Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-9q27g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-22T05:48:20.489Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Further Evidence That Repeated Checking Leads to Reduced Memory Confidence, Vividness and Detail: New Evidence That Repeated Object Exposure Also Results in Memory Distrust

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 August 2013

Meredith Medway
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
Mairwen K. Jones*
Affiliation:
Discipline of Behavioural and Social Sciences in Health, Faculty of Health Sciences, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
*
Address for correspondence: Dr Mairwen Jones, Discipline of Behavioural and Social Sciences in Health, Faculty of Health Sciences, The University of Sydney, PO Box 170, Lidcombe NSW 182Australia. Email: mairwen.jones@sydney.edu.au
Get access

Abstract

Researchers have suggested that distrust in one's memory is both a cause and a consequence of repeated checking. We investigated whether reduced clarity and confidence occurs to an equal degree with repeated object use and repeated checking. In addition, whether decreased memory confidence persists after a delay in checking or use was examined. Participants (N = 113) either repeatedly checked or repeatedly used a virtual stove or a light bulb stimulus (the control stimulus). Significant declines in memory accuracy, confidence, vividness and detail were observed for the experimental compared to the control stimulus. No significant differences in these effects between the checking and exposure conditions were found. A significant increase in state anxiety across pre-, mid- and post-test was found for both conditions. These findings provide further support for the notion that repeated checking can be self-perpetuating due to its impact on memory processes. The findings also suggest that checking is not necessary for these effects to occur as repeated use without checking also results in significant declines in memory accuracy, confidence, vividness and detail. Theoretical and clinical implications are discussed.

Type
Standard Papers
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Australian Academic Press Pty Ltd 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abramowitz, J.S., & Nelson, C.A. (2007). Treating doubting and checking concerns. In Antony, M.M., Purdon, C. & Summerfeldt, L.J. (Eds.), Psychological treatment of obsessive compulsive disorder: Fundamentals and beyond (pp. 169186). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Beech, H.R. (Ed.). (1974). Obsessional states. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Boschen, M.J., & Vuksanovic, D. (2007). Deteriorating memory confidence, responsibility perceptions and repeated checking: Comparisons in OCD and control samples. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 20982109. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.03.009Google Scholar
Boschen, M.J., Wilson, K.L., & Farrell, L.J. (2011). Attenuating memory distrust in a repeated checking task. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49 (8), 466471. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2011.05.001Google Scholar
Coles, M.E., Radomsky, A.S., & Horng, B. (2006). Exploring the boundaries of memory distrust from repeated checking: Increasing external validity and examining thresholds. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 9951006. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.08.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Constans, J.I., Foa, E.B., Franklin, M.E., & Mathews, A. (1995). Memory for actual and imagined events in OC checkers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 665671. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(94)00095-2Google Scholar
Cougle, J.R., Salkovskis, P.M., & Wahl, K. (2007). Perception of memory ability and confidence in recollections in obsessive-compulsive checking. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21 (1), 118130. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.03.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doctorow, E.L. (1976). Ragtime. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Ecker, W., & Engelkamp, J. (1995). Memory for actions in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 349371. doi:10.1017/S1352465800016477Google Scholar
Exner, C., Martin, V., & Rief, W. (2009). Self-focused ruminations and memory deficits in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 33, 163174. doi:10.1007/s10608-007-9162-xGoogle Scholar
Foa, E.B., Huppert, J.D., Leiberg, S., Langner, R., Kichic, R., Hajcak, G., & Salkovskis, P.M. (2002). The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory: Development and validation of a short version. Psychological Assessment, 14, 485496. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/pas/index.aspxGoogle Scholar
Hajcak, G., Huppert, J.D., Simons, R.F., & Foa, E.B. (2004). Psychometric properties of the OCI-R in a college sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42 (1), 115123. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.002Google Scholar
Hermans, D., Engelen, U., Grouwels, L., Joos, E., Lemmens, J., & Pieters, G. (2008). Cognitive confidence in obsessive–compulsive disorder: Distrusting perception, attention and memory. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 98113. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.11.001Google Scholar
Hermans, D., Martens, K., De Cort, K., Pieters, G., & Eelen, P. (2003). Reality monitoring and metacognitive beliefs related to cognitive confidence in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 383401. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00015-3Google Scholar
Hoehn-Saric, R., McLeod, D.R., & Hipsley, P. (1995). Is hyperarousal essential to obsessive–compulsive disorder? Diminished physiologic flexibility, but not hyperarousal, characterizes patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 688693. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950200078017Google Scholar
Jaafari, N., Frasca, M., Rigalleau, F., Rachid, F., Gil, R., Olié, J.P., . . . Vibert, N. (2011). Forgetting what you have checked: A link between working memory impairment and checking behaviors in obsessive-compulsive disorder. European Psychiatry. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2011.07.001Google Scholar
Jennings, H., Nedeljkovic, M., & Moulding, R. (2011). The influence of confidence in memory on checking behaviours. E-Journal of Applied Psychology, 7 (2), 916. Retrieved from http://ojs.lib.swin.edu.au/index.php/ejapGoogle Scholar
Kim, D., Bae, H., & Park, Y.C. (2008). Validity of the Subjective Units of Disturbance Scale in EMDR. Journal of EMDR Practice and Research, 2 (1), 5762. doi:10.1891/1933-3196.2.1.57Google Scholar
Lindsay, M., Crino, R., & Andrews, G. (1997). Controlled trial of exposure and response prevention in obsessive–compulsive disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 171 (2), 135139. doi:10.1192/bjp.171.2.135Google Scholar
Macdonald, P.A., Antony, M.M., Macleod, C.M., & Richter, M.A. (1997). Memory and confidence in memory judgments among individuals with obsessive compulsive disorder and non-clinical controls. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 497505. doi:10.1016/s0005-7967(97)00013-2Google Scholar
Mengestu, D. (2008). Children of the revolution. London: Vintage.Google Scholar
Nedeljkovic, M., & Kyrios, M. (2007). Confidence in memory and other cognitive processes in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 28992914. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.08.001Google Scholar
Nedeljkovic, M., Moulding, R., Kyrios, M., & Doron, G. (2009). The relationship of cognitive confidence to OCD symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 463468. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.10.001Google Scholar
Rachman, S. (1976). Obsessional-compulsive checking. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 14, 269277. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(76)90002-4Google Scholar
Rachman, S. (2002). A cognitive theory of compulsive checking. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 625639. doi:10.1016/s0005-7967(01)00028-6Google Scholar
Rachman, S. (2003). Compulsive checking. In P. Silva, De & Menzies, R.G. (Eds.), Obsessive compulsive disorder: Theory, research and treatment (pp. 139162). New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Radomsky, A.S., & Alcolado, G.M. (2010). Don't even think about checking: Mental checking causes memory distrust. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41, 345351. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.03.005Google Scholar
Radomsky, A.S., Gilchrist, P., & Dussault, D. (2006). Repeated checking really does cause memory distrust. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 305316. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.02.005Google Scholar
Radomsky, A.S., Rachman, S., & Hammond, D. (2001). Memory bias, confidence and responsibility in compulsive checking. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 813822. doi:10.1016/s0005-7967(00)00079-6Google Scholar
Radomsky, A.S., Shafran, R., Coughtrey, A.E., & Rachman, S. (2010). Cognitive-behavior therapy for compulsive checking in OCD. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 17, 119131. doi:10.1016/j.cbpra.2009.10.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, M.E., & Wilson, M.S. (2008). Factor structure and response bias of the Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory-Revised (OCI-R) in a female undergraduate sample from New Zealand. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 37 (2), 27. Retrieved from http://www.psychology.org.nz/NZ_JournalGoogle Scholar
Roper, G., Rachman, S., & Hodgson, R. (1973). An experiment on obsessional checking. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 11 (3), 271277. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(73)90003-xGoogle Scholar
Ruscio, A.M., Stein, D.J., Chiu, W.T., & Kessler, R.C. (2010). The epidemiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Molecular Psychiatry, 15, 5363. doi:10.1038/mp.2008.94Google Scholar
Sher, K.J., Frost, R.O., Kushner, M., Crews, T.M., & Alexander, J.E. (1989). Memory deficits in compulsive checkers: Replication and extension in a clinical sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27 (1), 6569. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(89)90121-6Google Scholar
Spielberger, C. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Tallis, F. (1993). Doubt reduction using distinctive stimuli as a treatment for compulsive checking: An exploratory investigation. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 1, 4552. doi:10.1002/cpp.5640010107Google Scholar
Tallis, F., Pratt, P., & Jamani, N. (1999). Obsessive compulsive disorder, checking, and non-verbal memory: A neuropsychological investigation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 161166. doi:10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00075-8Google Scholar
Tekcan, A.İ., Topçuoğlu, V., & Kaya, B. (2007). Memory and metamemory for semantic information in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 21642172. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.10.002Google Scholar
Tolin, D.F., Abramowitz, J.S., Brigidi, B.D., Amir, N., Street, G.P., & Foa, E.B. (2001). Memory and memory confidence in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 913927. doi:10.1016/s0005-7967(00)00064-4Google Scholar
Vaccaro, L.D., Jones, M.K., Menzies, R.G., & Clare, T.S. (2010). DIRT [Danger Ideation Reduction Therapy] for obsessive compulsive checkers: A comprehensive guide to treatment. Brisbane, Australia: Australian Academic Press.Google Scholar
van den Hout, M.A., & Kindt, M. (2003a). Phenomenological validity of an OCD-memory model and the remember/know distinction. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 369378. doi:10.1016/s0005-7967(02)00097-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Hout, M.A., & Kindt, M. (2003b). Repeated checking causes memory distrust. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 301316. doi:10.1016/s0005-7967(02)00012-8Google Scholar
van den Hout, M.A., & Kindt, M. (2004). Obsessive–compulsive disorder and the paradoxical effects of perseverative behaviour on experienced uncertainty. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 35, 165181. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2004.04.007Google Scholar