Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-ckgrl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-13T11:44:22.806Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Hearing-impaired students learning new words from written context

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Peter A. De Villiers*
Affiliation:
Smith College
Sarah B. Pomerantz
Affiliation:
University of Texas
*
Peter A. de Villiers, Department of Psychology, Smith College, Northampton, MA 01063

Abstract

Normally hearing students acquire most of their reading vocabulary from printed context, but little is known about this process in hearing-impaired students. Two studies, therefore, investigated hearing-impaired students' ability to derive lexical and syntactic information about unknown words embedded in short passages of text. The passages varied in their informativeness about the meaning of the unknown words. Ability to derive at least a partial meaning for a word in context was determined both by the type of context and the reading comprehension levels of the students. However, there was no relationship between reading comprehension scores and ability to determine the form class of the words in context. The results are related to the importance of integrating semantic information into a meaning schema for the passage in order to acquire new meanings for unknown words and to the local strategies adopted by poorer readers when attempting to answer comprehension questions. Implications for explaining, and trying to ameliorate, the well-documented vocabulary limitations of hearing-impaired students are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1992

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Allen, T. E. (1986). Patterns of academic achievement among hearing-impaired students. In Schildroth, A. N. & Karchmer, M. A. (Eds.), Deaf children in America. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.Google Scholar
Bromberg, M., Liebb, J., & Traiger, A. (1984). 504 absolutely essential words. Woodbury, NY:Barrons.Google Scholar
Carey, S., & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new word. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 15, 1729.Google Scholar
Carnine, D., Kameenui, E. J., & Coyle, G. (1984). Utilization of contextual information in determining the meaning of unfamiliar words. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 188203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dave, B., & King, S. (1990). Acquisition of word meanings from context by deaf readers. American Annals of the Deaf, 135, 227234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickinson, D. K. (1984). First impressions: Children's knowledge of words gained from a single exposure. Applied Psycholinguistics, 5, 369373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drum, P. A., & Konopak, B. C. (1987). Learning word meanings from written context. In McKeown, M. & Curtis, M. (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Elshout-Mohr, M., & van, Daalen-Kapteijns M. M. (1987). Cognitive processes in learning word meanings. In McKeown, M. & Curtis, M. (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Engen, E., & Engen, T. (1983). The Rhode Island test of language structure. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.Google Scholar
Fischler, I. (1983). Contextual constraint and comprehension of written sentences by deaf college students. American Annals of the Deaf, 128, 418424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geers, A. E., & Moog, J. S. (1987). Factors predictive of the development of reading and writing skills in the congenitally deaf: Report on the oral sample (NIH-NINCDS-83- 19). Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Health.Google Scholar
Giorcelli, L. (1982). The comprehension of some aspects of figurative language by deaf and hearing subjects. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign.Google Scholar
Gipe, J. (1979). Investigating techniques for teaching word meanings. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 624644.Google Scholar
Heibeck, T. H., & Markman, E. M. (1987). Word mapping in children: An examination of fast mapping. Child Development, 58, 10211034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkins, R. E., Stein, M. L., & Wysocki, K. (1984). Learning vocabulary through reading. American Educational Research Journal, 21, 767787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, C. M., & Quigley, S. P. (1985). Reading and deafness. San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.Google Scholar
LaSasso, C., & Davey, B. (1987). The relationship between lexical knowledge and reading comprehension for prelingually, profoundly hearing-impaired students. Volta Review, 89, 211220.Google Scholar
Marzano, R. J., & Marzano, J. S. (1988). A cluster approach to elementary vocabulary instruction. New York: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
McGill-Franzen, A., & Gormley, K. (1980). The influence of context on deaf readers' understanding of passive sentences. American Annals of the Deaf, 125, 937942.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKeown, M. (1985). The acquisition of word meaning from context by children of high and low ability. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 482496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G., & Gildea, P. M. (1987). How children learn words. Scientific American, 257, 9499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moores, D. (1987). Factors predictive of literacy in deaf adolescents (NIH-NINCDS-83–19). Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Health.Google Scholar
Nagy, W. E., Anderson, R. C., & Herman, P. A. (1987). Learning words from context during normal reading. American Educational Research Journal, 24, 237270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagy, W. E., & Herman, P. A. (1987). Breadth and depth of vocabulary knowledge: Implications for acquisition and instruction. In McKeown, M. & Curtis, M. (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nolen, S., & Wilbur, R. (1985). The effects of context on deaf students' comprehension of difficult sentences. American Annals of the Deaf, 130, 231235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Odom, P. B., Blanton, R. L., & Nunnaly, J. C. (1967). Some doze technique studies of language capability in the deaf. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 10, 816827.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paul, P. (1984). The comprehension of multi-meaning words from selected frequency levels by deaf and hearing subjects. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign.Google Scholar
Pomerantz, S. B., & de, Villiers P. A. (in preparation). Teaching vocabulary in narrative context.Google Scholar
Quigley, S. P., & King, C. M. (1980). Syntactic performance of hearing impaired and normal hearing individuals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 329356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quigley, S. P., & Paul, P. (1984). Language and deafness. San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L. (1990). Preschoolers' QUIL: Quick incidental learning of words. In Ramsden, G. Conti & Snow, C. E. (Eds.), Children's language (Vol. 7). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Silverman, T., & Rosenstein, J. (1969). The contribution of associative processes to written meaning. In Rosenstein, J. & MacGinitie, W. (Eds.), Verbal behavior of the deaf child: Studies of word meanings and associations. New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1987). Most vocabulary is learned from context. In McKeown, M. & Curtis, M. (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Powell, J. S. (1983). Comprehending verbal comprehension. American Psychologist, 38, 878893.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walter, G. (1978). Lexical abilities of hearing and hearing-impaired children. American Annals of the Deaf, 123, 976982.Google ScholarPubMed
Werner, H., & Kaplan, E. (1952). The acquisition of word meaning: A developmental study. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development (No. 15), 3120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolk, S. A., & Schildroth, A. (1984). Consistency of an associational strategy used by hearingimpaired students. Journal of Research in Reading, 7, 135142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoshinaga-Itano, C., & Downey, D. M. (1986). A hearing-impaired child's acquisition of schemata: Something's missing. Topics in Language Disorders, 7, 4557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar