No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Disciplining the disciplined: Making sense of the gender gap that lies at the core of puritanical morals
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 October 2023
Abstract
Because suppression of sex has been and is at the core of puritanical morals, a proper account thereof would need to explain why suppression of sex has been largely directed towards the human female. Not only do the authors not account for this pattern, but their general model would seem to predict the reverse – that is, greater suppression/control of the male libido.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Baumeister, R. F., Catanese, K. R., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Is there a gender difference in strength of sex drive? Theoretical views, conceptual distinctions, and a review of relevant evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(3), 242–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Reynolds, T., Winegard, B., & Vohs, K. D. (2017). Competing for love: Applying sexual economics theory to mating contests. Journal of Economic Psychology, 63, 230–241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2017.07.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., & Twenge, J. M. (2002). Cultural suppression of female sexuality. Review of General Psychology, 6(2), 166–203. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.6.2.166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2012). The evolutionary psychology of crime. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Criminology, 1(1), 90–98.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2021). When men behave badly: The hidden roots of sexual deception, harassment, and assault. Little Brown Spark.Google Scholar
Endendijk, J. J., van Baar, A. L., & Deković, M. (2020). He is a stud, she is a slut! A meta-analysis on the continued existence of sexual double standards. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 24(2), 163–190. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868319891310CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frankenbach, J., Weber, M., Loschelder, D. D., Kilger, H., & Friese, M. (2022). Sex drive: Theoretical conceptualization and meta-analytic review of gender differences. Psychological Bulletin, 148(9-10), 621–661. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000366CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kreager, D. A., Staff, J., Gauthier, R., Lefkowitz, E. S., & Feinberg, M. E. (2016). The double standard at sexual debut: Gender, sexual behavior and adolescent peer acceptance. Sex Roles, 75(7), 377–392. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0618-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marks, M. J., Young, T. M., & Zaikman, Y. (2018). The sexual double standard in the real world. Social Psychology, 50(2), 67–79. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000362Google Scholar
Rudman, L. A. (2017). Myths of sexual economics theory: Implications for gender equality. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 41(3), 299–313. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684317714707CrossRefGoogle Scholar
You have
Access
Fitouchi et al.'s target article is so capably written, so bold in ambition and scope, and so rich in ideas that seemingly only a churl would choose to dwell on its faults. Alas, churls we must be!
For, insofar as control and strident suppression of sex (Baumeister & Twenge, Reference Baumeister and Twenge2002) has always been at the core of puritanical morals, it seems that explaining these morals requires some type of account of why the said suppression has been far more intensely concerned with one half of our species, that is, the human female.
But therein lies the rub. Not only do Fitouchi et al. not give an account of this pattern, but their model, it seems, should indicate the reverse. That is, if the principal aim of puritanical morals is to reduce the occurrence of “antisocial” acts that stem from indulging/expressing one's “hedonistic impulses” (related to sexual longings), the morals' proponents should channel the bulk of their puritan efforts toward the male libido. Not only is it rather clear that males have stronger desires (Baumeister, Catanese, & Vohs, Reference Baumeister, Catanese and Vohs2001; Frankenbach, Weber, Loschelder, Kilger, & Friese, Reference Frankenbach, Weber, Loschelder, Kilger and Friese2022), but males also appear to be more willing and able to use violent means (be it against their partners or their potential rivals) (e.g., see Buss [Reference Buss2012, Reference Buss2021] for an overview) toward their sexual ends. Moreover, the contrasts in question are so strikingly clear that they cannot be ignored by any puritan scheme that runs on the functional logic Fitouchi et al. stipulate. Thus, given the facts as they stand, it seems that Fitouchi et al. should make a clear prediction that it is men everywhere (the less “disciplined” half) that puritanical morals would target first and foremost.
Yet the reverse is the case – Baumeister and colleagues (Baumeister, Reynolds, Winegard, & Vohs, Reference Baumeister, Reynolds, Winegard and Vohs2017; Baumeister & Twenge, Reference Baumeister and Twenge2002) provide a broad overview of the historic suppression/control of female libido and Kreager, Staff, Gauthier, Lefkowitz, and Feinberg (Reference Kreager, Staff, Gauthier, Lefkowitz and Feinberg2016), Marks, Young, and Zaikman (Reference Marks, Young and Zaikman2018), and Endendijk, van Baar, and Deković (Reference Endendijk, van Baar and Deković2020) report a range of results that seem to support the persistence of a gender-based double standard that leads to more disapproval of women's sexual conduct than similar conduct by men. One could also advert to the historic existence of Magdalen “homes”/asylums for so-called “fallen women” (with no counterpart in the form of “womanizer asylums”) as well as the fact that the language (e.g., English, German, or Russian) includes more pejorative terms for so-called “promiscuous” women than so-called “promiscuous” men (Endendijk et al., Reference Endendijk, van Baar and Deković2020).
There have been varied attempts, some more persuasive than other, to make sense of these facts (Baumeister & Twenge, Reference Baumeister and Twenge2002; Baumeister et al., Reference Baumeister, Reynolds, Winegard and Vohs2017; Rudman, Reference Rudman2017), but all of these varied attempts at least begin with the premise that there is, like it or not, a clear cultural pattern – the gendered suppression of sex – that must be accounted for, a premise Fitouchi et al. appear unable to grant.
Financial support
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Competing interest
None.