Hostname: page-component-68945f75b7-9klrw Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-06T05:25:25.018Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Individual invention versus socio-ecological innovation: Unifying the behavioral and evolutionary sciences

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 December 2007

Lauren McCall
Affiliation:
National Evolutionary Synthesis Center, Durham, NC 27705. lauren.mccall@duke.eduwww.nescent.org

Abstract

Great promise for the evolutionary analysis of animal behavior lies in the distinction between generative novelties and the evolutionary innovations to which they can give rise. Ramsey et al. succeed in emphasizing the contribution of individual learning and intelligence to behavioral innovations, but do not correct the tendency to confound individual invention with socio-ecological or group-level innovation.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baldwin, J. M. (1896a) A new factor in evolution. American Naturalist 30:441–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P. (2004) The active role of behaviour in evolution. Biology and Philosophy 19:283–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P. P. G. (1988) The active role of behaviour in evolution. In: Evolutionary processes and metaphors, ed. Ho, M.-W. & Fox, S. W., pp. 191207. Wiley.Google Scholar
Danchin, E., Giraldeau, L. A., Valone, T. J. & Wagner, R. H. (2004) Public information: From nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science 305:487–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erwin, D. H. & Krakauer, D. C. (2004) Insights into innovation. Science 304:1117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fragaszy, D. & Visalberghi, E. (2001) Recognizing a swan: Socially-biased learning. Psychologia 44:8298.Google Scholar
Futuyma, D. (1998) Evolutionary biology 3rd edition. Sinauer.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. & McElreath, R. (2003) The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology 12:123–35.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. & Kendal, J. (2003) What the models say about social learning. In: Biology of traditions, ed. Fragaszy, D. & Perry, S.. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lloyd Morgan, C. (1896) On modification and variation. Science 4:733–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Odling-Smee, F. J., Laland, K. N. & Feldman, M. W. (2003) Niche construction: The neglected process in evolution. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N., eds. (2003b) Animal innovation. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, A. R. (1988) Does biology constrain culture? American Anthropologist 90:819–31.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1957) The strategy of the genes: A discussion of some aspects of theoretical biology. George Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar