Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-n9wrp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T14:32:38.897Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sentence interpretation in bilingual speakers of English and Dutch1

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 December 2010

Janet L. Mcdonald*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University
*
Janet L. McDonald, Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, U.S.A.

Abstract

Speakers of English and Dutch vary in how strongly they use various syntactic (e.g., word order, prepositions, case inflection) and semantic (e.g., noun animacy) cues to interpret native language sentences. For example, in simple NVN sentences, English speakers rely heavily on word order, while Dutch speakers rely on case inflection. This paper compares the cue usage of English/Dutch and Dutch/English bilinguals with varying amounts of second language exposure to that of native speaker control groups. For all constructions tested, dative constructions, simple NVN sentences, and relative clauses, it was found that with increasing exposure, cue usage in the second language gradually shifts from that appropriate to the first language to that appropriate to the second. A model of cue learning originally proposed to account for monolingual data is found to be compatible with the learning pattern exhibited by bilinguals.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1987

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

1

This paper is based on doctoral dissertation submitted to the Carnegie-Mellon University Psychology Department. Thanks go to Pat Carpenter, the late Bill Chase, Marcel Just, Brian Mac Whinney, and to the Vakgroep Psychologische Funktieleer at the Universiteit te Leiden, the Netherlands. This work was partially supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant BNS-8211705.

References

REFERENCES

Ammon, M., & Slobin, D. I. (1979). A cross-linguistic study of the processing of causative sentences. Cognition, 7, 317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1981). Second language acquisition from a functionalist perspective: Pragmatic, semantic and perceptual strategies. In Winitz, H. (Ed.), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences conference on native and foreign language acquisition. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1982). Functionalist approaches to grammar. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1987). Competition, variation and language learning. In MacWhinney, B. (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bates, E., MacWhinney, B., Caselli, C., Devescovi, A., Natale, F., & Venza, V. (1984). A cross-linguistic study of the development of sentence interpretation strategies. Child Development, 55, 341354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E., McNew, S., MacWhinney, B., Devescovi, A., & Smith, S. (1982). Functional constraints on sentence processing: A cross-linguistic study. Cognition, 11, 245299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., & Frohlich, M. (1978). Variables of classroom achievement in second language learning. Modern Language Journal, 62, 327336.Google Scholar
Brunswik, E. (1956). Perception and the representative design of psychology experiments. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1965). Some structural properties of simple active and passive sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4, 365370.Google Scholar
Cooper, R. L., Olshtain, E., Tucker, G. R., & Waterbury, M. (1979). The acquisition of complex English structures by adult native speakers of Arabic Hebrew. Language Learning, 29, 255275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
d'Angeljan, A., & Tucker, G. R. (1975). The acquisition of complex English structures by adult learners. Language Learning, 25, 281293.Google Scholar
Dik, S. C. (1978). Functional grammar. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Dik, S. C. (1980). Studies in functional grammar. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Frankel, D. G., Amir, M., Frenkel, E., & Arbel, T. (1980). A developmental study of the role of word order in comprehending Hebrew. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 29, 2335Google Scholar
Frankel, D. G., & Arbel, T. (1981). Developmental changes in assigning agent relations in Hebrew: The interaction between word order and structural cues. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 32, 102114.Google Scholar
Gardner, R. C., & Lambert, W. E. (1972). Attitudes and motivation in second language learning. Rowley, MA: Newbury.Google Scholar
Hakuta, K. (1982). Interaction between particles and word order in the comprehension and production of simple sentences in Japanese children. Developmental Psychology, 18, 6276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. G. (1967). Syntactic position and rated meaning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 240246.Google Scholar
Kail, M. (in press). Cue validity, cue cost, and processing types in French sentence comprehension. In MacWhinney, B. & Bates, E. (Eds.), Cross-linguistic studies of sentence processing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Koster, J. (1978). Locality principles in syntax. Dordrecht: Foris Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krashen, S. D. (1973). Lateralization, language learning and the critical period: Some new evidence. Language Learning, 23, 6374.Google Scholar
Lambert, W. E., Gardner, R. C., Olton, R., & Tunstall, K. (1968). A study of the roles of attitudes and motivation in second language learning. In Fishman, J. A. (Ed.), Readings in the sociology of language. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1978). The acquisition of morphophonology. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 43(1).Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B., Bates, E., & Kliegl, R. (1984). Cue validity and sentence interpretation in English, German and Italian. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 127150.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B., Pleh, C., & Bates, E. (1985). The development of sentence interpretation in Hungarian. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 178209.Google Scholar
McDonald, J. L. (1984). The mapping of semantic and syntactic processing cues by first and second language learners of English, Dutch and German. Doctoral dissertation, Carnegie-Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, Unpublished.Google Scholar
McDonald, J. L. (1986). The development of sentence comprehension strategies in English and Dutch. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 317335.Google Scholar
McDonald, J. L. (1987). Sentence interpretation processes: The influence of conflicting cues. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 100117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osgood, C. E., & Zehler, A. M. (1981). Acquisition of bi-transitive sentences: Pre-linguistic determinants of language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 8, 367383.Google Scholar
Schumann, J. H. (1978). Second language acquisition: The pidginization hypothesis. In Hatch, E. M. (Ed.), Second language acquisition. Rowley, MA: Newbury.Google Scholar
Seliger, H. W. (1977). Does practice make perfect?: A study of interaction patterns and L2 competence. Language Learning, 27, 263278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1982). Universal and particular in the acquisition of language. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I., & Bever, T. G. (1982). Children use canonical sentence schemas: A cross-linguistic study of word order and inflections. Cognition, 12, 229265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E., & Hoefnagel-Höhle, M. (1978). The critical period for language acquisition: Evidence from second language learning. Child Development, 49, 11141128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokolov, J. L. (in press, a). Cue validity in Hebrew sentence comprehension. Journal of Child Language.Google Scholar
Sokolov, J. (in press, b). The development of role assignment in Hebrew. In MacWhinney, B. & Bates, E., (Eds.), Cross-linguistic studies in sentence processing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Waryas, C. L., & Stremel, K. (1974). On the preferred form of the double object construction. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 3, 271280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar