Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T20:08:48.145Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Demography and Cultural Evolution: How Adaptive Cultural Processes Can Produce Maladaptive Losses—The Tasmanian Case

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 January 2017

Joseph Henrich*
Affiliation:
Emory University, Department of Anthropology, Geosciences Building, Atlanta, GA 30322-1720 (jhenric@emory.edu)

Abstract

A combination of archeological and ethnohistorical evidence indicates that, over an approximately 8,000-year period, from the beginning of the Holocene until European explorers began arriving in the eighteenth century, the societies of Tasmania lost a series of valuable skills and technologies. These likely included bone tools, cold-weather clothing, hafted tools, nets, fishing spears, barbed spears, spear-throwers, and boomerangs. To address this puzzle, and the more general question of how human cognition and social interaction can generate both adaptive cultural evolution and maladaptive losses of culturally acquired skills, this paper constructs a formal model of cultural evolution rooted in the cognitive details of human social learning and inference. The analytical results specify the conditions for differing rates of adaptive cultural evolution, and reveal regimes that will produce maladaptive losses of particular kinds of skills and related technologies. More specifically, the results suggest that the relatively sudden reduction in the effective population size (the size of the interacting pool of social learners) that occurred with the rising ocean levels at the end of the last glacial epoch, which cut Tasmania off from the rest of Australia for the ensuing ten millennia, could have initiated a cultural evolutionary process that (1) kept stable or even improved relatively simple technological skills, and (2) produced an increasing deterioration of more complex skills leading to the complete disappearance of some technologies and practices. This pattern is consistent with the empirical record in Tasmania. Beyond this case, I speculate on the applicability of the model to understanding the variability in rates of adaptive cultural evolution.

Resumen

Resumen

La evidencia arqueológica y etnohistórica indica que, a lo largo de aproximadamente 8,000 años, desde el principio del Holoceno hasta la llegada de exploradores europeos en el sigh XVIII, las sociedades de Tasmania perdieron gran parte de su cultura tecnológica. Las herramientas que desaparecieron probablemente incluyen el hueso, ropa resistente al frío, los instrumentos enmangados, arpones, lanzas de púas, los lanza-lanzadores y los bumerangs. ¿Cómo es posible que se perdiera todo esto? Para resolver este misterio, y también esclarecer deforma más general cómo el conocimiento humano y la interacción social pueden generar adaptaciones y también la pérdida de las mismas, e inclusive malas adaptaciones, en este artículo se construye un modelo formal de la evolución cultural que se basa en detalles cognoscitivos del aprendizaje y la inferencia humanos en el ámbito social. Los resultados analíticos especifican los regímenes de condiciones bajo los cuales la evolución cultural genera adaptaciones, y también los regímenes contrastantes bajo los cuales seproducen pérdidas que representan malas adaptaciones tanto de habilidades como de las tecnologías vinculadas con ellas. Más específicamente, los resultados sugieren que la reducción relativamente repentina en el tamaño eficaz de la población (el tamaño del grupo de aprendices sociales), es la causa más importante de estas pérdidas culturales. El motor ecológico de esta reducción fue el alza del nivel del mar en la época final de la glaciación pasada, que tuvo como efecto separar a Tasmania del resto de Australia durante los últimos diez milenios. La consecuencia fue un deterioro de las habilidades más complejas con las cuales contaba esta población. El expediente empiríco de la arqueología en Tasmania confirma este patrón. Más allá de este caso particular, se presentan especulaciones acerca de la aplicabilidad de este modelo para entender la variabilidad en los índices de la evolución cultural adaptativa en el marco tecnológico.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Society for American Archaeology 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References Cited

Allen, H. 1979 Left Out in the Cold: Why Tasmanians Stopped Eating Fish. The Artefact 4:110.Google Scholar
Alvard, M. 2003 The Adaptive Nature of Culture. Evolutionary Anthropology 12:136147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aunger, R. 2000 The Life History of Culture Learning in a Face-To-Face Society. Ethos 28(2): 138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bassett, E. 2004 Reconsidering Evidence of Tasmanian Fishing. In Worlds Apart, theme issue, edited by Whitehouse, N. J., Murphy, E. M., Plunkett, G., and McCormick, . Environmental Archaeology 9, in press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binford, L. 1983 In Pursuit of the Past: Decoding the Archaeological Record. Thames and Hudson, New York.Google Scholar
Bowdler, S. 1974 Pleistocene Data for Man in Tasmania. Nature 252:697698.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowdler, S. 1980 Fish and Culture: A Tasmanian Polemic. Mankind 12:334340.Google Scholar
Bowdler, S. 1982 Prehistoric Archaeology in Tasmania. In Advances in Word Archaeology, vol. 1, edited by Wendorf, F. and Close, A. E., pp. 149. Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. 1985 Culture and the Evolutionary Process. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
Brooks, A., Helgren, D., Cramer, J. S., Franklin, A., Hornyak, W., Keating, J. M., Klein, R. G., Rink, W. J., Schwarcz, H., Smith, J. N. L., Stewart, K., Todd, N. E., Verniers, J., and Yellen, J. E. 1995 Dating and Context of Three Middle Stone Age Sites with Bone Points in the Upper Semliki Valley, Zaire. Science 268:548553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, K.-H., Cavailli-Sforza, L. L., and Feldman, M. W. 1982 A Study of Cultural Transmission in Taiwan, Human Ecology 10:365382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chibnik, M. 1981 The Evolution of Cultural Rules. Journal of Anthropological Research 37:256268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collett, D. 1994 Engendered Space and Aboriginal Settlement on the Coast of Tasmania: A Preliminary Model. In Archaeology of the North, edited by Sullivan, M., Brockwell, A. and Webb, A., pp. 341357. Northern Australian Research Unit, Darwin.Google Scholar
Colley, S., and Jones, R. 1988 Rocky Cape Revisited—New Light on Prehistoric Tasmanian Fishing. In The Walking Larder, edited by Clutton-Brock, J., pp. 336346. Allen and Unwin, London.Google Scholar
Diamond, J. 1977 Colonization Cycles in Man and Beast. World Archaeology 8:249261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, J. 1978 The Tasmanians: The Longest Isolation, the Simplest Technology. Nature 273:185186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, S. A. 1998 Foundations of Social Evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gott, B. 2002 Fire-Making in Tasmania: Absence of Evidence is Not Evidence. Current Anthropology 43:650655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, J. R. 1998 The Nurture Assumption: Why Children Turn Out the Way They Do. Touchstone, New York.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. 2001 Cultural Transmission and the Diffusion of Innovations: Adoption Dynamics Indicate that Biased Cultural Transmission is the Predominate Force in Behavioral Change and Much of Sociocultural Evolution. American Anthropologist 103:9921013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J. 2002 Decision-making, Cultural Transmission and Adaptation in Economic Anthropology. In Theory in Economic Anthropology, edited by Ensminger, J., pp. 251295, Row-man and Littlefield, Lanham, Maryland.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Albers, W., Boyd, R., McCabe, K., Gigerenzer, G., Young, H. P., and Ockenfels, A. 2001 Is Culture Important in Bounded Rationality? In Bounded Rationality: The Adaptive Toolbox, edited by Gigerenzer, G. and Selten, R., pp. 343359. Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., and Boyd, R. 1998 The Evolution of Conformist Transmission and the Emergence of Between-Group Differences. Evolution and Human Behavior 19:215242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., and Boyd, R. 2002 On Modeling Cultural Evolution: Why Replicators Are Not Necessary for Cultural Evolution. Journal of Cognition and Culture 2:87112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., and Gil-White, F. 2001 The Evolution of Prestige: Freely Conferred Deference as a Mechanism for Enhancing the Benefits of Cultural Transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior 22:165196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., and McElreath, R. 2003 The Evolution of Cultural Evolution. Evolutionary-Anthropology 12:123135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hewlett, B. S., and Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. 1986 Cultural Transmission among Aka Pygmies. American Anthropologist 88:922934.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmberg, A. R. 1950 Nomads of the Long Bow. Smithsonian Institution Institute of Social Anthropology Publication No. 10. United States Government Printing Office, Washington D.C. Google Scholar
Horton, D. R. 1979 Tasmanian Adaptation. Mankind 12:2834.Google Scholar
Jones, R. 1974 Tasmanian Tribes. In Aboriginal Tribes of Australia, edited by Tindale, N. B., pp. 319354. University of California–Los Angeles Press, San Francisco.Google Scholar
Jones, R. 1976 Tasmania: Aquatic Machines and Off-Shore Islands. In Problems in Economic and Social Archaeology, edited by Sieveking, G., Longworth, I. H., and Wilson, K. E., pp. 235263. Duckworth, London.Google Scholar
Amsden, Diana 1977a Man as an Element of a Continental Fauna: The Case of the Sundering Of The Bassian Bridge. In Sunda and Sahul: Prehistoric Studies in Southeast Asia, Melansia and Australia, edited by Allen, J., Golson, J., and Jones, R., pp. 317386. Academic Press, London.Google Scholar
Amsden, Diana 1977b The Tasmanian Paradox. In Stone Tools As Cultural Markers: Change, Evolution and Complexity, edited by Wright, V. S., pp. 189204. Humanities Press, New Jersey.Google Scholar
Amsden, Diana 1977c Why Did the Tasmanians Stop Eating Fish? In Explorations in Ethno-Archaeology, edited by Gould, R., pp. 1147. University of New Mexico Press, Alberqueque, and School of American Research. Santa Fe.Google Scholar
Jones, R. 1990 From Kakadu to Kutikina: The Southern Continent at 18,000 Years Ago. In The Word at 18,000 B.P. Vol. 2, edited by Gamble, C. and Softer, O., pp. 264295. Unwin Hyman, London.Google Scholar
Jones, R. 1995 Tasmanian Archaeology: Establishing the Sequence. Annual Review of Anthropology 24:423446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lancy, D. F. 1996 Playing on Mother Ground: Cultural Routines for Children's Development. Guilford Press, London. Population Structure, Cultural Transmission and Frequency Seriation. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 16:301333.Google Scholar
Lothrop, S. K. 1928 The Indians of Tierra del Fuego. Museum of the American Indian and Heye Foundation, New York. London. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
urandos, H. 1997 Continent of Hunter-Gatherers. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. M.A. Google Scholar
cGrew, W. C. 1987 Tools to Get Food: The Subsistants of Tasmanian Aborigines and Tanzanian Chimpanzees Compared. Journal of Anthropological Research 43:247258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neiman, F. D. 1995 Stylistic Variation in Evolutionary Perspective: Inferences from Decorative Diversity and Interassemblages Distance in Illinois Woodland Ceramic Assemblages. American Antiquity 60:736. OhCrossRefGoogle Scholar
magari, K., and Berkes, F. 1997 Transmission of Indigenous Knowledge and Bush Skills among Western James Bay Cree Women of Subarctic Canada. Human Ecology 25:197222.Google Scholar
Oswalt, W. H. 1973 Habitat and Technology: The Evolution of Hunting. Holt, Rhinehart and Winston, New York.Google Scholar
Oswalt, W. H. 1976 An Anthropological Analysis of Food-Getting Technology. John Wiley, New York.Google Scholar
Pardoe, C. 1991 Isolation and Evolution in Tasmania. Cultural Anthropology 32:121.Google Scholar
Parry, W. J. 1981 Fear of Fish and Forests: Food Taboos and Diet Optimization in Western Tasmania. Michigan Discussions in Anthropology 6(2):80101.Google Scholar
Plomin, R., Defries, J., and McLearn, G. E. 2000 Behavioral Genetics. W. H. Freeman, New York.Google Scholar
Plomley, N. J. B. 1966 Friendly Mission: The Tasmania Journals and Papers of George Augustus Robinson 1829-1834. Tasmanian Historical Research Association, Hobart, Australia.Google Scholar
Price, G. 1970 Selection and Covariance. Nature 227:520521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, G. 1972 Extensions of Covariance Selection Mathematics. Annals of Human Genetics 35:485490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ranson, D., Allen, J., and Jones, R. 1983 Australia's Prehistory Uncovered. Australian Natural History 21:8287.Google Scholar
Rivers, W. H. R. 1926 Psychology and Ethnology. Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner, London.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., and McCauley, C. R. 2000 Disgust. In Handbook of Emotions, edited by Lewis, M. and Haviland-Jones, J. M., pp. 637653. 2nd ed. Guilford Press, New York.Google Scholar
Ryan, L. 1981 The Aboriginal Tasmanians. University of Queensland Press, London.Google Scholar
Schlag, K. H. 1998 Why Imitate, and If So, How? A Boundedly Rational Approach to Multi-Armed Bandits. Journal of Economic Theory 78:130156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shennan, S. 2001 Demography and Cultural Innovation: A Model and Its Implications for the Emergence of Modern Human Culture. Cambridge Archaeology Journal 11(1):516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shennan, S. 2003 Genes, Memes, and Human History: Darwinian Archaeology and Cultural Evolution. Thames and Hudson, London.Google Scholar
ennan, S. J., and Steele, J. 1999 Cultural Learning in Hominids: A Behavioural Ecological Approach. In Mammalian Social Learning: Comparative and Ecological Approach, edited by Box, H. O. and Gibson, K. R., pp. 367388. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Sim, R. 1999 Why the Tasmanians Stopped Eating Fish: Evidence For Late Holocene Expansion in Resource Exploitation Strategies. In Australian Coastal Archaeology, edited by Hall, J. and McNiven, I., pp. 263269. The Australian National University, Canberra.Google Scholar
Skinner, H. D. 1923 The Morioris of the Chatham Islands. Memoirs of the Bernice P. Bishop Museum, Vol. 9, No. 1. Bernice P. Bishop Museum, Honolulu.Google Scholar
Sutton, D. G. 1980 A Culture History of the Chatham Islands. The Journal of the Polynesian Society 89(1):6794.Google Scholar
Thomas, N. 1981 Social Theory, Ecology and Epistemology: Theoretical Issues in Australian Prehistory. Mankind 13:165177. Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. R, Ancrenaz, M., Gwendolyn, B., Galdikas, B., Knott, C. D., Singeton, I., Suzuki, A., Utami, S. S., and Merrill, M. 2003 Orangutan Cultures and the Evolution of Material Culture. Science 299:102105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vanderwal, R. L. 1978 Adaptive Technology in Southwest Tasmania. Australia Archaeology 8:107127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walters, I. N. 1981 Why the Tasmanians Stopped Eating Fish. Artefact 6:7177.Google Scholar
Webb, C., and Allen, J. 1990 A Functional Analysis of Pleistocene Bone Tools from Two Sites in Southwest Tasmania. Archaeology of Oceania 25:7578. WCrossRefGoogle Scholar
hite, J. R, and O’Connell, J. F. 1982 A Prehistory of Australia, New Guinea and Sahul. Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
Yellen, J. E., Brooks, A. S., Cornelissen, E., Mehlman, M. J., and Stewart, K. 1995 A Middle Stone Age Worked Bone Industry from Katanda, Upper Semliki Valley, Zaire. Science 268:553556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed