Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-xq9c7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-15T18:53:40.815Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Contextual influences on children's spoken and written explanations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Morag L. Donaldson*
Affiliation:
University of Edinburgh
*
Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, 7 George Square, Edinburgh, UK EH8 9JZ

Abstract

The relative influences of medium and context variables were investigated by comparing 7- and 8-year-old children's spoken and written explanations in varying contexts: a story task, a question task, a sentence completion task, and a whole sentence production task. In the story task, performance in a “purpose” condition (which provided a specific communicative purpose for the production of an explanation) was compared with performance in a “neutral” condition. The frequency of explanations containing correct causal expressions was significantly higher in the purpose condition than in the neutral condition and significantly lower in the story task than in the three more structured tasks. In contrast to these contextual influences, performance in the written medium was very similar to performance in the spoken medium.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Astington, J. W., & Gopnik, A. (1991). Theoretical explanations of children's understanding of the mind. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bamberg, M., & Damrad-Frye, R. (1991). On the ability to provide evaluative comments: further explorations of children's narrative competencies. Journal of Child Language, 18, 689710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written composition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1986). Actual minds: Possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, C. A., Hunt, A. K., & Linton, M. J. (1988). Medium effects on children's story rewriting and story retelling. First Language, 8, 318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W. L. (1985). Linguistic differences produced by differences between speaking and writing. In Olson, D. R., Torrance, N., & Hildyard, A. (Eds.), Literacy, language and learning (pp. 105123). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. (1978). Children's minds. London: Fontana.Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. (1992). Human minds. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. L. (1986). Children's explanations: A psycholinguistic study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donaldson, M. L., & Elliot, A. (1990). Children's explanations. In Grieve, R. & , M.Hughes (Eds.), Understanding children (pp. 2650). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. L., & Laing, K. (1993). Children's comprehension and production of locative expressions. In Messer, D. J. & Turner, G. J. (Eds.), Critical influences on child language acquisition and development (pp. 159178). London: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
French, L. A., & Nelson, K. (1985). Young children's knowledge of relational terms. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geva, E., & Olson, D. (1983). Children's story-retelling. First Language, 4, 85110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hicks, D. (1990). Narrative skills and genre knowledge: Ways of telling in the primary school grades. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 83104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hood, L., & Bloom, L. (1979). What, when and how about why: A longitudinal study of early expressions of causality. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 44 (n6, Serial No. 181).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, B. M. (1981). Developmental relationships between speaking and writing. In Kroll, B. M. & Vann, R. I. (Eds.), Exploring speaking-writing relationships (pp. 3254). Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.Google Scholar
Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis: oral versions of personal experience. In Helm, J. (Ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual arts (pp. 1244). Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
McCabe, A., & Peterson, C. (1985). A naturalistic study of the production of causal connectives by children. Journal of Child Language, 12, 145159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menig-Peterson, C. L. (1975). The modification of communicative behaviour in preschool-aged children as a function of the listener's perspective. Child Development, 46, 10151018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, D. R. (1977). From utterance to text: the bias of language in speech and writing. Harvard Educational Review, 47, 257281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perera, K. (1984). Children's writing and reading: Analysing classroom language. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Perner, J., & Leekam, S. R. (1986). Belief and quantity: Three-year olds' adaptation to listener's knowledge. Journal of Child Language, 13, 305316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raban, B. (1988). Speaking and writing: Young children's use of connectives. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 4, 1325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1991). Diverse conversational contexts for the acquisition of various language skills. In Miller, J. (Ed.), Research on child language disorders: A decade of progress (pp. 105124). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Stenning, K., & Michell, L. (1985). Learning how to tell a good story: The development of content and language in children's telling of one tale. Discourse Processes, 8, 261279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tannen, D. (1985). Relative focus on involvement in oral and written discourse. In Olson, D. R., Torrance, N., & Hildyard, A. (Eds.), Literacy, language and learning (pp. 124147). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child's theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yde, P., & Spoelders, M. (1985). Text cohesion: An exploratory study with beginning writers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 6, 407416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yussen, S. R. (1982). Children's impressions of coherence in narratives. In Huston, B. A. (Ed.), Advances in reading/language research, (Vol. 1, pp. 245281). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar