnie, A., Harley, H., and Dooley-Colburg, S. (eds.), Verb first: studies in predicate initial languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 227242.Google Scholar
Matthei, E. (1982). The acquisition of prenominal modifier sequences. Cognition, 11(3), 301332.Google Scholar
Matthews, P. H. (1991). Morphology (2nd edition). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Matthewson, L. (2004). On the methodology of semantic fieldwork. International Journal of American Linguistics, 70(4), 369415.Google Scholar
Matthewson, L. (2012). Evidence about evidentials: where fieldwork meets theory. In Stolterfoht, B. and Featherston, S. (eds.), Empirical approaches to linguistic theory: studies in meaning and structure. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter, 85114.Google Scholar
Matthewson, L., Davis, H., and Rullman, H. (2007). Evidentials as epistemic modals: evidence from St’át’imcets. In J. van Craenenbroeck (ed.), Linguistic Variation Yearbook 2007. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 201–254.Google Scholar
Mazuka, R., Itoh, K., Kiritani, S., Niwa, S., Ikejiri, K., and Naitoh, K. (1989). Processing of Japanese garden path, center-embedded, and multiply left-embedded sentences. Annual Bulletin of the Research Institute of Logopedics and Phoniatrics, University of Tokyo, 23, 187212.Google Scholar
McCloskey, J. (1996). On the scope of verb movement in Irish. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 14(1), 47104.Google Scholar
McDaniel, D., McKee, C., and Garrett, M. F. (2010). Children’s sentence planning: syntactic correlates of fluency variations. Journal of Child Language, 37(1), 5994.Google Scholar
McKenzie, A. (2015). A survey of switch-reference in North America. International Journal of American Linguistics, 81(3), 409448.Google Scholar
Meira, S. and Franchetto, B. (2005). The Southern Cariban languages and the Cariban family. International Journal of American Linguistics, 71(2), 127190.Google Scholar
Meroni, L. and Crain, S. (2011). How children avoid kindergarten paths. Proceedings of the Fourth Tokyo Conference on Psycholinguistics. Tokyo: Hituzi Publishing Company, 159184Google Scholar
Merx, M. (2016). The production and comprehension of recursive prepositional phrases and possessives in Dutch children and adults. BA Thesis. University of Groningen.Google Scholar
Meyer, M. (2013). Ignorance and grammar. Ph.D. Dissertation. Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Miller, A. (2001). A grammar of Jamul Tiipay. Berlin: de Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Miller, G. (1956). The magical number seven plus or minus two: some limits in our capacity to process information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 8197.Google Scholar
Mithun, M. (2009). Re(e)volving complexity: adding intonation. In Givón, T. and Shibatani, M. (eds.), Syntactic complexity: diachrony, acquisition, neuro-cognition, evolution. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 5380.Google Scholar
Mithun, M. (2010). The fluidity of recursion and its implications. In van der Hulst, H., Koster, J., and van Riemsdijk, H. (eds.), Recursion and human language. Berlin/New York: De Gruyter Mouton, 1541.Google Scholar
Mix, K. S., Sandhofer, C., and Baroody, A. (2005). Number words and number concepts: the interplay of verbal and nonverbal quantification in early childhood. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 33, 305346.Google Scholar
Moore, D. (2006). Cláusulas relativas de Gavião de Rondônia. Boletim do Museu Paraense Emilio Goeldi, 1, 135143.Google Scholar
Moore, D. (2011). As línguas indigenas no Brasil hoje. In Mello, H., Altenhofen, C. and Raso, T. (eds.), Os Contatos linguísticos no Brasil. Belo Horizonte: Editora UFMG, 217240.Google Scholar
Moore, G. E. (1942). A reply to my critics. In Schlipp, P. A. (ed.), The philosophy of G. E. Moore. Evanston: Northwestern University, 535677.Google Scholar
Munn, A. (1993). Topics in the syntax and semantics of coordinate structures. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of Maryland, College Park.Google Scholar
Munn, A. (1995). The possessor that stayed close to home. In Samian, V. and Schaeffer, J. (eds.), Proceedings of the Western Conference on Linguistics, 24. Fresno: California State University, Fresno, 181195.Google Scholar
Munro, P. (ed.) (1980). Studies of switch-reference. UCLA Papers in Syntax 8. Department of Linguistics, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Munro, R., Rainer, L., Sauerland, S., and Fleck, D. W. (2012). Reported speech in Matses: perspective persistence and evidential narratives. International Journal of American Linguistics, 78(1), 4175.Google Scholar
Murray, S. (2010). Evidentiality and the structure of speech acts. Ph.D. Dissertation. Rutgers University.Google Scholar
Nadig, A. S. and Sedivy, J. (2002). Evidence of perspective-taking constraints in children’s on-line reference resolution. Psychological Science, 13(4) 329336.Google Scholar
Nascimento, M. and Maia, M. (2014). O processamento de sintagmas posposicionais e preposicionais encaixados e coordenados em Kaingang e em Português. Poster presented during the XXIX ENANPOLL, UFSC.Google Scholar
Nelson, J. S. (2016). First and second language acquisition of recursive operations: two studies. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of Massachusetts Amherst.Google Scholar
Nevins, A., Pesetsky, D., and Rodrigues, C. (2009a). Evidence and argumentation: a reply to Everett (2009a). Language, 85(3), 671681.Google Scholar
Nevins, A., Pesetsky, D., and Rodrigues, C. (2009b). Pirahã exceptionality: a reassessment. Language, 85(2), 355404.Google Scholar
Nichols, L. (2000). Rethinking switch reference. In Carnie, A., Jelinek, E., and Willie, M.. (eds.), Papers in honor of Ken Hale: working papers on endangered and less familiar languages. Cambridge: MIT Working Papers in Linguistics, 518.Google Scholar
Nikolaeva, I. and Tolskaya, M. (2001). A grammar of Udihe. Berlin: de Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Nimuendajú, C. (1948). The Mura and Pirahã. In Steward, J. (ed.), Handbook of South American Indians (vol. 3). Washington: Smithsonian Institution, Bureau of American Ethnology, 255269.Google Scholar
Ninio, A. (2011). Syntactic development, its input and output. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nishiguchi, S. (2006). Categorical and semantic ambiguity of possessives and adjectives. In Oba, Y. and Okada, S. (eds.), Osaka University Papers in English Linguistics, 11. Osaka: Osaka University, English Linguistics Section, 7383.Google Scholar
Nishiguchi, S. (2009). Possessive disambiguation. Ph.D. Dissertation. Osaka University.Google Scholar
Nonato, R. (2014). Clause chaining, switch reference and coordination. Ph.D. Dissertation. Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Nordlinger, R. (2006). Spearing the Emu drinking: subordination and the adjoined relative clause in Wambaya. Australian Journal of Linguistics, 26(1), 529.Google Scholar
Novogrodsky, R. and Friedmann, N. (2006). The production of relative clauses in SLI: a window to the nature of the impairment. Advances in Speech-Language Pathology, 8(4), 364375.Google Scholar
Nunes, J. (2001). Sideward movement. Linguistic Inquiry, 32(2), 303344.Google Scholar
Nuwer, M. R., Comi, G., Emerson, R., Fuglsang-Frederiksen, A., Guérit, J-M., Hinrichs, H., Ikeda, A., Lucas, F. J., and Rappelsburger, P. (1998). IFCN standards for digital recording of clinical EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 106(3), 259261.Google Scholar
Osthoff, H. and Brugmann, K. (1878). Vorwort. Morphologische Untersuchungen 1, iiixx.Google Scholar
Oswalt, R. L. (1961). A Kashaya grammar (Southwestern Pomo). Ph.D. Dissertation. University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Palmer, F. R. (1986). Mood and modality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy-psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1–2), 813.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pérez-Leroux, A. T., Castilla-Earles, A. P., Béjar, S., and Massam, D. (2012). Elmo’s sister’s ball: the problem of acquiring nominal recursion. Language Acquisition, 19(4), 301311.Google Scholar
Pesetsky, D. (1995). Zero syntax: experiencers and cascades. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Peterson, T. (2010). Evidentiality and epistemic modality in Gitksan at the semantics-pragmatics interface. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of British Columbia.Google Scholar
Peterson, T., Pérez-Leroux, A. T., Castilla-Earls, A. P., Béjar, S., and Massam, D. (2015) Structural complexity and the acquisition of recursive locative PPs. Proceedings of NELS 45. Amherst: GLSA.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. and Jackendoff, R. (2005). The faculty of language: what’s special about it? Cognition, 95, 201236.Google Scholar
Platzack, C. and Rosengren, I. (1997). On the subject of imperatives: a minimalist account of the imperative clause. Journal of Comparative Germanic Linguistics, 1(3), 177224.Google Scholar
Plungian, V. A. (2001). The place of evidentiality within the universal grammatical space. Journal of Pragmatics, 33(3), 349357.Google Scholar
Pollard, C. and Sag, I. (1994). Head-driven phrase structure grammar. Stanford: CSLI.Google Scholar
Posner, R. (1980). Semantics and pragmatics of sentence connectives in natural language. In Searle, J. R., Kierfer, F., and Bierwisch, M. (eds.), Speech act theory and pragmatics. Dordrecht: Springer, 169203.Google Scholar
Post, E. (1943). Formal reductions of the general combinatory decision problem. American Journal of Mathematics, 65(2), 197215Google Scholar
Postal, P. M. (1998). Three investigations of extraction. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Power, J. (2002). Notes on formal language theory and parsing. Manuscript. Department of Computer Science, National University of Ireland.Google Scholar
Pyers, J. E. and Senghas, A. (2009). Language promotes false-belief understanding. Psychological Science, 20(7), 805812.Google Scholar
Pylkkänen, L. (2002). Introducing arguments. Ph.D. Dissertation. Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Rackowski, A. and Travis, L. (2000). V-initial languages: X or XP movement and adverbial placement. In Carnie, A. and Guilfoyle, E. (eds.), The syntax of verb-initial languages. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 117142.Google Scholar
Ramirez, H. (1997). A Fala Tukano dos Ye’pâ-Masa, Tomo I Gramática. Manaus: CEDEM.Google Scholar
Reesnik, G. P. (1983). Switch reference and topicality hierarchies. Studies in Language, 7(2), 215246.Google Scholar
Reich, I. (2008). From discourse to “odd coordination”: on asymmetric coordination and subject gaps in German. In Fabricius-Hansen, C. and Ramm, W. (eds.), Subordination’ versus ‘coordination’ in sentence and text: a cross-linguistic perspective. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 281303.Google Scholar
Reinhart, T. (1999). The processing cost of reference-set computation: guess patterns in acquisition. OTS Working Papers in Linguistics 99(1). Utrecht: Utrecht University. Available at: https://dspace.library.uu.nl/handle/1874/17183.Google Scholar
Rivero, M. L. and Terzi, A. (1995). Imperatives, V-movement and logical mood. Journal of Linguistics, 31(2), 301322.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. (1982). Issues in Italian syntax. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. (2013). Introduction: core computational principles in natural language syntax. Lingua, 130, 113.Google Scholar
Roberts, I. (2007). Introduction. In Roberts, I. (ed.), Comparative grammar. Volume one: early work pre-1980. London: Routledge, 140.Google Scholar
Rocha, I. (2011). A estrutura argumental da língua Karitiana: desafios descritivos e teóricos. Master’s Thesis. Universidade de São Paulo.Google Scholar
Rocha, I. (2013). Estrutura argumental dos verbos em orações subordinadas em karitiana: valência verbal e sua interação com núcleos funcionais de aspecto (Ph.D. Grant Report – FAPESP). Manuscript. Universidade de São Paulo.Google Scholar
Rodgers, P. and Black, P.E. (2004). Recursive data structure. In Pieterse, V. and Black, P. E. (eds.), Dictionary of algorithms and data structures. Online at: www.nist.gov/dads/HTML/recursivstrc.htmlGoogle Scholar
Rodrigues, A. D. (1986). Línguas brasileiras: para o conhecimento das linguas indígenas. São Paulo: Loyola.Google Scholar
Rodrigues, C. (2004). Impoverished morphology and movement out of Case domains. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of Maryland at College Park.Google Scholar
Rodrigues, E. and Marcilese, M. (2014) Executive control in children’s comprehension of ordinal modifier phrases. In Costa, J., Fiéis, A., Freitas, M. J., Lobo, M., and Santos, A. L.. (eds.), New directions in the acquisition of romance languages: selected proceedings of the romance turn V (1st edition). Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing, 241260.Google Scholar
Roeper, T. (1972). Approaches to a theory of language acquisition with examples from German children. Ph.D. Dissertation. Harvard University.Google Scholar
Roeper, T. (2007). The prism of grammar: how child language illuminates humanism. Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roeper, T. (2010). Recursion: what is innate, why it needs a trigger, where it belongs in cross-linguistic work and how it fits into the mind. In França, A. and Maia, M. (eds.), Papers in psycholinguistics. Rio de Janeiro: Imprinta, 3860.Google Scholar
Roeper, T. (2011). The acquisition of recursion: how formalism articulates the child’s path. Biolinguistics, 5(1–2), 5786.Google Scholar
Roeper, T. (2013). Recursion typology and the acquisition path. Paper presented at Recursion in Brazilian Languages and Beyond 2013, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, August 7, 2013.Google Scholar
Roeper, T. (2014). Strict interface principles and the acquisition engine: from unlabelled to labelled and minimal modular contact. Language Sciences, 46(B), 115132.Google Scholar
Roeper, T. and de Villiers, J. (2011). The acquisition path of wh-Movement. In de Villiers, J. and Roeper, T. (eds.), Handbook of generative approaches to language acquisition. Dordrecht: Springer, 189246.Google Scholar
Roeper, T. and Pérez-Leroux, A. T. (2011). Simplicity and complexity in child language and its explanation. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 34(3), 363380.Google Scholar
Roeper, T. and Snyder, W. (2004). Recursion as an analytic device in acquisition. In Proceedings of GALA 2003 – Generative Approach to Language Acquisition. Utrecht: LOT Publications.Google Scholar
Roeper, T. and Snyder, W. (2005). Language learnability and the forms of recursion. In Di Sciullo, A. M. and Delmonte, R. (eds.), UG and external systems: language, brain and computation. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 155169.Google Scholar
Roeper, T. and Speas, M. (2014). Recursion: complexity in cognition. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Ross, J. (1967). Constraints on variables in syntax. Ph.D. Dissertation. Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Rummer, R., Engelkamp, J., and Konieczny, L. (2003). The subordination effect: evidence from self-paced reading and recall. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 15(4), 539566.Google Scholar
Rus, D. (2005). Slovenian embedded imperatives. In Brandstetter, C. and Rus, D. (eds.), Georgetown Working Papers in Linguistics 4. Washington, DC: Georgetown Department of Linguistics, 153183.Google Scholar
Russell, B. (1905) On denoting. Mind. Available at http://en.wikisource.org/wiki/On_Denoting.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, B. and Whitehead, A. N. (1910–1912). Principia mathematica. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sadock, J. and Zwicky, A. (1985). Speech act distinctions in syntax. In Shopen, T. (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description (vol. 1). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 155196.Google Scholar
Sag, I., Wasow, T., and Bender, E. (2003). Syntactic theory: a formal introduction. Stanford: CSLI.Google Scholar
Salles, R. (2015). Understanding recursion and looking for self-embedding in Pirahã: the case of the possessive constructions. MA Thesis. Catholic University of Rio de Janeiro.Google Scholar
Sanchez-Mendes, L. (2013). Degree modification on the adjectival and on the verbal domain in Karitiana. Presentation at CILLA VI. University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Sanchez-Mendes, L. (2014). A modificação de grau em Karitiana. Ph.D. Dissertation. Universidade de São Paulo.Google Scholar
Sandhofer, C. and Smith, L. (1999). Learning color words involves learning a system of mappings. Developmental Psychology, 35(3), 668679.Google Scholar
Sarnecka, B., Kamenskaya, V., Yamana, Y., Ogura, T., and Yudovina, Y. (2007). From grammatical number to exact numbers: early meanings of ‘one’, ‘two’, and ‘three’ in English, Russian, and Japanese. Cognitive Psychology, 55(2), 136168.Google Scholar
Sauerland, U. (2010a). Embedded implicatures and experimental constraints: a reply to Geurts and Pouscoulous and Chemla. Semantics and Pragmatics, 3(2), 113.Google Scholar
Sauerland, U. (2010b). Experimental evidence for complex syntax in Pirahã. Unpublished. Available online: http://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/001095Google Scholar
Sauerland, U. (2015). Against complexity parameters. In Trotzke, A. and Bayer, J. (eds.), Syntactic complexity across interfaces. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 924.Google Scholar
Sauerland, U. and Schenner, M. (2006). Embedded evidentials in Bulgarian. In Puig-Waldmüller, E. (ed.), Proceedings of Sinn und Bedeutung 11. Barcelona: Universitat Pompeu Fabra, 525539.Google Scholar
Sauerland, U. and Trotzke, A. (2011). Biolinguistic perspectives on recursion: introduction to the special issue. Biolinguistics, 5(1–2), 19.Google Scholar
Sauerland, U., Kratochvil, F., and Hollebrandse, B. (in prep). Complexity before evolution: complement clauses in Teiwa.Google Scholar
Saussure, F. de (1916). Cours de linguistique générale. Published by C. Bally and A. Sechehaye with de A. Riedlinger. Lausanne: Payot.Google Scholar
Schick, B., de Villiers, J., de Villiers, P., and Hoffmeister, R. (2007). Language and Theory of Mind: a study of deaf children. Child Development, 78(2), 376396.Google Scholar
Schlegel, F. (1808). Über die Sprache und Weisheit der Indier. Ein Beitrag zur Begründung der Alterthumskunde. Heidelberg: Mohr and Winter.Google Scholar
Schmerling, S. (1975). Asymmetric conjunction and rules of conversation. Syntax and Semantics, 3, 211231.Google Scholar
Schmidt, M. (1942). Los Kayabi en Mato-Grosso (Brasil). Sociedad Cientfica del Paraguay.Google Scholar
Sevcenco, A. and Avram, L. (2012). Romanian-speaking children’s comprehension of relatives. RRL, LVII 2, 219239.Google Scholar
Sevcenco, A., Roeper, T., and Pearson, B. (2016). The acquisition of recursive locative PPs and relative clauses in Child English. 12 Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition (GALA 12). Nantes, September 10–12.Google Scholar
Silva, G. R. (2014). Promising evidence for syntactic asymmetries from Topic, Focus and word order inside the CPs in Pirahã (Mura family). Manuscript. Campinas: University of Campinas.Google Scholar
Silva, G. Romling da and Franchetto, B. (2011). Prosodic distinctions between the varieties of the Upper-Xingu Carib language: results of an acoustic analysis. Amerindia, 35, 4152.Google Scholar
Silva, W. de Lima (2012). A descriptive grammar of Desano. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of Utah.Google Scholar
Silverstein, M. (1976). Hierarchies of features and ergativity. In Dixon, R. M. W. (ed.), Grammatical categories in Australian languages. Canberra: Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies, 112171.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (2007). Context and comments on Dan Everett’s claims (Letter to the editor). Human Development (online supplement), 50(6). Online: www.karger.com/ProdukteDB/Katalogteile/issn/_0018_716X/HDE-Letters-to-Editor-12-13-2007.pdfGoogle Scholar
Snedeker, J. and Trueswell, J. (2004). The developing constraints on parsing decisions: the role of lexical-biases and referential scenes in child and adult sentence processing. Cognitive Psychology, 49(3), 238299.Google Scholar
Snyder, W. and Roeper, T. (2004). Learnability and recursion across categories. In Brugos, A., Micciulla, L. and Smith, C. E. (eds.), BUCLD 28: Proceedings of the 28th Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville: Cascadilla Press, 543552.Google Scholar
Sorensen, R. (2013). Epistemic paradoxes. In Zalta, E. N. (ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Available at: https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/epistemic-paradoxes/Google Scholar
Southgate, V., Senju, A., and Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18(7), 587592.Google Scholar
Souza, P. (2004). Estudos de aspectos da língua Kayabi (Tupi). MA Thesis. UNICAMP.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S. and Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10(1), 8996.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S. and Tsivkin, S. (2001). Initial knowledge and conceptual change: space and number. In Bowerman, M. and Levinson, S. (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 7098.Google Scholar
Stabler, E. (2014). Recursion in grammar and performance. In Roeper, T. and Speas, M. (eds.), Recursion: complexity and cognition. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 159177.Google Scholar
Stalnaker, R. (1978). Assertion. In Cole, P. (ed.), Syntax and semantics 9: pragmatics, New York: Academic Press, 315332.Google Scholar
Stapert, E. (2007). Linguistic theory and fieldwork in interaction: the case of Pirahã. In Austin, P. K., Bond, O., and Nathan, D. (eds.), Proceedings of Conference on Language Documentation and Linguistic Theory. London: SOAS, 235242.Google Scholar
Stenzel, K. (2007). The semantics of serial verb constructions in two Eastern Tukanoan languages: Kotiria (Wanano) and Waikhana (Piratapuyo). In Deal, A. R. (ed.), Proceedings of 4th conference on the semantics of under-represented languages in the Americas. University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers 35. Amherst: GLSA, 275290.Google Scholar
Stenzel, K. (2008). Evidentials and clause modality in Wanano. Studies in Language, 32(2), 404444.Google Scholar
Stenzel, K. (2013). A reference grammar of Kotiria (Wanano). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Stenzel, K. (2016). More on switch-reference in Kotiria (Wanano, East Tukano). In van Gijn, R. and Hammond, J. (eds.), Switch-Reference 2.0. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 425452.Google Scholar
Stenzel, K. (in prep). From noise-making to nonvisual evidence: Genesis of an evidential category.Google Scholar
Stenzel, K. and Gomez-Imbert, E. (2018). Evidentiality in Tukanoan languages. In Aikhenvald, A. Y. (ed.), The Oxford handbook of evidentiality. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 357387.Google Scholar
Stenzel, K., Marques, T., Galvez Trinidade, J., and Wacho Cabral, M. (2017). Kotiria. In K. Stenzel and B. Franchetto (eds.), On this and other worlds: voices from Amazonia. Berlin: Language Science Press, 289275.Google Scholar
Storto, L. (1999). Aspects of a Karitiana grammar. Ph.D. Dissertation. Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Storto, L. (2010). Copular constructions in Karitiana: a case against case movement. 5th Conference of the Semantics of Under-represented Languages of the Americas. Cambridge: Harvard/MIT. May 15–17.Google Scholar
Storto, L. (2012). Subordination in Karitiana. Amerindia, 35, 219237.Google Scholar
Storto, L. (2013). Temporal and aspectual interpretations in non-finite clauses. In Molsing, K. V. and Tramunt Ibaños, A. M. (eds.), Time and TAME in language. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing, 7189.Google Scholar
Storto, L. (2014). Constituent order and information structure in Karitiana. In van Gijn, R., Matić, D., van Putten, S., and Galucio, A. V. (eds.), Information structure and reference tracking in complex sentences. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 163192.Google Scholar
Storto, L. and Rocha, I. (2014). Strategies of valence change in Karitiana. In Queixalos, F., Telles, S., and Bruno, A. C. (eds.), Incremento de valencia en las lenguas amazónicas. Bogotá: Instituto Caro y Cuervo, 5169.Google Scholar
Strom, C. (1992). Retuarã syntax. Arlington: Summer Institute of Linguistics/University of Texas.Google Scholar
Tavakolian, S. (1981). The conjoined clause analysis of relative clauses. In Tavakolian, S. (ed.), Language acquisition and linguistic theory. Cambridge: MIT Press, 167187.Google Scholar
Tomalin, M. (2003). Linguistics and the formal sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tomalin, M. (2006). Linguistics and the formal sciences: the origins of generative grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tomalin, M. (2011). Syntactic structures and recursive devices: a legacy of imprecision. Journal of Logic, Language, and Information, 20(3), 297315.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2000). Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition, 74(3), 209253.Google Scholar
Trotzke, A., Bader, M., and Frazier, L. (2013). Third factors and the performance interface in language design. Biolinguistics, 7, 134.Google Scholar
Trueswell, J. C., Sekerina, I., Hill, N. M., and Logrip, M. L. (1999) The kindergarten-path effect. Cognition, 73(2), 89134.Google Scholar
Uriagereka, J. (1999). Multiple spell-out. In Epstein, S. D. and Hornstein, N. (eds.), Working minimalism. Cambridge: MIT Press, 251282Google Scholar
Valian, V. (2009). Abstract linguistic representations and innateness: the development of determiners. In Lewis, W. D., Karimi, S., Harley, H., and Farrar, S. (eds.), Time and again: theoretical perspectives on formal linguistics in honor of D. Terence Langendoen. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 189206.Google Scholar
van Hout, A., Kamiya, M., and Roeper, T. (2013). Passivization, reconstruction and edge phenomena: connecting English and Japanese nominalizations. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 31(1), 137159.Google Scholar
Vieira, M. (1995). The expression of quantificational notions in Asurini do Trocará: against the universality of determiner quantification. In Bach, E., Jelinek, E., Kratzer, A., and Partee, B. (eds.), Quantification in natural languages. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers, 701720.Google Scholar
Villarinho, C. N. G. (2012). Um papel para a língua no desenvolvimento de habilidades cognitivas superiores: o traço de ponto-de-vista em estruturas completivas e o domínio de crenças falsas. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation. PUC-Rio.Google Scholar
Villas Boas, O. and Villas Boas, C.. (1989). Xingu: Os Kayabi do rio São Manuel. Porto Alegre: Kuarup.Google Scholar
Villiers, J. de (1999). Language and Theory of Mind: what are the developmental relationships? In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Cohen, D. (eds.), Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism and developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd edition). Oxford: Oxford University Press, 83123.Google Scholar
Villiers, J. de (2005). Can language acquisition give children a point of view? In Astington, J. and Baird, J. (eds.), Why language matters for Theory of Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 186219.Google Scholar
Villiers, J. de (2007). The interface of language and Theory of Mind. Lingua, 117(11), 18581878.Google Scholar
Villiers, J. de and de Villiers, P. (2009). Complements enable representation of the contents of false belief: evolution of a theory. In Foster-Cohen, S.. (ed.), Language acquisition. New York: Palgrave Macmillan, 169195.Google Scholar
Villiers, J. de and Pyers, J. E. (2002). Complements to cognition: a longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding. Cognitive Development, 17(1), 10371060.Google Scholar
Villiers, J. de, Roeper, T., Harrington, E., and Gadilauskas, E. (2012). Tense and truth in children’s question answering. In BUCLD 36 Proceedings. Somerville: Cascadilla Press, 152163.Google Scholar
Vivanco, K. (2014). Orações relativas em Karitiana: um estudo experimental. MA Thesis. Universidade de São Paulo.Google Scholar
Vries, M. de (2002). The syntax of relativization. Utrecht: LOT.Google Scholar
Vries, M. de (2006). The syntax of appositive relativization: on specifying coordination, false free relatives and promotion. Linguistic Inquiry, 37(2), 229270.Google Scholar
Wasow, T. and Arnold, J. (2003). Post-verbal constituent ordering in English. In Rohdenburg, G. and Mondorf, B. (eds.), Determinants of grammatical variation in English. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 119154.Google Scholar
Watumull, J. and Roberts, I. (2014). Leibnizian linguistics. Manuscript. University of Cambridge.Google Scholar
Watumull, J., Hauser, M., Roberts, I., and Horstein, N. (2014). On recursion. Frontiers in Psychology: Language Sciences, 4, 17.Google Scholar
Wegener, C. (2012). A grammar of Savosavo. Berlin: de Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Weighall, A. (2008). The kindergarten path effect revisited: children’s use of context in processing structural ambiguities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99(2), 7595.Google Scholar
Weiss, H. (1998). Para um dicionário da língua Kayabi. Ph.D. Dissertation. Universidade de São Paulo.Google Scholar
Wilkins, J. (1668). An essay towards a real character, and a philosophical language. London: The Royal Society.Google Scholar
Willett, T. (1988). A cross-linguistic survey of the grammaticization of evidentiality. Studies in Language, 12(1), 5197.Google Scholar
Williams, E. (1980). Predication. Linguistic Inquiry, 11(1), 203238.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H. and Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103128.Google Scholar
Wurmbrand, S. (2001). Infinitives: restructuring and clausal structure. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Wurmbrand, S. (2002). Syntactic vs. semantic control. In Zwart, C. J-W. and Abraham, W. (eds.), Studies in comparative Germanic syntax: proceedings of the 15th workshop on comparative Germanic syntax. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 93127.Google Scholar
Wynn, K. (1990). Children’s understanding of counting. Cognition, 36(2), 155193.Google Scholar
Yang, C. (2010). Who’s afraid of George Kingsley Zipf? Unpublished manuscript. www.ling.upenn.edu/~ycharles/papers/zipfnew.pdfGoogle Scholar
Yang, C. (2013). Ontogeny and phylogeny of language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 110(16), 63246327.Google Scholar
Yang, C. and Roeper, T. (2011). Minimalism and language acquisition. In Boeckx, C. (ed.), The Oxford handbook of linguistic minimalism. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 551573.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Edited by Luiz Amaral, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, Marcus Maia, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Andrew Nevins, University College London and Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Tom Roeper, University of Massachusetts, Amherst
  • Book: Recursion across Domains
  • Online publication: 05 June 2018
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Edited by Luiz Amaral, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, Marcus Maia, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Andrew Nevins, University College London and Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Tom Roeper, University of Massachusetts, Amherst
  • Book: Recursion across Domains
  • Online publication: 05 June 2018
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Edited by Luiz Amaral, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, Marcus Maia, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Andrew Nevins, University College London and Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Tom Roeper, University of Massachusetts, Amherst
  • Book: Recursion across Domains
  • Online publication: 05 June 2018
Available formats
×