Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-5wvtr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-20T20:21:36.891Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Executive functions as predictors of syntactic awareness in English monolingual and English–Spanish bilingual language brokers and nonbrokers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 September 2015

VANESSA R. RAINEY*
Affiliation:
University of West Florida
DENISE DAVIDSON
Affiliation:
Loyola University Chicago
CHRISTINE LI-GRINING
Affiliation:
Loyola University Chicago
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Vanessa R. Rainey, Department of Psychology, University of West Florida, 11000 University Parkway, Pensacola, FL 32514. E-mail: vrainey@uwf.edu

Abstract

Executive function (EF) skills (cognitive flexibility, inhibition, and working memory) and syntactic awareness of monolingual (English) and bilingual (English–Spanish) language brokers and nonbrokers were examined. EF skills may be salient for brokers, who must not only inhibit interference from another language but also exhibit EF skills when translating. The brokers displayed better cognitive flexibility when compared to the other groups. For the bilingual groups, cognitive flexibility was also a predictor of English syntactic awareness when accuracy was taken into account. Cognitive flexibility and inhibition were important factors on a Spanish syntactic awareness measure. These results are discussed in terms of how linkages between EF skills and syntactic awareness may be moderated by language group.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Acoach, C. L., & Webb, L. M. (2004). The influence of language brokering on Hispanic teenagers’ acculturation, academic performance, and nonverbal decoding skills: A preliminary study. Howard Journal of Communications, 15, 119. doi:10.1080/10646170490275459 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Altemeier, L. E., Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. W. (2008). Executive functions for reading and writing in typical literacy development and dyslexia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30, 588606. doi:10.1080/13803390701562818 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bajo, M. T., Padilla, F., & Padilla, P. (2000). Comprehension processes in simultaneous interpreting. In Chesterman, A., Salvador, N. Gallardo-San, & Gambier, Y. (Eds.), Translation in context (pp. 127142). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barac, R., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingual effects on cognitive and linguistic development: Role of language, cultural background, and education. Child Development, 83, 413422. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01707 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baum, S., & Titone, D. (2014). Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism, executive control, and aging. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 857894. doi:10.1017/S0142716414000174 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81, 16411660. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70, 636644. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00046 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2010). Global-local and trail-making tasks by monolingual and bilingual children: Beyond inhibition. Developmental Psychology, 46, 93105. doi:10.1037/a0015466 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E. (2015). Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of attention. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 117121. doi:10.1111/cdep.12116 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., & Barac, R. (2012). Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control. Cognition, 122, 6773. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., & Craik, F. I. M. (2010). Cognitive and linguistic processing in the bilingual mind. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 1923. doi:10.1177/0963721409358571 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Feng, X. (2009). Language proficiency and executive control in proactive interference: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual children and adults. Brain and Language, 109, 93100. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2008.09.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Martin, M. M., & Viswanathan, M. (2005). Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9, 103119. doi:10.1177/13670069050090010701 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bierman, K. L., Nix, R. L., Greenberg, M. T., Blair, C., & Domitrovich, C. E. (2008). Executive functions and school readiness intervention: Impact, moderation, and mediation in the head start REDI program. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 821843. doi:10.1017/S0954579408000394 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonifacci, P., Giombini, L., Bellocchi, S., & Contento, S. (2011). Speed of processing, anticipation, inhibition and working memory in bilinguals. Developmental Science, 14, 256269. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00974.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruyer, R., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). Combining speed and accuracy in cognitive psychology: Is the inverse efficiency score (IES) a better dependent variable than the mean reaction time (RT) and the percentage of errors (PE)? Psychologica Belgica, 51, 513. doi:10.5334/pb-51-1-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buriel, R., Perez, W., de Ment, T. L., Chavez, D. V., & Moran, V. R. (1998). The relationship of language brokering to academic performance, biculturalism, and self-efficacy among Latino adolescents. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 20, 283297. doi:10.1177/07399863980203001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cain, K. (2007). Syntactic awareness and reading ability: Is there any evidence for a special relationship? Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 679694. doi:10.1017/S0142716407070361 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, R., & Sais, E. (1995). Accelerated metalinguistic (phonological) awareness in bilingual children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 6168. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1995.tb00664.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11, 282298. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cartwright, K. B., Marshall, T. R., Dandy, K. L., & Isaac, M. C. (2010). The development of graphophonological–semantic cognitive flexibility its contribution to reading comprehension in beginning readers. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11, 6185. doi:10.1080/15248370903453584 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, S. H., Zhou, Q., Uchikoshi, Y., & Bunge, S. A. (2014). Variations on the bilingual advantage? Links of Chinese and English proficiency to Chinese American children's self-regulation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 111. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01069 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christopher, M. E., Miyake, A., Keenan, J. M., Pennington, B., DeFries, J. C., Wadsworth, S. J., et al. (2012). Predicting word reading and comprehension with executive function and speed measures across development: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 470488. doi:10.1037/a0027375 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colzato, L. S., Bajo, M. T., van den Wildenberg, W., Paolieri, D., Nieuwenhuis, S., La Heij, W., et al. (2008). How does bilingualism improve executive control? A comparison of active and reactive inhibition mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 302312. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.2.302 Google ScholarPubMed
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don't. Cognition, 113, 135149. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cromdal, J. (1999). Childhood bilingualism and metalinguistic skills: Analysis and control in young Swedish–English bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 120. doi:10.1017/S0142716499001010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummings, K. D., Baker, D. L., & Good, R. H. (2006). Guía para la administración y calificación de IDEL. In Baker, D. L., Good, R. H., Knutson, N., & Watson, J. M. (Eds.), Indicadores Dinámicos del Éxito en la Lectura (7th ed.). Eugene, OR: Dynamic Measurement Group.Google Scholar
Davidson, D., Raschke, V. R., & Pervez, J. (2010). Syntactic awareness in young monolingual and bilingual (Urdu–English) children. Cognitive Development, 25, 166182. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.07.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deák, G. O. (2003). The development of cognitive flexibility and language abilities. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 31, 271327. doi:10.1016/S0065-2407(03)31007-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Andrade Varanda, C. (2011). Syntactic awareness: Probable correlations with central coherence and non-verbal intelligence in autism. Jornal da Sociedade Briasileira de Fonoaudiologia, 23, 142151.Google Scholar
Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). The Delis–Kaplan executive function system. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Demakis, G. J. (2004). Frontal lobe damage and tests of executive processing: A meta-analysis of the category test, Stroop test, and trail-making test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 441450. doi:10.1080/13803390490510149 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135168. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixon, W. E., Salley, B. J., & Clements, A. D. (2006). Temperament, distraction, and learning in toddlerhood. Infant Behavior & Development, 29, 342357. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.01.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dorner, L. M., Orellana, M. F., & Li-Grining, C. P. (2007). “I helped my mom,” and it helped me: Translating the skills of language brokers into improved standardized test scores. American Journal of Education, 113, 451478. doi:10.1086/512740 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (4th ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Lugo, D. E., Padilla, E. R., & Dunn, L. M. (1986). Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143149. doi:10.3758/BF03203267 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foursha-Stevenson, C., & Nicoladis, E. (2011). Early emergence of syntactic awareness and cross-linguistic influence in bilingual children's judgments. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15, 521534. doi:10.1177/1367006911425818 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. A. (2013a). Bilingual children show advantages in nonverbal auditory executive function task. International Journal of Bilingualism. Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/1367006912472263 Google Scholar
Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. A. (2013b). Executive function and early reading skills. Reading and Writing, 26, 453472. doi:10.1007/s11145-012-9376-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galambos, S. J., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1990). The effects of learning two languages on levels of metalinguistic awareness. Cognition, 34, 156. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(90)90030-N CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
García, A. M., Ibáñez, A., Huepe, D., Houck, A. L., Michon, M., Lezama, C. G., et al. (2014). Word reading and translation in bilinguals: The impact of formal and informal translation expertise. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 114. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01302 Google ScholarPubMed
Gaux, C., & Gombert, J. E. (1999). Implicit and explicit syntactic knowledge and reading in pre-adolescents. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 169188. doi:10.1348/026151099165212 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gombert, J. É., & Colé, P. (2000). Activités métalinguistiques, lecture et illettrisme [Metalinguistic activities, reading and illiteracy.] In Kail, M. & Fayol, M. (Eds.), L'acquisition du langage [Language acquisition] (Vol. 2, pp. 117150). Paris: P.U.F. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Good, R. H., & Kaminski, R. A. (2002). Dynamic indicators of basic early literacy skills (6th ed.). Eugene, OR: Institute for the Development of Education Achievement.Google Scholar
Gouveia, P. A. R., Brucki, S. M. D., Malheiros, S. M. F., & Bueno, O. F. A. (2007). Disorders in planning and strategy application in frontal lobe lesion patients. Brain and Cognition, 63, 240246. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2006.09.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grady, C. L., Luk, G., Craik, F., & Bialystok, E. (2015). Brain network activity in monolingual and bilingual older adults. Neuropsychologia, 66, 170181. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.042 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, D. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 6781. doi:10.1017/S1366728998000133 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guo, T. M., Liu, H. Y., Misra, M., & Kroll, J. F. (2011). Local and global inhibition in bilingual word production: fMRI evidence from Chinese–English bilinguals. NeuroImage, 56, 23002309. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.049 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gutiérrez, X. (2013). The construct validity of grammaticality judgment tests as measures of implicit and explicit knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35, 423449. doi:10.1017/S0272263113000041 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halgunseth, L. (2003). Language brokering: Positive developmental outcomes. In Coleman, M. & Ganong, L. (Eds.), Points and counterpoints: Controversial relationship and family issues in the 21st century: An anthology (pp. 154157). Los Angeles: Roxbury.Google Scholar
Hernandez, A. E., Dapretto, M., Mazziotta, J., & Bookheimer, S. (2001). Language switching and language representation in Spanish–English bilinguals: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 14, 510520. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0810 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18, 625658. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0116-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmbeck, G. N. (1997). Toward terminological, conceptual, and statistical clarity in the study of mediators and moderators: Examples from the child-clinical and pediatric psychology literatures. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 599610. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.65.4.599 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ibáñez, A. J., Macizo, P., & Bajo, M. T. (2010). Language access and language selection in professional translators. Acta Psychologica, 135, 257266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jia, G., Aaronson, D., & Wu, Y. H. (2002). Long-term language attainment of bilingual immigrants: Predictive factors and language group differences. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 599621. doi:10.1017.S0142716402004058 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jia, G., & Fuse, A. (2007). Acquisition of English grammatical morphology by native Mandarin-speaking children and adolescents: Age-related differences. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 12801299. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2007/090)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katz, L. A. (2004). An investigation of the relationship of morphological awareness to reading comprehension in fourth and sixth grades. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Kovelman, I., Baker, S. A., & Petitto, L. (2008). Bilingual and monolingual brains compared: A functional magnetic imaging investigation of syntactic processing and a possible “neural signature” of bilingualism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 153169. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kraus, N., & Chandrasekaran, B. (2010). Music training for the development of auditory skills. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 599605. doi:10.1038/nrn2882 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., & Hoshino, N. (2014). Two languages in mind: Bilingualism as a tool to investigate language, cognition, and the brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 159163. doi:10.1177/0963721414528511 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loewen, S. (2009). Grammaticality judgment tests and the measurement of implicit and explicit L2 knowledge. In Ellis, R., Loewen, S., Elder, C., Erlam, R., Philp, J., & Reinders, H. (Eds.), Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language learning, testing and teaching (pp. 94112). Tonawanda, NY: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malakoff, M., & Hakuta, K. (1991). Translation skill and metalinguistic awareness in bilinguals. InBialystok, E. (Ed.), Language processing in bilingual children (pp. 141166). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 8193. doi:10.1017/S1366728907003227 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, J. L. (2006). Beyond the critical period: Processing-based explanations for poor grammaticality judgment performance by late second language learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 381401. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2006.06.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McQuillan, J., & Tse, L. (1995). Child language brokering in linguistic minority communities: Effects on cultural interaction, cognition, and literacy. Language and Education, 9, 195215. doi:10.1080/09500789509541413 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mezzacappa, E. (2004). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75, 13731386. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00746.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mokhtari, K., & Thompson, B. (2006). How problems of reading fluency and comprehension are related to difficulties in syntactic awareness skills among fifth graders. Reading Research and Instruction, 46, 7394. doi:10.1080/19388070609558461 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morales, A., & Hanson, W. E. (2005). Language brokering: An integrative review of the literature. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 27, 471503. doi:10.1177/0739986305281333 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N. (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10, 719726. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00623.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nation, K., & Snowling, M. (2000). Factors influencing syntactic awareness skills in normal readers and poor comprehenders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 229241. doi:10.1017/S0142716400002046 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orellana, M. F., Dorner, L., & Pulido, L. (2003). Accessing assets: Immigrant youth's work as family translators or “para-phrasers.” Social Problems, 50, 505524. doi:10.1525/sp.2003.50.4.505 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Portes, A., & Rumbaut, R. G. (2005). Introduction: The second generation and the children of immigrants longitudinal study. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 28, 983999. doi:10.1080/01419870500224109 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Price, C. J., Green, D. W., & von Studnitz, R. (1999). A functional imaging study of translation and language switching. Brain, 122, 22212235. doi:10.1093/brain/122.12.2221 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H. (2011). Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 682691. doi:10.1017/S1355617711000580 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Simard, D., Fortier, V., & Foucambert, D. (2013). Measuring the metasyntactic ability of Portuguese-speaking children living in a French-speaking environment. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 1931. doi:10.1017/S1366728911000071 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soveri, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Laine, M. (2011). Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within-group analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology: Cognition, 2, 18. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00183 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clair-Thompson, H. L. St., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 745759. doi:10.1080/17470210500162854 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Tao, L., Marzecová, A., Taft, M., Asanowicz, D., & Wodniecka, Z. (2011). The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: The role of age of acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 119. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00123 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1978). Methods of modeling capacity in simple processing systems. In Castellan, N. J. Jr. & Restle, F. (Eds.), Cognitive theory (Vol. 3, pp. 199239). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tse, L. (1996). Who decides? The effects of language brokering on home-school communication. Journal of Educational Issues of Language Minority Students, 16, 225234.Google Scholar
Tukey, J. W. (1977): Exploratory data analysis. Reading, MA: Addison–Wesley.Google Scholar
Valdes, G., Chavez, C., & Angelelli, C. (2003). A performance team: Young interpreters and their parents. In Valdes, G. (Ed.), Expanding definitions of giftedness: The case of young interpreters from immigrant countries (pp. 6397). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Valenzuela, A. (1999). Gender roles and settlement activities among children and their immigrant families. American Behavioral Scientist, 42, 720742. doi:10.1177/0002764299042004009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Villanueva, C. M., & Buriel, R. (2010). Speaking on behalf of others: A qualitative study of the perceptions and feelings of adolescent Latina language brokers. Journal of Social Issues, 66, 197210. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2009.01640.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2003). The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (4th ed.). London: Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Yang, S., Yang, H., & Lust, B. (2011). Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Dissociating culture and language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 412422. doi:10.1017/S13667289100006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yudes, C., Macizo, P., Morales, L., & Bajo, M. T. (2012). Comprehension and error monitoring in simultaneous interpreters. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 119. doi:10.1017/S0142716412000112 Google Scholar