Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-m42fx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-19T18:16:02.290Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Efficacy of an interactive apparatus as environmental enrichment for common bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

LK Lauderdale*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Southern Mississippi, 118 College Drive, Hattiesburg, MS 39406, USA Chicago Zoological Society, Brookfield Zoo, 3300 Golf Road, Brookfield, IL 60513, USA
LJ Miller
Affiliation:
Chicago Zoological Society, Brookfield Zoo, 3300 Golf Road, Brookfield, IL 60513, USA
*
* Contact for correspondence: lisa.lauderdale@czs.org

Abstract

Environmental enrichment is a key component in improving the psychological and physiological well-being of animals under professional care. Environmental enrichment involves the addition of stimuli, including objects and cognitive challenges, into the environment in order to increase species-specific behaviour and provide opportunities for choice and control. The effectiveness of enrichment should be evaluated on a case-by-case basis to determine if the desired result has been achieved. Environmental enrichment devices (EEDs) can be utilised to present novel problems to animals under professional care. Here, a submerged interactive cognitive apparatus was presented to eight bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) five days a week for 18 weeks and behavioural indicators of animal welfare assessed. As a group, dolphins spent more time in social swims compared to solitary swims and more time at the bottom of the habitat than the middle or top throughout the day, even when the apparatus was not immediately available. Individuals’ differences were apparent in the type and amount of engagement with the apparatus. Three dolphins engaged with the apparatus by solving it or consuming the reward. Two dolphins, D4 and D8, engaged simultaneously with the apparatus and participated in more social swimming with each other. D4 solved the interactive apparatus and engaged in more social active and solitary active behaviours. D1 and D4 increased their use of the bottom of the habitat. This study is the first report of underwater enrichment increasing dolphins time at depth throughout the day even when the enrichment device is not available. The interactive apparatus was an effective form of enrichment for dolphins participating in successful trials.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2020 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Blowers, TE, Waterman, JM, Kuhar, CW and Bettinger, TL 2012 Female Nile hippopotamus (Hippopotamus amphibius) space use in a naturalistic exhibit. Zoo Biology 2: 129136. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20366CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brando, SI 2012 Animal learning and training: implications for animal welfare. The Veterinary Clinics of North America. Exotic Animal Practice 15: 387398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cvex.2012.06.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlstead, K 1998 Determining the causes of stereotypic behav-iors in zoo carnivores: toward appropriate enrichment strategies. In: Shepherdson, DJ, Mellen, J and Hutchins, M (eds) Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals pp 172183. Smithsonian Institution Press: Washington DC, USAGoogle Scholar
Chamove, AS 1989 Environmental enrichment: a review. Animal Technology 40(3): 155178Google Scholar
Charmoy, K, Sullivan, T and Miller, LJ 2015 Impact of differ-ent forms of environmental enrichment on foraging and activity levels in gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Animal Behavior and Cognition 2: 233240. https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.08.03.2015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, FE 2011 Great ape cognition and captive care: Can cognitive challenges enhance well-being? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 135: 112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2011.10.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, FE 2013 Marine mammal cognition and captive care: A proposal for cognitive enrichment in zoos and aquariums. Journal of Zoo and Aquarium Research 1: 16Google Scholar
Clark, FE, Davies, SL, Madigan, AW, Warner, AJ and Kuczaj, SA 2013 Cognitive enrichment for bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): Evaluation of a novel underwater maze device. Zoo Biology 32: 608619. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21096CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clegg, ILK, Borger-Turner, JL and Eskelinen, HC 2015 C-Well: The development of welfare assessment measures and pro-tocols for captive bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Animal Welfare 24: 267282. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.24.3.267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connor, RC, Smolker, R and Bejder, L 2006 Synchrony, social behaviour and alliance affiliation in Indian Ocean bottlenose dol-phins, Tursiops aduncus. Animal Behaviour 72: 13711378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.03.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connor, RC, Wells, RS, Mann, J and Read, AJ 2000 The bot-tlenose dolphin: social relationships in a fission-fusion society In: Mann, J, Connor, R, Tyack, PL and Whitehead, H (eds) Cetacean Societies: Field Studies of Dolphins and Whales. The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, USAGoogle Scholar
DeJong, TM 1975 A comparison of three diversity indices based on their components of richness and evenness. Oikos 26: 222227. https://doi.org/10.2307/3543712CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delfour, F and Beyer, H 2012 Assessing the effectiveness of environmental enrichment in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops trunca-tus). Zoo Biology 31: 137150. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20383Google Scholar
Dudzinski, KM 1996 Communication and behavior in the Atlantic spotted dolphins (Stenella frontalis): Relationships between vocal and behavioral activities. Texas A&M University: Texas, USAGoogle Scholar
Duffy-Echevarria, EE, Connor, RC and St Aubin, DJ 2008 Observations of strand-feeding behavior by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in Bull Creek, South Carolina. Marine Mammal Science 24: 202206. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-7692.2007.00151.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dugard, P and Todman, J 2012 Single-case and Small-n Experimental Designs: A Practical Guide to Randomization Tests. Routledge Academics: New York, NY, USA. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203180938CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eskelinen, HC, Winship, KA and Borger-Turner, JL 2015 Sex, age, and individual differences in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in response to environmental enrichment. Animal Behavior and Cognition 2: 241253. https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.08.04.2015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fay, C and Miller, LJ 2015 Utilising scents as environmental enrichment: Preference assessment and application with Rothschild giraffe. Animal Behavior and Cognition 2: 285291. https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.08.07.2015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fellner, W, Bauer, GB, Stamper, SA, Losch, BA and Dahood, A 2013 The development of synchronous movement by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Marine Mammal Science 29: 203225. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-7692.2012.00609.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, BG 1999 Environmental enrichment for laboratory rodents: Animal welfare and the methods of science. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 2: 267280. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0204_2Google ScholarPubMed
Galhardo, L, Appleby, MC, Waran, NK and dos Santos, ME 1996 Spontaneous activities of captive performing bottlenose dol-phins (Tursiops truncatus). Animal Welfare 5: 373389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garner, JP, Mason, GJ and Smith, R 2003 Stereotypic route-tracing in experimentally caged songbirds correlates with general behavioural disinhibition. Animal Behaviour 66: 711727. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2002.2254CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidet, KK, Tate, J, Divirgilio-Thomas, D, Kolanowski, A and Happ, MB 2009 Methods to improve reliability of video recorded behavioral data. Research in Nursing 32: 465474. https://doi.org/10.1002/nur.20334Google ScholarPubMed
Harley, HE, Fellner, W and Stamper, MA 2010 Cognitive research with dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) at Disney's The Seas: A program for enrichment, science, education, and conservation. International Journal of Comparative Psychology 23: 331343CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harvey, BN 2015 The nature of social relationships in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): associations and the role of affiliative, agonistic, and socio-sexual behaviors. University of Southern Mississippi, USAGoogle Scholar
Held, SD and Spinka, M 2011 Animal play and animal welfare. Animal Behaviour 81: 891899. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbe-hav.2011.01.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, HM, Dietrich, S, Yeater, D, McKinnon, M, Miller, M, Aibel, S and Dove, A 2015a Developing a catalogue of socio-sex-ual behaviors of beluga whales (Delphinapterus leucas) in the care of humans. Animal Behavior and Cognition 2: 105123. https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.05.01.2015Google Scholar
Hill, HM, Guarino, S, Crandall, S, Lenhart, E and Dietrich, S 2015b Young belugas diversify adult beluga (Delphinapterus leucas) behavior. Animal Behavior and Cognition 2: 267284. https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.08.06.2015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoy, JM, Murray, PJ and Tribe, A 2010 Thirty years later: Enrichment practices for captive mammals. Zoo Biology 29: 303316. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20254CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunter, SA, Bay, MS, Martin, ML and Hatfield, JS 2002 Behavioral effects of environmental enrichment on harbor seals (Phoca vitulina concolor) and gray seals (Halichoerus grypus). Zoo Biology 21: 375387. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.10042CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunter, SC, Gusset, M, Miller, LJ and Somers, MJ 2014 Space use as an indicator of enclosure appropriateness in African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus). Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 17(2): 98110. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2014.884401CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kratochwill, TR 2013 Single Subject Research: Strategies for Evaluating Change. Academic Press: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Kuczaj, SA and Eskelinen, HC 2014 Why do dolphins play? Animal Behavior and Cognition 1: 113127. https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.05.03.2014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuczaj, SA, Lacinak, T, Otto, F, Trone, M, Solangi, M and Ramos, J 2002 Keeping environmental enrichment enriching. International Journal of Comparative Psychology 15: 127137CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuczaj, SA, Lacinak, T and Turner, TN 1998 Environmental enrichment for marine mammals at Sea World. In: Shepherdson, DJ, Mellen, J and Hutchins, M (eds) Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals pp 314328. Smithsonian Institution Press: Washington DC, USAGoogle Scholar
Kuczaj, SA, Makecha, R, Trone, M, Paulis, RD and Ramos, JA 2006 Role of peers in cultural innovation and cultural transmission: Evidence from the play of dolphin calves. International Journal of Comparative Psychology 19: 223240CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lauderdale, LK and Miller, LJ 2019 Common bottlenose dol-phin (Tursiops truncatus) problem-solving strategies in response to a novel interactive apparatus. Behavioural Processes 169: 103990. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2019.103990CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leighty, KA, Soltis, J and Savage, A 2010 GPS assessment of the use of exhibit space and resources by African elephants (Loxodonta africana). Zoo Biology 29(2): 210220. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20250Google ScholarPubMed
Mason, GJ 2010 Species differences in responses to captivity: stress, welfare and the comparative method. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 25: 713721. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2010.08.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meehan, CL and Mench, JA 2007 The challenge of challenge: Can problem-solving opportunities enhance animal welfare? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 102: 246261. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melfi, V 2013 Is training zoo animals enriching? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 147: 299305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applan-im.2013.04.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellen, J and MacPhee, MS 2001 Philosophy of environmental enrichment: past, present, and future. Zoo Biology 20: 211226. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.1021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, LJ, Pisacane, CB and Vicino, GA 2016 Relationship between behavioural diversity and faecal glucocorticoid metabo-lites: A case study with cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus). Animal Welfare 25: 325329. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.25.3.325CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, KN, Line, SW and Markowitz, H 1998 Zoos, enrichment, and the skeptical observer: the practical value of assessment. In: Shepherdson, DJ, Mellen, J and Hutchins, M (eds) Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals pp 314328. Smithsonian Institution Press: Washington DC, USAGoogle Scholar
Nakahara, F, Komaba, M, Sato, R, Ikeda, H, Komaba, K and Kawakubo, A 2017 Spontaneous prosocial choice by captive bot-tlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus. Behavioural Processes 135: 811. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2016.11.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neto, MP, Silveira, M and dos Santos, ME 2016 Training bot-tlenose dolphins to overcome avoidance of environmental enrich-ment objects in order to stimulate play activities. Zoo Biology 35:16. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21282CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newberry, RC 1995 Environmental enrichment: Increasing the bio-logical relevance of captive environments. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 44: 229243. https://doi.org/10.1016/0168-1591(95)00616-ZCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, RI, Vannest, KJ, Davis, JL and Sauber, SB 2011 Combining non-overlap and trend for single-case research: Tau-U. Behavior Therapy 42: 284299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2010.08.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peet, RK 1974 The measurement of species diversity. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 5: 285307. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.es.05.110174.001441CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perez, BC, Mehrkam, LR, Foltz, AR and Dorey, NR 2018 Effects of enrichment presentation and other factors on behav-ioral welfare of pantropical spotted dolphin (Stenella attenuata). Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 21: 130140. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2017.1383161CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puppe, B, Ernst, K, Schon, PC and Manteuffel, G 2007 Cognitive enrichment affects behavioural reactivity in domestic pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 105: 7586. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Samuelson, MM, Lauderdale, LK, Pulis, K, Solangi, M, Hoffland, T and Lyn, H 2017 Olfactory enrichment in California sea lions (Zalophus californianus): An effective tool for captive wel-fare? Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 20: 7585. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2016.1246362CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, M, Nogge, G and Kolter, L 2014 Implementing unpredictability in feeding enrichment for Malayan sun bears (Helarctos malayanus). Zoo Biology 33: 5462. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21112CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Serres, A and Delfour, F 2017 Environmental changes and anthropogenic factors modulate social play in captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Zoo Biology 36: 99111. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21355CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shannon, CE and Weaver, W 1949 The Mathematical Theory of Communication. University of Illinois Press: Urbana, IL, USAGoogle Scholar
Shepherdson, D, Mellen, J and Hutchins, M 2005 Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals pp 172183. Smithsonian Institution Press: Washington DC, USAGoogle Scholar
Shepherdson, DJ, Carlstead, K, Mellen, JD and Seidensticker, J 1993 The influence of food presentation on the behavior of small cats in confined environments. Zoo Biology 12:203216. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.1430120206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Similä, T and Ugarte, F 1993 Surface and underwater observations of cooperatively feeding killer whales in northern Norway. Canadian Journal of Zoology 71: 14941499. https://doi.org/10.1139/z93-210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, BP and Litchfield, CA 2010 An empirical case study examining effectiveness of environmental enrichment in two captive Australian Sea Lions (Neophoca cinerea). Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 13: 103122. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888700903371863CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smolker, R, Richards, A, Connor, R, Mann, J and Berggren, P 1997 Sponge carrying by dolphins (Delphinidae, Tursiops spp): A foraging specialization involving tool use? Ethology 103: 454465. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1997.tb00160.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swaisgood, RR and Shepherdson, DJ 2005 Scientific approach-es to enrichment and stereotypies in zoo animals: What's been done and where should we go next? Zoo Biology 24: 499518. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20066CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swaisgood, RR, White, AM, Zhou, XP, Zhang, HM, Zhang, GQ, Wei, RP, Hare, VJ, Tepper, EM, and Lindburg, DG 2001 A quantitative assessment of the efficacy of an environmental enrichment program for giant pandas. Animal Behaviour 61: 447457. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1610CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vannest, KJ and Ninci, J 2015 Evaluating intervention effects in single-case research designs. Journal of Counseling and Development 93: 403411. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcad.12038CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waples, KA and Gales, NJ 2002 Evaluating and minimizing social stress in the care of captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus). Zoo Biology 21: 526. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.10004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, RS 2009 Learning from nature: Bottlenose dolphin care and husbandry. Zoo Biology 28: 635651. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20252Google ScholarPubMed
White, BC, Houser, LA, Fuller, JA, Taylor, S and Elliott, JL 2003 Activity-based exhibition of five mammalian species: Evaluation of behavioral changes. Zoo Biology 22: 269285. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.10085CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Lauderdale and Miller supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 78.9 KB