Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-vt8vv Total loading time: 0.001 Render date: 2024-08-11T14:17:46.893Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Role of Predisposition to Hallucinations on Non-Clinical Paranoid vs. Socially Anxious Individuals after Hearing Negative Affective-Laden Sounds: An Experimental Investigation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 September 2012

Bárbara Lopes*
Affiliation:
University of Coimbra, Portugal
José Pinto-Gouveia
Affiliation:
University of Coimbra, Portugal
*
Reprint requests to Bárbara Lopes, Faculdade de Psicologia e de Ciências da Educação, Universidade de Coimbra, Rua do Colégio Novo, Apartado 6153, P-3001-0082, Coimbra, Portugal. E-mail: barbaracslopes@gmail.com

Abstract

Background: Research suggested that negative affective-laden sounds act as environmental stressors that elicit negative affect (Bradley and Lang, 2000a). Aims: We tried to test for the role of an interaction between predisposition to hallucinatory experiences and exposure to negative affective laden sounds for the presence of paranoid ideation. Method: We used an experimental design that followed the vulnerability × stress model. We defined three groups from a sample of students: paranoia group vs. social anxiety group vs. control group. Their psychological characteristics were measured through self-reports of paranoia, anxiety, predisposition to hallucinations and depressive symptoms at Time 1 (before the experiment). Participants had to listen to either negative affective laden sounds (e.g. screaming) or positive affective laden sounds (e.g. sound of ocean waves). Their paranoid ideation and positive vs. negative emotional reactions to sounds were measured through self-reports at Time 2 (after the experiment). Results: Data showed that the paranoia group presented more serious psychological vulnerabilities than the social anxiety group. A MANCOVA also showed that the independent variables (“group” and “experimental sound conditions”) had statistically significant main effects on general paranoia ideation at Time 2. Furthermore, there was a significant three-way interaction between group x predisposition to hallucinatory experiences × experimental condition of sounds for the presence of general paranoid ideation at Time 2. Limitations included the small sample size and the effects of parasite variables, e.g. noise. Conclusions: Individuals’ predisposition for hallucinatory experiences increases the probability of possessing paranoid ideation. This tendency is a characteristic of paranoid non-clinical individuals.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, P., Freeman, D., McGuire, P., Garety, P., Kuipers, E., Fowler, D., et al. (2005). The prediction of hallucinatory predisposition in non-clinical individuals: examining the contribution of emotion and reasoning. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 127132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bentall, R. P. and Slade, P. D. (1985). Reality testing and auditory hallucinations: a signal detection analysis. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 3, 159169.Google Scholar
Bradley, M. M. and Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behaviour Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 4959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, M. M. and Lang, P. J. (1999a). Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): stimuli, instruction manual and affective ratings (Tech. Rep. N° C-1). Gainesville: FL: University of Florida.Google Scholar
Bradley, M. M. and Lang, P. J. (1999b). International Affective Digitized Sounds (IADS): stimuli, instruction manual and affective ratings (Tech. Rep. No. B-2). Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.Google Scholar
Bradley, M. M. and Lang, P. J. (2000a). Affective reactions to acoustic stimuli. Psychophysiology, 37, 204215.Google Scholar
Bradley, M. M. and Lang, P. J. (2000b). Measuring emotion: behavior, feeling, and physiology. In Lane, R. D. and Nadel, L. (Eds.), Cognitive Neuroscience of Emotion (pp. 242276). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Claridge, G. (1972). The schizophrenias as nervous types. British Journal of Psychiatry, 121, 117.Google Scholar
Combs, D. R., Penn, D. L. and Fenigstein, A. (2002). Ethnic differences in sub-clinical paranoia: an expansion of norms of the paranoia scale. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 8, 248256.Google Scholar
Combs, D. R. and Penn, D. L. (2004). The role of sub-clinical paranoia on social perception and behavior. Schizophrenia Research, 69, 93104.Google Scholar
Combs, D. R., Michael, C. O. and Penn, D. L. (2006). Paranoia and emotion perception across the continuum. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45, 1931.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (1997). Dissecting the computational architecture of social inference mechanisms. In Characterizing Human Psychological Adaptations (Ciba Symposium Volume #208). Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Ellet, L., Lopes, B. and Chadwick, P. (2003). Paranoia in a non-clinical population of college students. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 191, 425430.Google Scholar
Fenigstein, A. (1997). Paranoid thought and schematic processing. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 16, 7794.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenigstein, A. and Vanable, P. A. (1992). Paranoia and self-consciousness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 129138.Google Scholar
Freeman, D. (2007). Suspicious minds: the psychology of persecutory delusions. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 425457.Google Scholar
Freeman, D. and Freeman, J. (2008). Paranoia: the 21st century fear. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Freeman, D. and Garety, P. A. (1999). Worry, worry processes and dimensions of delusions: an exploratory investigation of a role for anxiety processes in the maintenance of delusional distress. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 27, 4762.Google Scholar
Freeman, D. and Garety, P. (2000). Comments on the content of persecutory delusions: does the definition need clarification? British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39, 407414.Google Scholar
Freeman, D. and Garety, P. A. (2003). Connecting neurosis and psychosis: the direct influence of emotion on delusions and hallucinations. Behavior Research and Therapy, 41, 923947.Google Scholar
Freeman, D., Garety, P. A., Bebbington, P.E., Smith, B., Rollinson, R., Fowler, D., et al. (2005). Psychological investigation of the structure of paranoia in a non-clinical population. British Journal of Psychiatry, 186, 427435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, D., Garety, P. A. and Kuipers, E. (2001). Persecutory delusions: developing the understanding of belief maintenance and emotional distress. Psychological Medicine, 31, 12931306.Google Scholar
Freeman, D., Gittins, M., Pugh, K., Antley, A., Slater, M. and Dunn, G. (2008). What makes one person paranoid and another person anxious? The differential prediction of social anxiety and persecutory ideation in an experimental situation. Psychological Medicine, 38, 11211132.Google Scholar
Freeman, D., Pugh, K., Antley, A., Slater, M., Bebbington, P., Gittins, M., et al. (2008). Virtual reality study of paranoid thinking in the general population. British Journal of Psychiatry, 192, 258263.Google Scholar
Gilbert, P. (1989). Human Nature and Suffering. London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gilbert, P. (2001a). Evolutionary approaches to psychopathology: the role of natural defences. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 35, 1727.Google Scholar
Gilbert, P. (2001b). Evolution and social anxiety: the role of attraction, social competition and social hierarchies. In Schneier, F.R. (Ed.), Social Anxiety Disorder: the psychiatric clinics of North America (Vol. 24, pp. 723751). Philadelphia, PA: Saunders.Google Scholar
Gilbert, P. (2002). Evolutionary approaches to psychopathology and cognitive therapy. In Gilbert, P. (Ed.), Cognitive Psychotherapy: an international quarterly (Evolutionary psychology and cognitive therapy special edition), 16, 236264.Google Scholar
Gilbert, P., Boxall, M., Cheung, M. and Irons, C. (2005). The relation of paranoid ideation and social anxiety in a mixed population. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 12, 124133.Google Scholar
Heponiemi, T., Ravaja, N., Elovainio, M. and Keltikangas-Järvinen, L. (2007). Relationship between hostility, affective ratings of pictures and state affects during task-inducing stress. Journal of Psychology, 141, 183201.Google Scholar
Johns, L. C. and van Os, J. (2001). The continuity of psychotic experiences in the general population. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 11251141.Google Scholar
Juslin, P. N. and Väjfäll, D. (2008). Emotional responses to music: the need to consider underlying mechanisms. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 31, 559621.Google Scholar
Khalfa, S., Bella, S. D., Roy, M., Peretz, I. and Lupien, S. J. (2003). Effects of relaxing music on salivary cortisol level after psychological stress. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 999, 374376 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knight, W. E. and Richard, N. S. (2001). Relaxing music prevents stress-induced increases in subjective anxiety, systolic blood pressure, and heart rate in healthy males and females. Journal of Music Therapy, 38, 254272.Google Scholar
Launay, G. and Slade, P. D. (1981). The measurement of hallucinatory predisposition in male and female prisoners. Personality and Individual Differences, 2, 221234.Google Scholar
Lincoln, M. T., Peter, N., Schäfer, M. and Moritz, S. (2008). Impact of stress on paranoia: an experimental investigation of moderators and mediators. Psychological Medicine, 39, 11291139.Google Scholar
Lincoln, M. T., Lange, J., Burau, J., Exner, C. and Moritz, S. (2010). The effect of state anxiety on paranoid ideation and jumping to conclusions: an experimental investigation. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36, 11401148.Google Scholar
Lopes, B. and Pinto-Gouveia, J. (in press). A relação da paranóia com o afecto negativo em duas amostras não-clínicas da amostra Portuguesa. Psychologica. Google Scholar
Lopes, B., Pinto-Gouveia, J. and Martins, S. (in press). Estudo da Adaptação Portuguesa da “General Paranoia Scale” (GPS) de Fenigstein e Vanable (1992) para duas amostras Portuguesas (estudantes e população geral). Psychologica. Google Scholar
Lovibond, P. and Lovibond, S. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety lnventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 335343.Google Scholar
Martin, J. A. and Penn, D. L. (2001). Social cognition and sub-clinical paranoid ideation. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40, 261265.Google Scholar
Morrison, A. P., Wells, A. and Nothard, S. (2000). Cognitive factors in the predisposition to auditory and visual hallucinations. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39, 6778.Google Scholar
Pais-Ribeiro, J., Honrado, A. and Leal, I. (2004). Contribuição para o estudo da adaptação portuguesa das escalas de depressão ansiedade stress de Lovibond e Lovibond. Psychologica, 36, 235246.Google Scholar
Pinto-Gouveia, J., Cunha, M. and Salvador, M. C. (2003). Assessment of social phobia by self-report questionnaires: the Social Interaction and Performance Anxiety and Avoidance Scale and the Social Phobia Safety Behaviours Scale. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 31, 291311.Google Scholar
Pinto-Gouveia, J., Fonseca, L., Robalo, M., Allen, A., Matos, A. and Gil, E. (1986). Ansiedade social: utilização dos questionários de auto-resposta SAD, FNE, e SISST numa população portuguesa. Psiquiatria Clínica, 7, 4348.Google Scholar
Ponciano, E. (2003). Adaptação das Escalas de Ansiedade Estado vs. Traço e de Raiva-Estado vs. Traço e Expressão numa população Portuguesa. Tese de Doutoramento da Universidade de Coimbra.Google Scholar
Redondo, J., Fraga, I., Padrón, I. and Piñeiro, A. (2008). Affective ratings of sound stimuli. Behaviour Research Methods, 40, 784790.Google Scholar
Salomon, E., Kim, M., Beaulieu, J. and Stefano, B. J. (2003). Sound therapy induced relaxation: down regulating stress processes and pathologies. Medical Science Monitor, 9, 96101.Google Scholar
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R. and Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Startup, H., Freeman, D. and Garety, P. A. (2007). Persecutory delusions and catastrophic worry in psychosis: developing the understanding of delusion distress and persistence. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 523537.Google Scholar
Stemmler, G., Heldmann, M., Pauls, C. and Scherer, T. (2001). Constraints of emotion specificity in fear and anger: the context counts. Psychophysiology, 38, 275291.Google Scholar
Stevenson, R. A. and James, T. W. (2008). Affective auditory stimuli: categorization of the International Affective Digitized Sounds (IADS) by discrete emotional categories. Behaviour Research Methods, 40, 315321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valmaggia, L. R., Freeman, D., Green, C., Garety, P., Swapp, D., Antley, A., et al. (2007). Virtual reality and paranoid ideations in people with an “at risk mental state” for psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 191 (suppl 51), s638.Google Scholar
Watson, D. and Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 448457.Google Scholar
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.