Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-pfhbr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-11T23:25:36.838Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive-linguistic skills and vocabulary knowledge breadth and depth in children’s L1 Chinese and L2 English

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 January 2023

Dora Jue Pan
Affiliation:
School of Humanities and Social Science, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shenzhen, China
Mariko Nakayama
Affiliation:
Language Sciences, Graduate School of International Cultural Studies, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
Catherine McBride*
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Family Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, USA
Zebedee Rui En Cheah
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
Mo Zheng
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
Charlie Cheuk Laam Yeung
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
*
*Corresponding author. Email: mcbridca@purdue.edu

Abstract

This study examined the correlates of different aspects of vocabulary knowledge in L1 Chinese and L2 English in Hong Kong bilingual children (N = 481, age = 6–12 years old). Their nonverbal IQ, cognitive-linguistic skills, receptive and expressive vocabulary knowledge breadth, and vocabulary knowledge depth in Chinese and English were measured. Results demonstrated that morphological awareness was uniquely correlated with different aspects of vocabulary knowledge across Chinese and English. Phonological processing skills played different roles in vocabulary knowledge in L1 and L2. In addition, receptive vocabulary breadth uniquely contributed to expressive vocabulary breadth across languages. Moreover, both receptive and expressive vocabulary breadth contributed to vocabulary knowledge depth in L1 Chinese and L2 English. The findings highlight some shared and unique aspects of different vocabulary constructs across languages.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, R. C., & Freebody, P. (1981). Vocabulary knowledge. In Guthrie, J. T. (Ed.), Comprehension and teaching: Research reviews (pp. 77117). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
Benelli, B., Belacchi, C., Gini, G., & Lucangeli, D. (2006). “To define means to say what you know about things”: The development of definitional skills as metalinguistic acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 33, 7197. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000905007312 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berko, J. (1958). The child’s learning of English morphology. WORD, 14, 150177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biemiller, A. (2006). Vocabulary development and instruction: A prerequisite for school learning. In Neuman, S. & Dickinson, D. (Eds.), The handbook of early literacy research (Vol. 2) (pp. 41–51). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In Feldman, L. B. (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 289–209). Hilddsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.Google Scholar
Chen, X., Hao, M., Geva, E., Zhu, J., & Shu, H. (2009). The role of compound awareness in Chinese children’s vocabulary acquisition and character reading. Reading and Writing, 22, 615631. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9127-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, W., Tong, X., & Deacon, H. (2019). From Cantonese lexical tone awareness to second language English vocabulary: Cross-Language mediation by segmental phonological awareness. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62, 18751889. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-17-0323 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chung, K. K. H., McBride-Chang, C., Wong, S. W. L., Cheung, H., Penney, T. B., & Ho, S. H. C. (2008). The role of visual and auditory temporal processing for Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 58, 1535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid “automatized” naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learing disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471479. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(76)90075-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody picture vocabulary test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Education Bureau of the Hong Kong Special Administrative Region. (2004). The English language curriculum guide (primary 1–6). Hong Kong: HKSAR Government.Google Scholar
Gottardo, A., Yan, B., Siegel, L. S., & Wade-Woolley, L. (2001). Factors related to English reading performance in children with Chinese as a first language: More evidence of cross-language transfer of phonological processing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 530542. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.93.3.530 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ho, C. S. H., Zheng, M., McBride, C., Hsu, L. S. J., Waye, M. M. Y., & Kwok, J. C. Y. (2017). Examining an extended simple view of reading in Chinese: The role of naming efficiency for reading comprehension. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 51, 293302. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.08.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulme, C., Zhou, L., Tong, X., Lervåg, A., & Burgoyne, K. (2019). Learning to read in Chinese: Evidence for reciprocal relationships between word reading and oral language skills. Developmental Science, 22, 111. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12745 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kieffer, M. J., & Lesaux, N. K. (2008). The role of derivational morphology in the reading comprehension of Spanish-speaking English language learners. Reading and Writing, 21, 783804. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9092-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ku, Y. M., & Anderson, R. C. (2003). Development of morphological awareness in Chinese and English. Reading and Writing, 16, 399422. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024227231216 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuo, L.-J., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161180. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lahey, M. (1988). Language disorders and language development. Needham, MA: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Li, M., & Kirby, J. R. (2015). The effects of vocabulary breadth and depth on English reading. Applied Linguistics, 36, 611634. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amu007 Google Scholar
Liu, P. D., & McBride-Chang, C. (2010). What is morphological awareness? Tapping lexical compounding awareness in Chinese third graders. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 6273. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016933 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Y., Yeung, S. S. S., Lin, D., & Wong, R. K. S. (2017). English expressive vocabulary growth and its unique role in predicting English word reading: A longitudinal study involving Hong Kong Chinese ESL children. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 49, 195202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.02.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Cheung, H., Chow, B. W. Y., Chow, C. S. L., & Choi, L. (2006). Metalinguistic skills and vocabulary knowledge in Chinese (L1) and English (L2). Reading and Writing, 19, 695716. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-5742-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Cho, J.-R., Liu, H., Wagner, R. K., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Cheuk, C. S. -M., & Muse, A. (2005a). Changing models across cultures: Associations of phonological awareness and morphological structure awareness with vocabulary and word recognition in second graders from Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and the United States. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 140160. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JECP.2005.03.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McBride-Chang, C., & Ho, C. S.-H. (2005). Predictors of beginning reading in Chinese and English: A 2-year longitudinal study of Chinese kindergartners. Scientific studies of Reading, 9, 117144. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0902_2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Wat, C. P., & Wagner, R. K. (2003). Morphological awareness uniquely predicts young children’s Chinese character recognition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 743751. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.743 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Tardif, T., Cho, J. R., Shu, H., Fletcher, P., Stokes, S. F., Wong, A., & Leung, K. (2008). What’s in a word? Morphological awareness and vocabulary knowledge in three languages. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 437462. https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271640808020X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Wagner, R. K., Muse, A., Chow, B. W. Y., & Shu, H. (2005b). The role of morphological awareness in children’s vocabulary acquisition in English. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 415435. https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271640505023X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metsala, J. L. (1999). Young children’s phonological awareness and non-word repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In Metsala, J. L. & Ehri, L. C. (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89120). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nagy, W. E., & Anderson, R. C. (1984). How many words are there in printed school English? Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304330. https://doi.org/10.2307/747823 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nation, I. S. P. (2001). Learning vocabulary in another language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ouellette, G. P. (2006). What’s meaning got to do with it: The role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 554566. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.3.554 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pan, J., McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Liu, H., Zhang, Y., & Li, H. (2011). What is in the naming? A 5-year longitudinal study of early rapid naming and phonological sensitivity in relation to subsequent reading skills in both native Chinese and English as a second language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 897908. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024344 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, P. D., Hirbert, E. H., & Kamil, M. L. (2007). Vocabulary assessment: What we know and what we need to learn. Reading Research Quarterly, 42, 282296. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.42.2.4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qian, D. (1999). Assessing the roles of depth and breadth of vocabulary knowledge in reading comprehension. Canadian Modern Language Review, 56, 282308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qian, D. (2002). Investigating the relationship between vocabulary knowledge and academic reading performance: An assessment perspective. Language Learning, 52, 513536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Ramirez, G., Chen, X., & Pasquarella, A. (2013). Cross-linguistic transfer of morphological awareness in Spanish-speaking English language learners: The facilitating effect of cognate knowledge. Topics in Language Disorders, 33, 7392. https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0b013e318280f55a CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1976). Standard progressive matrices: Sets A, B, C, D, and E. Oxford, UK: Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Read, J. (2013). Second language vocabulary assessment. Language Teaching, 46, 4152. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0261444812000377 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarborough, H. S. (2005). Developmental relations between language and reading: Reconciling a beautiful hypothesis with some ugly facts. In Catts, H. W. & Kamhi, A. G. (Eds.), The connections between language and reading disabilities (pp. 324). London, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Schmitt, N. (2014). Size and depth of vocabulary knowledge: What the research shows. Language Learning, 64, 913951. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12077 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitt, N. (2019). Understanding vocabulary acquisition, instruction, and assessment: A research agenda. Language Teaching, 52, 261274. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0261444819000053 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shu, H., Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Wu, N., & Xuan, Y. (2003). Properties of school Chinese: Implications for learning to read. Child Development, 74, 2747. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00519 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Song, S., Su, M., Kang, C., Liu, H., Zhang, Y., McBride-Chang, C., Tardif, T., Li, H., Zhang, Z., & Shu, H. (2015). Tracing children’s vocabulary development from preschool through the school-age years: An 8-year longitudinal study. Developmental Science, 18, 119131. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12190 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sparks, E., & Deacon, S. H. (2015). Morphological awareness and vocabulary acquisition: A longitudinal examination of their relationship in English-speaking children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36, 299321. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716413000246 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 934947. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.934 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tannenbaum, K., Torgesen, J. K., & Wagner, R. K. (2006). Relationships between word knowledge and reading comprehension in third-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 381398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorndike, R., Hagen, E., & Sattler, J. (1986). Stanford- Binet intelligence scale (4th ed.). Chicago, IL: Riverside.Google Scholar
Tong, X., McBride, C., Ho, C. S. H., Waye, M. M. Y., Chung, K. K. H., Wong, S. W. L., & Chow, B. W. Y. (2018). Within- and cross-language contributions of morphological awareness to word reading and vocabulary in Chinese–English bilingual learners. Reading and Writing, 31, 17651786. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9771-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tong, X., Tong, X., & McBride, C. (2017). Unpacking the relation between morphological awareness and Chinese word reading: Levels of morphological awareness and vocabulary. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 48, 167178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.07.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Goch, M., Verhoeven, L., & McQueen, J. (2019). Success in learning similar-sounding words predicts vocabulary depth above and beyond vocabulary breadth. Journal of Child Language, 46, 184197. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000918000338 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vermeer, A. (2001). Breadth and depth of vocabulary in relation to L1/L2 acquisition and frequency of input. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 217234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong, S. W., Ho, C. S. H., McBride, C., Chow, B. W. Y., & Waye, M. M. Y. (2017). Less is more in Hong Kong: Investigation of Biscriptal and trilingual development among Chinese twins in a (relatively) small city. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 20, 6671. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2016.90 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong, S. W. L., Cheung, H., Zheng, M., Yang, X., McBride, C., Ho, C. S., Leung, S. M. J., Chow, W. Y. B., & Waye, M. M. Y. (2020). Effect of Twinning on Chinese and English vocabulary knowledge. Child Development, 91, 18861897. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13400 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xie, R., Wu, X., Nguyen, T. P., Zhao, Y., & Feng, J. (2020). Bidirectional longitudinal relationship between Chinese children’s compounding awareness and vocabulary knowledge from Grades 3 to 6. Journal of Research in Reading, 43, 482495. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12324 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeung, P., Ho, C. S., Chik, P. P., Lo, L., Luan, H., Chan, D. W., & Chung, K. K. (2011). Reading and spelling Chinese among beginning readers: What skills make a difference? Scientific Studies of Reading, 15, 285313. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2010.482149 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeung, S. S., & Chan, C. K. K. (2013). Phonological awareness and oral language proficiency in learning to read English among Chinese kindergarten children in Hong Kong. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 550568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhuang, Z. Y. (2000). Xiang Gang Xiao Xue Ke Ben Yong Zi Gui Fan [The standardized use of word for Hong Kong primary school]. Hong Kong, China: Joint Publishing.Google Scholar