Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-2xdlg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-02T16:18:14.899Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

An evaluation of tail paint as an aid or alternative to oestrus detection

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 September 2010

M. J. Ducker
Affiliation:
National Institute for Research in Dairying, Shinfield, Reading RG2 9AT
Rosemary A. Haggett
Affiliation:
National Institute for Research in Dairying, Shinfield, Reading RG2 9AT
W. J. Fisher
Affiliation:
National Institute for Research in Dairying, Shinfield, Reading RG2 9AT
Glenys A. Bloomfield
Affiliation:
National Institute for Research in Dairying, Shinfield, Reading RG2 9AT
S. V. Morant
Affiliation:
National Institute for Research in Dairying, Shinfield, Reading RG2 9AT
Get access

Abstract

One hundred Friesian heifers were tail-painted between 14 and 21 days after calving. Once a week the paint strip was renewed if any paint had been removed. Over the whole period of observation the ovulation detection rate by definite signs of oestrus was high (0·79), whilst the proportion of silent ovulations detected by tail paint removal was low (0·10). In addition, tail paint was not removed on 0·28 of the occasions when definite oestrus with ovulation occurred and on 0·26 of the occasions when all the paint was removed it was not associated with any reproductive event. In practice, the critical time for tail paint to be effective is during the service period. Again, tail paint identified fewer (P < 0·001) ovulations than definite signs of oestrus (0·66) and had a significantly higher false positive rate (P < 0·001). Month of calving did not affect these results but the accuracy of tail paint declined as the season progressed (P < 0·001). False positive indications were not associated with individual animal characteristics. In a second trial 43 cows were tail-painted and 43 were not. There was no significant difference in the mean number of days from calving to first insemination or successful pregnancy between the two groups. It is concluded that in these trials tail paint was not an effective or reliable aid or alternative to oestrus detection.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © British Society of Animal Science 1983

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Ball, P. J. H. and Pope, G. S. 1976. Measurement of concentrations of progesterone in fat-free cows' milk: its potential value in studies of reproduction. J. Endocr. 69: 408418.Google ScholarPubMed
Ducker, M. J., Haggett, Rosemary A., Bloomfield, Glenys A., Morant, S. V. and Gurr, M. I. 1982. The effect of level of feeding and β-carotene on dairy cow fertility. Arum. Prod. 34: 369 (Abstr.).Google Scholar
Esslemont, R. J. 1974. Heat detection in large dairy herds. Proc. Symp. Detection and Control of Breeding Activity in Farm Animals, School of Agric, Aberdeen, pp.5766.Google Scholar
Esslemont, R. J. 1979. Management with special reference to fertility. In Feeding Strategy for the High, Yielding Dairy Cow (ed. Broster, W. H. and Swan, H.), pp. 255292. Granada Publishing, St. Albans.Google Scholar
Foote, R. H. 1975. Estrus detection and estrus detection aids. J. Dairy Sci. 58: 248256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macmillan, K. L. and Curnow, R. J. 1977. Tail painting—a simple form of oestrus detection in New Zealand dairy herds. N. Z. J. Exp. Agric. 5: 357361.Google Scholar
Mulvany, P. 1977. Dairy cow condition scoring. Natn. Inst. Res. Dairy Pap. 4468.Google Scholar
Nelder, J. A. and Wedderburn, R. W. M. 1972. Generalized linear models. Jl R. statist. Soc. A 135: 370384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar