Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-qxsvm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-26T23:47:59.432Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The human fear paradox turns out to be less paradoxical when global changes in human aggression and language evolution are considered

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 May 2023

Antonio Benítez-Burraco
Affiliation:
Department of Spanish, Linguistics, and Theory of Literature (Linguistics), Faculty of Philology, University of Seville, Seville 41004, Spain abenitez8@us.es http://antoniobenitez.wix.com/benitez-burraco
Ljiljana Progovac
Affiliation:
Department of English (Linguistics), College of Liberal Arts & Science, Wayne State University, Detroit, MI 48202, USA

Abstract

Our commentary focuses on the interaction between Grossmann's fearful ape hypothesis (FAH) and the human self-domestication hypothesis (HSDH), also taking into account language acquisition and evolution. Although there is considerable overlap between the two hypotheses, there are also some discrepancies, and our goal is to consider the extent to which HSDH can explain the phenomena identified by FAH without invoking fearfulness as directly adaptive.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Our commentary addresses the discrepancies pointed out by Grossmann between his fearful ape hypothesis (FAH) and the human self-domestication hypothesis (HSDH). In brief, according to FAH, fearfulness itself was adaptive in the context of cooperative caregiving unique to human group life, resulting in increased caregiving by adults, and ultimately, in increased cooperation with others. On the other hand, according to HSDH, increased cooperation in humans resulted from (sexual) selection for less aggressive partners (e.g., Hare, Reference Hare2017; Hare & Woods, Reference Hare and Woods2020), rather than for fearfulness per se. Grossmann concludes that HSDH is not well-positioned to explain what he identifies as the phenomenon of “increased fearfulness” in humans, given that successful animal domestication leads to a reduction in fearfulness, rather than an increase (Zeder, Reference Zeder2012). Our contention is that certain aspects of FAH may prove incompatible with some features of the human phenotype, which are better accommodated under HSDH, while at the same time, much of FAH, including cautiousness, can actually be accommodated under HSDH. The evidence we use to support these claims includes research on language acquisition and language evolution, as well as neurobiological considerations.

First, FAH relies, to a great extent, on research suggesting that human infants exhibit increased fearful responses to strangers, particularly Herrmann, Hare, Cissewski, and Tomasello's (Reference Herrmann, Hare, Cissewski and Tomasello2011) study. Notably, however, these authors also found that human children behave like bonobos in this respect. But bonobos have been claimed to have gone through a self-domestication process (Hare, Wobber, & Wrangham, Reference Hare, Wobber and Wrangham2012). Also, in the experiment by Herrmann et al., “strangers” are people totally unknown to children. But humans do exhibit reduced fearfulness toward more individuals than, for example, chimps, as our kinship systems go far beyond parent–child ties (Gowlett, Reference Gowlett and Callan2018). Chimps contrast with bonobos in the fact that they have not been self-domesticated. In this sense, it is also noteworthy that hospitality codes, as found in present-day hunter–gatherers, which entail tolerance (i.e., reduced fearfulness) toward strangers, became generalized in our species during Upper Paleolithic (see Nikolsky & Benítez-Burraco, Reference Nikolsky and Benítez-Burraco2022, for discussion), in parallel with an increase in self-domestication features, as attested by the anthropological record (Cieri et al., Reference Cieri, Churchill, Franciscus, Tan and Hare2014). It thus seems to us that FAH is not well-positioned to accommodate some unique elements that characterize humans, which are better accommodated under HSDH, the argument which we will further develop below.

Focusing on language, in particular, research suggests that shyness can impact negatively on language acquisition. For instance, shyer children tend to perform worse on measures of word learning (Hilton, Twomey, & Westermann, Reference Hilton, Twomey and Westermann2019), seemingly because of a less-efficient formation and retention of novel word–object mappings (Hilton & Westermann, Reference Hilton and Westermann2017). Clinical conditions featuring abnormally high fearfulness and avoidance behaviors result in inadequate language acquisition, as is the case with Reactive attachment disorder (Raaska et al., Reference Raaska, Lapinleimu, Sinkkonen, Salmivalli, Matomäki, Mäkipää and Elovainio2012). Accordingly, it might be difficult to reconcile any trend toward increased fearfulness, as claimed by the FAH, with how human children acquire language. More specifically, at the age of 2.5 when children's fearfulness of novelty was compared to that of other primates in Herrmann et al.'s (Reference Herrmann, Hare, Cissewski and Tomasello2011) study, children have already experienced contact with a variety of cultural objects, some dangerous for them, and have already mastered a great deal of language, including being exposed to many warnings of dangers around them. This element in itself, unique to humans, may be contributing to the state of enhanced cautiousness/fearfulness in children, rather than selection for fearfulness itself.

With respect to language evolution, there is extensive research supporting the view that increased self-domestication in humans favoured the potentiation of many of the traits that make language more complex through a cultural mechanism, specifically, learning and shared intentionality (Thomas & Kirby, Reference Thomas and Kirby2018). In our own work, we have emphasized the role of management of aggression in language evolution, in particular reactive aggression, which is a crucial ingredient of HSDH. In this respect, we have proposed that self-domestication favoured the emergence of early grammars, especially suitable for verbal aggression (insult), which in turn contributed to a gradual replacement of reactive physical aggression with verbal/cognitive contest, so that language complexity and self-domestication processes were engaged in a mutually reinforcing feedback loop (Benítez-Burraco & Progovac, Reference Benítez-Burraco and Progovac2020; Progovac & Benítez-Burraco, Reference Progovac and Benítez-Burraco2019). In comparison, it is not clear how increased fearfulness in humans would have resulted in the evolution of sophisticated languages.

Furthermore, we find that other outcomes of HSDH can account for the increased protective behavior toward children by adults, which is at the core of FAH, without a need to invoke selection for fearfulness. One such outcome is prolonged childhood/juvenile period, that is, helplessness, which, in itself, can be the cause of more adult caring and attention, including enhanced co-parenting, as children would have demanded more attention during longer periods and from more people (Bogins, Reference Bogins1999). Moreover, HSDH is associated with a reduction in sexual dimorphism, which in humans manifests itself as males becoming more like females (Gleeson & Kushnick, Reference Gleeson and Kushnick2018). If females on average are more cautious, that is, less bold or reckless than males (e.g., McLean & Anderson, Reference McLean and Anderson2009), and if fearfulness responses are also inversely correlated to physical strength (e.g., Manson et al., Reference Manson, Chua, Rodriguez, Barlev, Durkee and Lukaszewski2022), then this feminization brought about by self-domestication could by itself have contributed to the human fearfulness (or cautiousness) phenotype, without a need to invoke any direct selection for fearfulness.

Finally, we wish also to note that most if not all the neurobiological mechanisms involved in fearful responses and invoked to provide support for FAH are indeed impacted by (self-)domestication, from the dopamine system to the oxytocin system. Accordingly, changes in the dopamine system have been extensively documented in domesticated animals (e.g., Komiyama et al., Reference Komiyama, Iwama, Osada, Nakamura, Kobayashi, Tateno and Gojobori2014; Sato et al., Reference Sato, Rafati, Ring, Younis, Feng, Blanco-Aguiar and Andersson2020). Likewise, in humans, oxytocin has been identified as a target of sexual selection contributing to a reduction in physically aggressive behavior (Hare, Reference Hare2017). In our opinion, this further reinforces our argument that HSDH can account for many aspects of the human distinctive phenotype, including fearfulness/cautiousness, weakening the case for FAH.

Financial support

This research was funded by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (grant PID2020-114516GB-I00 funded by MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033 and by “ERDF A way of making Europe” to ABB).

Competing interest

None.

References

Benítez-Burraco, A., & Progovac, L. (2020). A four-stage model for language evolution under the effects of human self-domestication. Language & Communication, 73, 117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langcom.2020.03.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogins, B. (1999). Evolutionary perspectives on human growth. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 109153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cieri, R. L., Churchill, S. E., Franciscus, R. G., Tan, J., & Hare, B. (2014). Craniofacial feminization, social tolerance, and the origins of behavioral modernity. Current Anthropology, 55(4), 419443. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/677209CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gleeson, B. T., & Kushnick, G. (2018). Female status, food security, and stature sexual dimorphism: Testing mate choice as a mechanism in human self-domestication. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 167(3), 458469. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.23642CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gowlett, J. A. J. (2018). Kinship (early human), the archaeological evidence for. In Callan, H. (Ed.), The international encyclopedia of anthropology. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118924396.wbiea2343Google Scholar
Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 155186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, B., Wobber, V., & Wrangham, R. (2012). The self-domestication hypothesis: Evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression. Animal Behavior, 83(3), 573585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, B., & Woods, V. (2020). Survival of the friendliest: Understanding our origins and rediscovering our common humanity. Random House.Google Scholar
Herrmann, E., Hare, B., Cissewski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2011). A comparison of temperament in nonhuman apes and human infants. Developmental Science, 14(6), 13931405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilton, M., Twomey, K. E., & Westermann, G. (2019). Taking their eye off the ball: How shyness affects children's attention during word learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 183, 134145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilton, M., & Westermann, G. (2017). The effect of shyness on children's formation and retention of novel word–object mappings. Journal of Child Language, 44(6), 13941412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Komiyama, T., Iwama, H., Osada, N., Nakamura, Y., Kobayashi, H., Tateno, Y., & Gojobori, T. (2014). Dopamine receptor genes and evolutionary differentiation in the domestication of fighting cocks and long-crowing chickens. PLoS One, 9(7), e101778. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0101778CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manson, J. H., Chua, K. J., Rodriguez, N. N., Barlev, M., Durkee, P. K., & Lukaszewski, A. W. (2022). Sex differences in fearful personality traits are mediated by physical strength. Social Psychological and Personality Science. https://doi.org/10.1177/19485506221094086CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLean, C. P., & Anderson, E. R. (2009). Brave men and timid women? A review of the gender differences in fear and anxiety. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(6), 496505. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.05.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nikolsky, A., & Benítez-Burraco, A. (2022). Human aggression and music evolution: A model. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/a8up7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Progovac, L., & Benítez-Burraco, A. (2019). From physical aggression to verbal behavior: Language evolution and self-domestication feedback loop. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2807.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raaska, H., Lapinleimu, H., Sinkkonen, J., Salmivalli, C., Matomäki, J., Mäkipää, S., & Elovainio, M. (2012). Experiences of school bullying among internationally adopted children: Results from the Finnish Adoption (FINADO) study. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 43(4), 592611.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sato, D. X., Rafati, N., Ring, H., Younis, S., Feng, C., Blanco-Aguiar, J. A., … Andersson, L. (2020). Brain transcriptomics of wild and domestic rabbits suggests that changes in dopamine signalling and ciliary function contributed to evolution of tameness. Genome Biology and Evolution, 12(10), 19181928. https://doi.org/10.1093/gbe/evaa158CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, J., & Kirby, S. (2018). Self domestication and the evolution of language. Biological Philosophy, 33, 9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeder, M. A. (2012). The domestication of animals. Journal of Anthropological Research, 68(2), 161190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar