No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Is an ecological approach radical enough?
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 February 2010
Abstract
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1981
References
Alcock, J. (1979) Animal behavior: An evolutionary approach. Sunderland, Mass.: Sinauer. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Argyris, C. (1975) Dangers in applying results from experimental social psychology. American Psychologist 30:469–85. [LP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barker, R. B. & Wright, H. F. (1955) Midwest and its children: The psychological ecology of an American town. New York: Harper & Row. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Bateson, P. P. G. (1973) Internal influences on early learning in birds. In: Constraints on Learning, ed. Hinde, R. A. & Hinde, J. S., pp. 101–16. London: Academic Press. [PB]Google Scholar
Baum, W. M. (1973a) The correlation-based law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 20:137–53. [JAN]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baum, W. M. (1973b) Time allocation and negative reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 20:313–22. [JAN]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bekoff, M. & Fox, M. W. (1972) Postnatal neural ontogeny: Environment-dependent and/or environment-expectant? Developmental Psychobiology 5:323–41. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bitterman, M. E. (1975) The comparative analysis of learning. Science 188:699–709. [RCG, TDJ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bitterman, M. E. (1976) Issues in the comparative psychology of learning. In: Evolution of brain and behavior in vertebrates, ed. Masterton, R. B., Bitterman, M. E., Campbell, C. B. G. & Hotton, N., pp. 217–25. Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Bitterman, M. E. & Woodard, W. T. (1976) Vertebrate learning: Common processes. In: Evolution of brain and behavior in vertebrates, ed. Masterton, R. B., Bitterman, M. E., Campbell, C. B. G. & Hotton, N., pp. 169–89. Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Bitterman, M. E.; LoLordo, V. M.; Overmier, J. B. & Rashotte, M. E., eds. (1979) Animal learning: Survey and analysis. New York: Plenum Press. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, W. J. (1959) Preadaptation and multiple evolutionary pathways. Evolution 13:194–211. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, W. J. (1980) The definition and recognition of biological adaptation. American Zoologist 20:217–27. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, W. J. & von Wahlert, G. (1965) Adaptation and the form-function complex. Evolution 19:269–99. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1970) Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. Psychological Review 77:32–48. [TDJ, AWL, JAN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1971) Species-specific defense reactions. In: Aversive conditioning and learning, ed. Brush, F. R., pp. 183–233. New York: Academic Press. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1972) The avoidance learning problem. In: The psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 6, ed. Bower, G. H., pp. 97–145. New York: Academic Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Brainerd, C. J. (1978) The stage question in cognitive-developmental theory. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1:173–213. [WRC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandon, R. N. (1978) Adaptation and evolutionary theory. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science 9:181–206. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breitmeyer, B. G. (1980) Unmasking visual masking: A look at the “why” behind the veil of the “how.” Psychological Review 87:52–69. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breland, K. & Breland, M. (1961) The misbehavior of organisms. American Psychologist 16:681–84. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977) Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist 32:513–531. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979) The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press. [WRC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brower, L. P.; Cook, L. M. & Croze, H. J. (1967) Predator responses to artificial Batesian mimics released in a neotropical environment. Evolution 21:11–23. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, P. L. & Jenkins, H. M. (1968) Auto-shaping of the pigeon's key-peck. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 11:1–8. [AWL]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunswik, E. (1952) The conceptual framework of psychology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [MTG, LP, TDJ]Google Scholar
Burtt, E. H. Jr (1979) The behavioral significance of color. New York and London: Garland STPM Press. [JPH]Google Scholar
Cabanac, M. (1974) Thermoregulatory behavior. In: Environmental physiology, ed. Robertshaw, D., pp. 231–69. London: Butterworths. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1974) Evolutionary epistemology. In: The philosophy of Karl Popper, vol. 1, ed. Schlipp, P. A., pp. 413–63. LaSalle, Ill.: Open Court Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Charlesworth, W. R. (1976) Human intelligence as adaptation: An ethological approach. In: The nature of intelligence, ed. Resnick, L., pp. 147–68. Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Charlesworth, W. R. (1978) Ethology: Understanding the other half of intelligence. Social Science Information 17:231–77. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charlesworth, W. R. (1979) An ethological approach to studying intelligence. Human Development 22:212–16. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, R. W. (1971) Einstein: The life and times. New York: World Publishing Co. [TRA]Google Scholar
Clarke, J. C.; Westbrook, R. F. & Irwin, J. (1979) Potentiation instead of overshadowing in the pigeon. Behavioral and Neural Biology 25:18–29. [SR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clutton-Brock, T. H. & Harvey, P. H. (1979) Comparison and adaptation. Proceedings of the Boyal Society of London, Series B 205:547–65. [TDJ]Google ScholarPubMed
Collias, N. E. & Collias, E. C. (1956) Some mechanisms of family integration in ducks. Auk 73:378–400. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, J. O. (1953) A gradient theory of multiple-choice learning. Psychological Review 60:15–22. [JWK]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darwin, C. R. (1859) On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. London: John Murray. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. R. (1876) The movements and habits of climbing plants. 2nd ed.New York: D. Appleton. [MTG]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1978) Beliefs about beliefs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1:568–70. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, P. A. (1977) Choice in concurrent schedules and a quantitative formulation of the law of effect. In: Handbook of operant behavior, ed. Honig, W. K. & Staddon, E. R., pp. 233–87. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall. [AWL, JAN]Google Scholar
Dobzhansky, Th. (1956) What is an adaptive trait? American Naturalist 90:337–347. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dobzhansky, Th. (1968) On some fundamental concepts of Darwinian biology. In: Evolutionary biology, vol. 2, ed. Dobzhansky, Th., Hecht, M. K. & Steere, W. M., pp. 1–34. New York: Appleton. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emlen, J. M. (1973) Ecology: An evolutionary approach. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Emlen, S. T. (1967a) Migratory orientation in the indigo bunting, Passerina cyanea. Part I: Evidence for the use of celestial cues. Auk 84:309–42. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emlen, S. T. (1967b) Migratory orientation in the indigo bunting, Passerina cyanea. Part II: Mechanism of migratory orientation. Auk 84:463–89. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emlen, S. T. (1969) The development of migratory orientation in young indigo buntings. Lining Bird 8:113–26. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Emlen, S. T. (1970) Celestial rotation: Its importance in the development of migratory orientation. Science 170:1198–1201. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emlen, S. T. (1972) The ontogenetic development of orientation capabilities. In: Animal orientation and navigation, ed. Caller, S. R., Schmidt-Koenig, K., Jacobs, G. J. & Belleville, R. F., pp. 191–210. Washington, D.C.: NASA (SP-262). [TDJ, JLL]Google Scholar
Emlen, S. T. (1975) Migration: Orientation and navigation. In: Avian biology, vol. 5, ed. Farner, D. S. & King, J. R., pp. 129–219. New York: Academic Press. [TDJ, JLL, HCP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emlen, S. T. & Oring, L. W. (1977) Ecology, sexual selection and the evolution of mating systems. Science 197: 215–23. [PB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galef, B. G. & Osborne, B. (1978) Novel taste facilitation of the association of visual cues with toxicosis in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 92:907–16. [MTG, TDJ, SR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carcia, J., Ervin, F. R. & Koelling, R. A. (1966) Learning with prolonged delay of reinforcement. Psychonomic Science 5:121–22. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Garcia, J. & Ervin, F. R. (1968) Gustatory-visceral and telereceptor-cutaneous conditioning - adaptations in internal and external milieus. Communications in Behavioral Biology Part A, 1:389–415. [JG]Google Scholar
Garcia, J. & Koelling, R. A. (1966) Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science 4:123–24. [JG, TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia, J.; McGowan, B. K. & Creen, K. F. (1969) Biological constraints on conditioning. In: Classical Conditioning II: Current research and theory, ed. Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. F., Hamilton, Ontario: McMaster University Press. [JG]Google Scholar
Garcia, J. & Hankins, W. G. (1975) The evolution of bitter and the acquisition of toxiphobia. In: Olfaction and taste V, ed. Denton, D. A. & Coghlan, J. P., pp. 39–45. New York: Academic Press. [JG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia, J.; Palmerino, C. C.; Rusiniak, K. W. & Kiefer, S. W. (1980, in press) Taste aversions and the nurture of instinct. In: The Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, ed. McCaugh, J. L. & Thompson, R. F.. New York: Plenum Publishing Corporation. [JG]Google Scholar
Garcia, J. & Rusiniak, K. W. (1980) What the nose learns from the mouth. In: Chemical Signals, ed. Muller-Schwarze, D. & Silverstein, R. M., pp. 141–56. New York: Plenum Publishing Corporation. [JG, SR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghiselin, M. T. (1966) On semantic pitfalls of biological adaptation. Philosophy of Science 33:147–53. [MTG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghiselin, M. T. (1974) The economy of nature and the evolution of sex. Berkeley: University of California Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Gibbon, J.; Berryman, R. & Thompson, R. L. (1974) Contingency spaces and measures in classical and instrumental conditioning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 21:585–605. [JAN]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, J. A. (1979) The meaning of ecologically oriented inquiry in contemporary psychology. American Psychologist 34:127–40. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1966) The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. [TDJ, JK]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1977) The theory of affordances. In: Perceiving, acting and knowing: Towards an ecological psychology, ed. Shaw, R. & Bransford, J., pp. 67–82. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [TDJ, JK]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. [WRC, JK]Google Scholar
Classman, R. B. (1977) How can so little brain hold so much knowledge? Applicability of the principle of natural selection to mental processes. Psychological Record 27:393–415. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonzalez, R. C.; Ferry, M. & Powers, A. S. (1974) The adjustment of goldfish to reduction in magnitude of reward in massed trials. Animal Learning and Behavior 2:23–26. [RCG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonzalez, R. C., Potts, A., Pitcoff, K. & Bitterman, M. E. (1972) Runway performance of goldfish as a function of complete and incomplete reduction in amount of reward. Psychonomic Science 27:305–07. [RCG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goss-Custard, J. D. (1980) Feeding behavior of Redshank, Tringa totanus, and optimal foraging theory. In: Foraging behavior: Ecological, ethological, and psychological approaches, ed. Kamil, A. C. & Sargent, T. D., pp. 115–33. New York: Garland Publishing Inc. [ACK]Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1971) Development of species identification in birds. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1976) The roles of experience in the development of behavior and the nervous system. In: Neural and behavioral specificity, ed. Gottlieb, G., pp, 25–54. New York: Academic Press. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1978) Development of species identification in ducklings: IV. Change in species-specific perception caused by auditory deprivation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 92:375–87. [TDJ, SJS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottlieb, G. (1979) Development of species identification in ducklings: V. Perceptual differentiation in the embryo. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 93:831–54. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1980) Development of species identification in ducklings: VI. Specific experience required to maintain species-typical perception in Peking ducklings. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 94:579–87. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1977) Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [TRA]Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1980) The evolutionary biology of constraint. Daedalus 109(2):39–52. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. & Lewontin, R. C. (1979) The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist program. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 205:581–98. [GWB, TDJ]Google Scholar
Gray, L. & Tardif, R. R. (1979) The development of feeding diversity in deermice. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 93:1127–35. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hailman, J. P. (1967) The ontogeny of an instinct: The pecking response in chicks of the laughing gull (Larus atricilla L.) and related species. Behaviour Supplement 15:1–159. [JPH, TDJ]Google Scholar
Hailman, J. P. (1969) How an instinct is learned. Scientific American 221:98–106. [RCG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hailman, J. P. (1976) Uses of the comparative study of behavior. In: Evolution, brain, and behavior: Persistent problems, ed. Masterton, R. B., Hodos, W. & Jerison, J., pp. 13–22. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [JPH]Google Scholar
Hailman, J. P. (1977a) Bee dancing and evolutionary epistemology. American Naturalist, 111:187–89. [JPH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hailman, J. P. (1977b) Optical signals: Animal communication and light. Bloomington and London: Indiana University Press. [JPH]Google Scholar
Hailman, J. P. (1979) Environmental light and conspicuous colors. In: The behavioral significance of color, ed. Burtt, E. H. Jr, pp. 289–354. New York and London: Garland STPM Press. [JPH]Google Scholar
Hanson, N. (1969) Perception and discovery. An introduction to scientific inquiry. San Francisco: Freeman. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. (1970) On the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 13:243–66. [JAN]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrnstein, R. J. & Hineline, P. N. (1966) Negative reinforcement as shock frequency reduction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 9:421–30. [JAN]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinde, R. A. (1973) Constraints on learning - an introduction to the problems. In: Constraints on learning: Limitations and predispositions, ed. Hinde, R. A. & Stevenson, J.-Hinde, pp. 1–19. New York: Academic Press. [PB, TDJ]Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. (1975) The concept of function. In: Function and evolution in behavior: Essays in honor of Professor Niko Tinbergen, FRS, ed. Baerends, G., Beer, C. G. & Manning, A., pp. 3–15. Oxford: Clarendon Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Hoffman, H. S. & Ratner, A. (1973) A reinforcement model of imprinting: Implications for socialization in monkeys and man. Psychological Review 80:527–544. [RCG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogan, J. A. (1977) The ontogeny of food preferences in chicks and other animals. In: Leartting mechanisms in food selection, ed. Baker, L. M., Best, M. & Domjan, M., pp. 71–97. Waco, Tex.: Baylor University Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Mollis, K. L. (in press) Pavlovian conditioning of signal-centered action patterns and autonomic behavior: A biological analysis of function. Advances in the Study of Behavior. [SJS]Google Scholar
Hovland, C. I. (1951) Human learning and retention. In: Handbook of experimental psychology, ed. Stevens, S. S., pp. 613–89. New York: John Wiley & Sons. [WMS]Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1929) A functional interpretation of the conditioned reflex. Psychological Review 36:498–511. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1937) Mind, mechanism and adaptive behavior. Psychological Review 44:1–32. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1945) The place of innate individual and species differences in a natural-science theory of behavior. Psychological Review 52:55–60. [MTG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutchinson, G. E. (1957) Concluding remarks. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology, 22:415–27. [WMS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutchinson, G. E. (1967) The ecological theater and the evolutionary play. New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Hutchinson, G. E. (1978) An introduction to population ecology. New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press. [JK]Google Scholar
Immelmann, K. (1975) Ecological significance of imprinting and early learning. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 6:15–37. [WRC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jeffords, M. R.; Stemburg, J. G. & Waldbauer, G. P. (1979) Batesian mimicry: Field demonstration of the survival value of pipevine swallowtail and monarch color patterns. Evolution 33:275–86. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkins, H. M. (1979) Animal learning and behavior theory. In: The first century of experimental psychology, ed. Hearst, E., pp. 177–228. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Johnson, C.; Beaton, R. & Hall, K. (1975) Poison-based avoidance learning in non-human primates: Use of visual cues. Physiology and Behavior 14:403–07. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, T. D. (in press) Selective costs and benefits in the evolution of learning. In: Advances in the study of behavior, vol. 13, ed. Rosenblatt, J. S., Hinde, R. A., Beer, C. G. & Busnel, M.-C.. New York: Academic Press. [TFJ]Google Scholar
Johnston, T. D. (in preparation) “Learning” and “development” as concepts in the study of behavioral change. To appear in: Readings and essays in evolutionary epistemology, ed. Plotkin, H. C.. London: John Wiley & Sons. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Johnston, T. D. & Turvey, M. T. (1980) An ecological metatheory for theories of learning. In: The psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 14, ed. Bower, G. H., pp. 147–205. New York: Academic Press. [TRA, TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamil, A. C. (1978) Systematic foraging by a nectar-feeding bird, the amakihi (Loxops virens). Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 92:388–96. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamil, A. C. & Sargent, T. D. eds. (1980) Foraging behavior: Ecological, ethological and psychological approaches. New York: Carland Publishing Inc. [ACK]Google Scholar
Kamil, A. C.; Peters, J. & Lindstrom, F. (in press) An ecological perspective on the study of the allocation of behavior. In: Quantitative analyses of behavior II. Matching and Maximizing Accounts, ed. Commons, M. L., Herrnstein, R. J. & Rachlin, H.. New York: Ballinger. [ACK]Google Scholar
Kamin, L. J. (1968) “Attention-like” processes in classical conditioning. In: Miami symposium on the prediction of behavior: Aversive stimulation, ed. Jones, M. R., pp. 9–31. Miami: University of Miami Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Kamin, L. J. (1969) Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning. In: Punishment and aversive behavior, ed. Campbell, B. A. & Church, R. M., pp. 279–96. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Kantor, J. R. (1971) The aim and progress of psychology and other sciences. Chicago: Principia Press. [JCM]Google Scholar
Kimble, G. A. (1973) Scientific psychology in transition. In: Contemporary approaches to learning and memory, ed. McGuigan, F. J. & Lumsden, D. B., pp. 1–19. Washington, D.S.: V. H. Winston & Sons. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R. (1978) Optimal foraging: Decision rules for predators. In: Bahavioural ecology, ed. Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B., pp. 23–63. Oxford: Blackwell. [ACK]Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R.; Erichsen, J. T.; Webber, M. I. & Charnov, E. L. (1977) Optimal prey selection in the great tit (Parus major). Animal Behaviour 25:30–38. [ACK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohler, Wolfgang (1925) The mentality of apes. London: Routledge & K. Paul. (Translated from the German.) [JK]Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (1962) The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Kuo, Z. Y. (1922) How are our instincts acquired? Psychological Review 29:344–65. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuo, Z. Y. (1929) The net result of the anti-heredity movement in psychology. Psychological Review 36:181–99. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lachman, J. L. & Lachman, R. (1979) Theories of memory organization and human evolution. In: Memory organization and structure, ed. Puff, R. C., pp. 133–193. New York: Academic Press. [TDJ, JLL]Google Scholar
Lachman, J. L. & Lachman, R. (1980) Picture-naming: Retrieval and activation of long-term memory. In: New directions in memory and aging: Proceedings of the George Talland Memorial Conference, ed. Poon, L. W., Fozard, J. L., Cermak, L. S., Arenberg, D., and Thompson, L. W., pp. 313–43. [JLL]Google Scholar
Lachman, R.; Lachman, J. L. & Butterfield, E. C. (1979) Cognitive psychology and information processing: An introduction. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [JLL]Google Scholar
Lakatos, I. (1970) Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programmes. In: Criticism and the growth of knowledge, ed. Lakatos, I. & Musgrave, A., pp. 91–195. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lattal, K. A. (1974) Combinations of response-reinforcer dependence and independence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour 22:357–62. [JAN]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lea, S. E. G. (1979) Foraging and reinforcement schedules in the pigeon: Optimal and non-optimal aspects of choice. Animal Behavior 27:821–828. [ACK, SJS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehrman, D. S. (1953) A critique of Konrad Lorenz's theory of instinct. Quarterly Review of Biology 28:337–63. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehrman, D. S. (1962) Varieties of learning and memory in animals. In: Macromolecular specificity and biological memory, ed. Schmitt, F. O., pp. 108–10. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Lehrman, D. S. (1970) Semantic and conceptual issues in the nature-nurture problem. In: Development and evolution of behavior. Essays in memory of T. C. Schneirla, ed. Aronson, L. R., Tobach, E., Lehrman, D. S. & Rosenblatt, J. S., pp. 17–52. San Francisco: Freeman. [TDJ, SJS]Google Scholar
Lett, B. T. (1980) Taste potentiates color-sickness associations in pigeons and quail. Animal Learning and Behavior 8:193–98. [AWL, SR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewontin, R. C. (1978) Adaptation. Scientific American 239:212–30. [TRA, TDJ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewontin, R. C. (1979) Sociobiology as an adaptationist program. Behavioral Science 24:5–14. [TDJ, BS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lockard, R. B. (1971) Reflections on the rise and fall of comparative psychology: Is there a message for us all? American Psychologist 26:168–179. [ACK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logue, A. W. (1978) John B. Watson and the continuity of the species. Behaviorism 6:71–79. [AWL]Google Scholar
Logue, A. W. (1979) Taste aversion and the generality of the laws of learning. Psychological Bulletin 86:276–296. [TDS, AWL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logue, A. W. (1980) Visual cues for illness-induced aversions in the pigeon. Behavioral and Neural Biology 28:372–77. [AWL]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logue, A. W. (in press a) The origins of behaviorism. In: A history of modern psychology: Concepts, methods, viewpoints, ed. Buxton, C.. New York: Academic Press. [AWL]Google Scholar
Logue, A. W. (in press b) The growth of behaviorism. In: A history of modern psychology: Concepts, methods, viewpoints, ed. Buxton, C.. New York: Academic Press. [AWL]Google Scholar
Logue, A. W. (in press c) The generality of taste aversion learning in humans. In: Quantitative analyses of behavior: Acquisition, ed. Commons, M. L., Wagner, A. R. & Herrnstein, R. J.. Cambridge, Mass.Ballinger Publishing Co. [AWL]Google Scholar
LoLordo, V. M. (1979) Constraints on learning. In: Animal learning: Survey and analysis, ed. Bitterman, M. E., LoLordo, V. M., Overmier, J. B. & Rashotte, M. E., pp. 473–504. New York: Plenum Press. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1941) Kant's doctrine of the a priori in the light of contemporary biology. General Systems, vol. VII. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 1962, pp. 23–35. (Originally published in Blätter für Deutsche Philosophie, 1941, 15:94–125.) [WRC]Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1965) Evolution and modification of behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [TDJ, WMS]Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1969) Innate bases of learning. In: On the biology of learning, ed. Pribram, K. H., pp. 13–93. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1977) Behind the mirror: A search for a natural history of human knowledge. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. [WRC]Google Scholar
MacArthur, R. H. & Pianka, E. R. (1966) On optimal use of a patchy environment. American Naturalist 100:603–09. [ACK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacArthur, R. W. & Wilson, E. O. (1967) The theory of island biogeography. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press. [TRA]Google Scholar
McCall, R. B. (1977) Challenges to a science of developmental psychology. Child Development 48:333–44. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGurk, H., ed. (1977) Ecological factors in human development. New York: North Holland. [WRC]Google Scholar
McNamara, J. & Houston, A. (1980) The application of statistical decision theory to animal behavior. Journal of Theoretical Biology 85:673–90. [SJS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maier, N. R. F. & Schneirla, T. C. (1935) Principles of animal psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill. [JWK]Google Scholar
Malone, J. C. Jr (1973) A note on general process learning theorists. Psychological Review 80:305. [JCM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malone, J. C. Jr (1975) The “paradigms” of learning. Psychological Record 25:479–89. [TDJ, JCM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malone, J. C. Jr (1978) Beyond the operant analysis of behavior. Behavior Therapy 9:584–91. [JCM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P. R. (1970) A comparative approach to vocal development: Song learning in the white-crowned sparrow. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Monographs 71 (No. 2, Pt. 2):1–25. [TDJ, JAN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P. R. (1977) Perception and innate knowledge. In: The nature of life, ed. Heidcamp, W. H., pp. 111–39. Baltimore: University Park Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Marler, P. R. & Mundinger, P. (1971) Vocal learning in birds. In: Ontogeny of vertebrate behavior, ed. Moltz, H., pp. 389–450. New York: Academic Press. [TDJ, SJS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, H. L. & Langenheim, J. H. (1957) Language analysis and the concept environment. Ecology 38:325–40. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayr, E. (1963) Animal species and evolution. Cambridge, Mass.: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayr, E. (1974) Behavior programs and evolutionary strategies. American Scientist 62:650–59. [TRA, WRC, HP]Google ScholarPubMed
Mayr, E. (1974) Teleological and teleonomic: A new analysis. Boston Studies in the Philosophy of Science 14:91–117. [LP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Medawar, P. B. (1969) Induction and intuition in scientific thought. Philadelphia: American Philosophical Society. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Miller, D. B. (1977) Roles of naturalistic observation in comparative psychology. American Psychologist 32:211–19. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, D. B. & Cottlieb, G. (1978) Maternal vocalizations of mallard ducks (Anas platyrhynchos). Animal Behaviour 26:1178–94. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills, W. (1898) The nature and development of animal intelligence. London: T. Fisher Unwin. [WRC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, B. R. (1973) The role of directed Pavlovian reactions in simple instrumental learning in the pigeon. In: Hinde, R. A. and Stevenson-Hinde, J. (eds), Constraints on Learning. London: Academic Press. [SR]Google Scholar
Nevin, J. A. (1974) Response strength in multiple schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 21:389–408. [JAN]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nevin, J. A. (1979) Reinforcement schedules and response strength. In: Reinforcement and the organization of behavior, ed. Zeiler, M. & Harzem, P.. Chichester, England: Wiley. [JAN]Google Scholar
Norman, D. A. (1980) Twelve issues for cognitive science. Cognitive Science 4:1–32. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Novikoff, A. B. (1945) The concept of integrative levels and biology. Science 101:209–15. [WMS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oppenheim, R. W. (in press) Preformation and epigenesis in the origins of the nervous system and behavior: Issues, concepts and their history. In: Perspectives in ethology, vol. 5, ed. Bateson, P. P. G. & Klopfer, P. H.. New York: Plenum Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Orians, G. H. (1969) On the evolution of mating systems in birds and mammals. American Naturalist 103:589–603. [PB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pantin, C. F. A. (1952) The elementary nervous system. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 140:147–68. [RAH]Google ScholarPubMed
Pavlov, I. P. (1927) Conditioned reflexes, tr. Anrep, G. V.. London: Oxford University Press. [JCM]Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. (1932) The reply of a physiologist to psychologists. Psychological Review 39:91–127. [JCM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pert, A. & Gonzalez, R. C. (1974) Behavior of the turtle (Chrysemys Picta Picta) in simultaneous, successive and behavioral contrast situations. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 87:526–38. [RCG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrinovich, L. (1973) Darwin and the representative expression of reality. In: Darwin and facial expression, ed. Ekman, P., pp. 223–56. New York: Academic Press. [LP]Google Scholar
Petrinovich, L. (1976) Molar reductionism. In: Knowing, thinking, and believing, ed. Petrinovich, L. & McGaugh, J. L., pp. 11–27. New York: Plenum Press. [LP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrinovich, L. (1979) Probabilistic functionalism: A conception of research method. American Psychologist 34:373–90. [TDJ, LP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrinovich, L. (in press) A method for the study of development. In: Interdisciplinary study of development: The Bielefeld conference, ed. Immelmann, K., Barlow, G., Main, M. & Petrinovich, L.. New York: Cambridge University Press. [LP]Google Scholar
Pietrewicz, A. T. & Kamil, A. C. (1977) Visual detection of cryptic prey by blue jays (Cyanocitta cristata). Science 195:580–82. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pietrewicz, A. T. & Kamil, A. C. (1979) Search image formation in the blue jay (Cyanocitta cristata). Science 204:1332–33. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1971) Biology and knowledge. Edinburgh: The University of Edinburgh Press. [HCP]Google Scholar
Plotkin, H. C. (in press) The evolution of closed and open programmes of development. In: Development in the nervous system, ed. Garrod, D. R. & Feldman, J.. Cambridge University Press. [HCP]Google Scholar
Plotkin, H. C. & Odling-Smee, F. J. (1979) Learning, change and evolution: An enquiry into the teleonomy of learning. Advances in the Study of Behaviour 10:1–41. [HCP, SJS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pyke, G. H.; Pulliam, H. R. & Charnov, E. L. (1977) Optimal foraging: A selective review of theory and tests. Quarterly Review of Biology 52:137–54. [ACK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. (1978) Computational models and empirical constraints. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1:93–99. [JLL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rachlin, H. & Baum, W. M. (1972) Effects of alternative reinforcement: Does the source matter? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 18:231–41. [JAN]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reed, E. S. (1979) An evolutionary analysis of intentional activity. Masters Thesis, University of Edinburgh. [WRC]Google Scholar
Reed, E. S. & Jones, R. K. (1979) James Gibson's ecological revolution in psychology. Philosophy of Social Science 9:189–204. [JK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. (1967) Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. Psychological Review 74:71–80. [PR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, R. A. (1972) Informational variables in Pavlovian conditioning. In: The psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 6, ed. Bower, G. H., pp. 1–46. New York: Academic Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. (1980) Pavlovian second-order conditioning. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [AWL]Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. & Holland, P. G. (1976) Some behavioral approaches to the study of learning. In: Neural mechanisms of learning and memory, ed. Rosenzweig, M. R. & Bennett, E. L., pp. 165–92. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. & Wagner, A. R. (1972) A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement. In: Classical conditioning II, ed. Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. F., pp. 64–99. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Revusky, S. (1971) The role of interference in association over a delay. In: Animal memory, ed. Honig, W. K. & James, P. H. R., pp. 155–213. New York: Academic Press. [PB, TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Revusky, S. (1977) Interference with progress by the scientific establishment: Examples from flavor aversion learning. In: Food aversion learning, ed. Milgram, N. W., Krames, L. & Alloway, T. M., pp. 53–71. New York: Plenum Press. [AWL]Google Scholar
Revusky, S. (1977) Learning as a general process with an emphasis on data from feeding experiments. In: Food aversion learning, ed. Milgram, N. W., Krames, L. & Alloway, T. M., pp. 1–51. New York: Plenum Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Revusky, S. & Garcia, J. (1970) Learned associations over long delays. In: The psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 4, ed. Bower, G. H. & Spence, J. T., pp. 1–84. New York: Academic Press. [TDJ, JAN]Google Scholar
Riley, A. L. & Baril, L. L. (1976) Conditioned taste aversions: a bibliography. Animal Learning and Behavior 4(B):1S–13S. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P. (1976a) The evolution of intelligence and access to the cognitive unconscious. In: Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology, vol. 6, ed. Sprague, J. A. & Epstein, A. N., pp. 245–80. New York: Academic Press. [PR]Google Scholar
Rozin, P. (1976b) The selection of foods by rats, humans and other animals. In: Advances in the study of behavior, vol. 6, ed. Rosenblatt, J., Hinde, R. A., Beer, C. & Shaw, E., pp. 21–76. New York: Academic Press. [PR]Google Scholar
Rozin, P. (1977) The significance of learning mechanisms in food selection: Some biology, psychology and sociology of science. In: Learning mechanisms in food selection, ed. Barker, L. M., Best, M. & Domjan, M., pp. 557–89. Waco, Texas: Baylor University Press. [PR]Google Scholar
Rozin, P. & Kalat, J. W. (1971) Specific hungers and poison avoidance as adaptive specializations of learning. Psychological Review 78:459–86. [TDJ, JWK, PR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P. & Kalat, J. W. (1972) Learning as a situation-specific adaptation. In: Biological boundries of learning, ed. Seilgman, M. E. P. & Hager, J. L., pp. 66–96. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. [PR]Google Scholar
Sackett, G. P., ed. (1978) Observing behavior: Theory and applications in mental retardation. Baltimore: University Park Press. [WRC]Google Scholar
Schleidt, W. M. (1981) The behavior of organisms, as it is linked to genes and populations. In: Perspectives in Ethology, ed. Bateson, P. P. G. & Klopfer, P. H., Vol. 4, pp. 147–55. New York: Plenum Publishing Corporation. [WMS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schleidt, W. M. & Crawley, J. N. (1980) Patterns in the behaviour of organisms. Jounal for Social and Biological Structure 3:1–15. [WMS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sehneirla, T. C. (1949) Levels in the psychological capacities of animals. In: Philosophy for the Future, ed. Sellars, R. W. et al. New York: Macmillan. [RAH]Google Scholar
Sehneirla, T. C. (1950) The relationship between observation and experiment in the field study of behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 51:1022–44. [TDJ, JWK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sehneirla, T. C. (1956) Interrelationships of the “innate” and the “acquired” in instinctive behavior. In: L'Instinct dans le comportement des animaux et de l'homme, ed. Grasse, P.-P., pp. 387–452. Paris: Masson. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Sehneirla, T. C. (1971) Army ants: A study in social organization. San Francisco: Freeman. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. (1974) On going back to nature: A review of Seligman and Hager's Biological Boundaries of Learning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 21:183–98. [TDJ, BS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1970) On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychological Review 77:406–18. [TDJ, JCM, LP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. & Hager, J. L., eds. (1972) Biological boundaries of learning. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall. [JG, TDJ, AWL]Google Scholar
Sevenster, P. (1973) Incompatibility of response and reward. In: Constraints on Learning, ed. Hinde, R. A. and Stevenson-Hinde, J.. Academic Press: London. [SR]Google Scholar
Shaw, R. & Bransford, J., eds. (1977) Perceiving, acting, and knowing: Toward an ecological psychology. New York: John Wiley & Sons. [WRC]Google Scholar
Shaw, R. E. & Turvey, M. T. (in press) Coalitions as models for ecosystems: A realist perspective on perceptual organization. In: Perceptual organization, ed. Kubovy, M. & Pomerantz, J.. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [TRA, JK]Google Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J. (1972) Constraints on learning. In: Advances in the study of behavior, vol. 4, ed. Lehrman, D. S., Hinde, R. A. & Shaw, E., pp. 1–68. New York: Academic Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J. (1975) Reinforcement and the organization of behavior in golden hamsters: Hunger, environment, and food reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 104:56–87. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J. (in press) Function and mechanism in learning. In: Advances in analysis of beavior, vol. 3: Biological factors in learning, ed. Zeiler, M. & Harzem, P.. New York: John Wiley & Sons. [SJS]Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1969) The sciences of the artificial. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1980) Cognitive science: The newest science of the artificial. Cognitive Science 4:33–46. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1931) The concept of the reflex in the description of behavior. Journal of General Psychology 5:427–58. [JCM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1950) Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review 57:193–216. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. (1959) A case history in scientific method. In: Psychology: A study of a science, vol. 2, ed. Koch, S., pp. 359–79. New York: McGraw-Hill. [JG, AWL]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1966) The phylogeny and ontogeny of behavior. Science 153:1205–13. [AWL]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slobodkin, L. B. (1968) Toward a predictive theory of evolution. In: Population biology and evolution, ed. Lewontin, R. C., pp. 187–205. Syracuse, N.Y.: Syracuse University Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Slobodkin, L. B. & Rapoport, A. (1974) An optimum strategy of evolution. Quarterly Review of Biology 49:181–200. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Small, W. S. (1901) Experimental study of the mental processes of the rat. II American Journal of Psychology 12:206–39. [TDJ, HR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smart, J. J. C. (1963) Philosophy and scientific realism. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. [MTG]Google Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1947) The role of secondary reinforcement in delayed reward learning. Psychological Review 54:1–8. [JG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Staddon, J. E. R. (1975) Learning as adaptation. In: Handbook of learning and cognitive processes, vol. 2, ed. Estes, W. K., pp. 37–98. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Staddon, J. E. R. ed. (1980) Limits to action: The allocation of individual behavior. New York: Academic Press. [JAN]Google Scholar
Staddon, J. E. R. & Simmelhag, V. L. (1971) The “superstition” experiment: A reexamination of its implications for the principles of adaptive behavior. Psychological Review 78:3–43. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutherland, N. S. & Mackintosh, N. J. (1971) Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning. New York: Academic Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Taylor, E. F. & Wheeler, J. A. (1966) Spacetime physics. San Francisco: Freeman. [TRA]Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1951) The study of instinct. London: Oxford University Press. [RAH. TDJ, LP]Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1963) On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 20:410–29. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tinbergen, N.; Broekhuysen, G. K.; Feekes, F.; Houghton, J. C. W.; Kruuk, H. & Szulc, E. (1962) Egg shell removal by the black-headed gull, Larus ridibundus, L.; A behaviour component of camouflage. Behaviour 19:74–117. [PB]Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1938) The determiners of behavior at a choice point. Psychological Review 45:1–41. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1949) There is more than one kind of learning. Psychological Review 56:144–55. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turvey, M. T. & Shaw, R. E. (1979) The primacy of perceiving: an ecological reformulation of perception for understanding memory. In: Perspectives on memory research, ed. Nilsson, L.-G, pp. 167–222. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [TRA, TDJ]Google Scholar
Uexküll, J. von (1957) A stroll through the worlds of animals and men. In: Instinctive behavior, ed. & trans. Schiller, C. H., pp. 5–80. New York: International University Press. [WMS]Google Scholar
Walls, G. L. (1942) The vertebrate eye and its adaptive radiation. Bloomfield Hills, Mich.: Cranbrook Institute of Science. [TDJ, PR, SR]Google Scholar
Whittaker, R. H., Levin, S. A. & Root, R. B. (1973) Niche, habitat, and ecotope. American Naturalist 107:321–38. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiens, J. A. (1970) Effects of early experience on substrate pattern selection in Rana aurora tadpoles. Copeia 1970:543–48. [TDJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilcoxon, H. C., Dragoin, W. B. & Kral, P. A. (1971) Illness-induced aversions in rat and quail: Relative salience of visual and gustatory cues. Science 171:826–28. [TDJ, AWL]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, C. G. (1966) Adaptation and natural selection. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press. [TDJ]Google Scholar
Yarczower, M. & Hazlett, L. (1977) Evolutionary scales and anagenesis. Psychological Bulletin 84:1088–97. [RCG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed