Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-pwrkn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-27T00:03:06.013Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

More than fear: Contributions of biobehavioral synchrony and infants' reactivity to cooperative care

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 May 2023

Elizabeth B. daSilva
Affiliation:
Division of Science, Indiana University-Purdue University Columbus, Columbus, IN 47203, USA elabendy@indiana.edu; https://www.iupuc.edu/science/contact-science/liz-dasilva.html
Bennett I. Bertenthal
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Indiana University-Bloomington, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA bbertent@indiana.edu; https://dcnlab.sitehost.iu.edu/

Abstract

We present two challenges to the fearful ape hypothesis: (1) biobehavioral synchrony precedes and moderates the effects of fear on cooperative care, and (2) cooperative care emerges in a more bidirectional manner than Grossmann acknowledges. We present evidence demonstrating how dyadic differences in co-regulation and individual differences in infants' reactivity shape caregivers' responses to infant affect.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Expressing and recognizing fear in conspecifics is critical to survival. In the fearful ape hypothesis, Grossmann thoughtfully integrates evidence from comparative, developmental, and neuroscience research to explain how fear developed differently in humans compared to our primate relatives. By focusing exclusively on fear, however, some of the most complex dynamic factors contributing to the development of cooperative care are neglected. We present two challenges to the fearful ape hypothesis: (1) biobehavioral synchrony is a precursor to cooperative care that precedes the development of fear and varies as a function of a multitude of factors, and (2) cooperative care emerges in a more bidirectional manner than implied by this target article.

First, most accounts, including Grossmann's, suggest that fear does not develop until the latter half of the first year. Yet, infants are already engaging in biobehavioral synchrony with their caregivers in the first few months of life (e.g., Feldman, Reference Feldman2007). Synchrony is highly associated with early cooperative behavior and contributes to the development of infants' emotion regulation (Feldman, Greenbaum, & Yirmiya, Reference Feldman, Greenbaum and Yirmiya1999), including their negative affect in novel or stressful situations such as the still-face paradigm (Tronick, Als, Adamson, Wise, & Brazelton, Reference Tronick, Als, Adamson, Wise and Brazelton1978). For example, infants often exhibit distress when mothers withdraw interactive cues during the still-face episode because it violates their expectations about parental responsiveness (Jones-Mason, Alkon, Coccia, & Bush, Reference Jones-Mason, Alkon, Coccia and Bush2018). Critically, synchrony is not present all the time even when the mother is contingently responsive, because successful affective communication between mother and infant involves a series of mismatched states that are repeatedly repaired through the dynamic and reciprocal interaction of the mother and child (Tronick & Beeghly, Reference Tronick and Beeghly2011). Recent work from our laboratory points to physiological synchrony as a key moderator of infants' affect regulation. We found that once mothers resume engagement following the still-face stressor, there is a significant reduction of negative affect for those infants who displayed positive physiological synchrony with their mothers during the initial face-to-face episode, but only among infants with less developed autonomic nervous systems (Abney, DaSilva, & Bertenthal, Reference Abney, DaSilva and Bertenthal2021). Mothers also dynamically alter their behaviors, including their gaze, facial expressions, vocalizations, and movements, to match those of their infants (e.g., Chong, Werker, Russell, & Carroll, Reference Chong, Werker, Russell and Carroll2003; MacLean et al., Reference MacLean, Rynes, Aragon, Caprihan, Phillips and Lowe2014). Moreover, high values on a composite measure of maternal prosody are associated with a reduction of infant negative affect following the still face, with larger reductions observed among infants with lower capacity for self-regulation (Kolacz, DaSilva, Lewis, Bertenthal, & Porges, Reference Kolacz, DaSilva, Lewis, Bertenthal and Porges2022). These results are important because they demonstrate that biobehavioral synchrony changes as a function of the characteristics of the dyad as well as infants' developmental status.

The fearful ape hypothesis focuses too narrowly on fear as a trigger of cooperative care without sufficient acknowledgement of the many other affective factors that contribute to effective parenting. Over time, repeated interactions with responsive caregivers provide predictability and safety for the infant, which in turn fosters the development of secure attachment with caregivers (Gee & Cohodes, Reference Gee and Cohodes2021). Importantly, good parenting develops both through responding to infants' distress, but also through responding to other emotions, including positive emotions such as joy (Leerkes, Blankson, & O'Brien, Reference Leerkes, Blankson and O'Brien2009; Lohaus, Keller, Ball, Elben, & Voelker, Reference Lohaus, Keller, Ball, Elben and Voelker2001) and interest (Hammond & Drummond, Reference Hammond and Drummond2019). The sharing of positive experiences is foundational to healthy caregiver–child interactions (e.g., Brown & Fredrickson, Reference Brown and Fredrickson2021; Ramsey & Gentzler, Reference Ramsey and Gentzler2015). By focusing exclusively on fear, Grossmann overlooks how maternal responsiveness to other emotions, and early, recurring experiences with parental co-regulation more broadly, lead to positive socioemotional development and cooperative care.

Second, the fearful ape hypothesis presents fearfulness as a state of helplessness that the caregiver must respond to without considering bidirectional pathways through which the infant has the agency to dynamically influence the interaction. In three well-known paradigms for infant research – still face, strange situation, and visual cliff – infants are faced with a challenge that violates their expectations. Yet, it is not the case that all infants exhibit negative affect and wait for their caregivers to soothe them. Rather, many infants actively solicit responses from their caregivers or look for cues signaling how to behave. For example, some infants as young as 2–3 months will attempt to re-engage their still-faced mother through social bids – looking at her while smiling or vocalizing (Bigelow & Power, Reference Bigelow and Power2016). Infants will also use social referencing by attending to adults' facial expressions to gauge how to respond to their uncertain situation, such as whether to traverse the deep side of the visual cliff (Sorce, Emde, Campos, & Klinnert, Reference Sorce, Emde, Campos and Klinnert1985), or what to do when presented with an unfamiliar toy (Klinnert, Emde, Butterfield, & Campos, Reference Klinnert, Emde, Butterfield and Campos1986). Over the course of the first year, infant–caregiver interactions become more interactive and bidirectional, such that infants initiate and actively elicit responses from their caregivers just as caregivers are responding to their infants (Beebe et al., Reference Beebe, Messinger, Bahrick, Margolis, Buck and Chen2016; Chow, Haltigan, & Messinger, Reference Chow, Haltigan and Messinger2010; Tronick & Gianino, Reference Tronick and Gianino1986). These changes in communicative pathways are further influenced by infants' temperament because differences in reactivity interact with maternal sensitivity and responsiveness to the child's needs. Notably, infants with negative temperaments receive less responsive parenting, including reduced parental co-regulation, compared to their less fussy peers (Bridgett et al., Reference Bridgett, Gartstein, Putnam, McKay, Iddins, Robertson and Rittmueller2009; Mills-Koonce et al., Reference Mills-Koonce, Gariépy, Propper, Sutton, Calkins, Moore and Cox2007). In sum, infants' own behaviors influence and elicit responses from their caregivers, suggesting that cooperative care involves more than the caregivers' response to the child.

In conclusion, the relation between the emergence of fear and cooperative care is more complex and multifaceted than implied by the fearful ape hypothesis. The reviewed evidence supporting how fear contributes to cooperative care is incomplete because it neglects the emergence of cooperative care in the broader context of infant–parent affect regulation, particularly early experiences with caregivers co-regulating both negative and positive emotions. Children's fearful responses may be adaptive in eliciting cooperative care, but not all the time and not for all individuals. This hypothesis overlooks many of the complex interactions between emotion, temperament, and biobehavioral synchrony that shape good parenting and cooperative care. As we discussed, cooperative care emerges from co-regulation, and as such it is a co-created experience that depends jointly on the caregiver as well as the child.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Competing interest

None.

References

Abney, D. H., DaSilva, E. B., & Bertenthal, B. I. (2021). Associations between infant–mother physiological synchrony and 4- to 6-month-old infants’ emotion regulation. Developmental Psychobiology, 63(6), e22161. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.22161CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beebe, B., Messinger, D., Bahrick, L. E., Margolis, A., Buck, K. A., & Chen, H. (2016). A systems view of mother–infant face-to-face communication. Developmental Psychology, 52(4), 556571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bigelow, A. E., & Power, M. (2016). Effect of maternal responsiveness on young infants’ social bidding-like behavior during the still face task. Infant and Child Development, 25(3), 256276. https://doi.org/10.1002/icdCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bridgett, D. J., Gartstein, M. A., Putnam, S. P., McKay, T., Iddins, E., Robertson, C., … Rittmueller, A. (2009). Maternal and contextual influences and the effect of temperament development during infancy on parenting in toddlerhood. Infant Behavior and Development, 32(1), 103116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.10.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, C. L., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2021). Characteristics and consequences of co-experienced positive affect: Understanding the origins of social skills, social bonds, and caring, healthy communities. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 39, 5863. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.02.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chong, S. C. F., Werker, J. F., Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (2003). Three facial expressions mothers direct to their infants. Infant and Child Development, 12, 211232. https://doi.org/10.1002/icdCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chow, S.-M., Haltigan, J. D., & Messinger, D. S. (2010). Dynamic infant–parent affect coupling during the face-to-face/still-face. Emotion, 10(1), 101114. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017824CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, R. (2007). Parent–infant synchrony: Biological foundations and developmental outcomes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 340345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C., & Yirmiya, N. (1999). Mother–infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 223231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gee, D. G., & Cohodes, E. M. (2021). Influences of caregiving on development: A sensitive period for biological embedding of predictability and safety cues. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 30(5), 376383. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637214211015673CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammond, S. I., & Drummond, J. K. (2019). Rethinking emotions in the context of infants’ prosocial behavior: The role of interest and positive emotions. Developmental Psychology, 55(9), 18821888. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000685CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones-Mason, K., Alkon, A., Coccia, M., & Bush, N. R. (2018). Autonomic nervous system functioning assessed during the still-face paradigm: A meta-analysis and systematic review of methods, approach and findings. Developmental Review, 50(May), 113139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.06.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klinnert, M. D., Emde, R. N., Butterfield, P., & Campos, J. J. (1986). Social referencing: The infant's use of emotional signals from a friendly adult with mother present. Developmental Psychology, 22(4), 427432. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.22.4.427CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolacz, J., DaSilva, E. B., Lewis, G. F., Bertenthal, B. I., & Porges, S. W. (2022). Associations between acoustic features of maternal speech and infants’ emotion regulation following a social stressor. Infancy, 27(1), 135158. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12440CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leerkes, E. M., Blankson, A. N., & O'Brien, M. (2009). Differential effects of maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress on social-emotional functioning. Child Development, 80(3), 762775. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01296.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lohaus, A., Keller, H., Ball, J., Elben, C., & Voelker, S. (2001). Maternal sensitivity: Components and relations to warmth and contingency. Parenting: Science and Practice, 1(4), 267284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacLean, P. C., Rynes, K. N., Aragon, C., Caprihan, A., Phillips, J. P., & Lowe, J. R. (2014). Mother–infant mutual eye gaze supports emotion regulation in infancy during the still-face paradigm. Infant Behavior and Development, 37(4), 512522. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.06.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mills-Koonce, W. R., Gariépy, J.-L., Propper, C., Sutton, K., Calkins, S., Moore, G., & Cox, M. (2007). Infant and parent factors associated with early maternal sensitivity: A caregiver-attachment systems approach. Infant Behavior and Development, 30(1), 114126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.11.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramsey, M. A., & Gentzler, A. L. (2015). An upward spiral: Bidirectional associations between positive affect and positive aspects of close relationships across the life span. Developmental Review, 36, 58104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2015.01.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J. J., & Klinnert, M. D. (1985). Maternal emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 21(1), 195200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tronick, E., & Beeghly, M. (2011). Infants’ meaning-making and the development of mental health problems. American Psychologist, 66(2), 107119. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021631.InfantsCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tronick, E. Z., & Gianino, A. (1986). Interactive mismatch and repair: Challenges to the coping infant. Zero to Three, 6(3), 16.Google Scholar
Tronick, E. Z., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., & Brazelton, T. B. (1978). The infant's response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17(1), 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed