Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-pfhbr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-10T01:27:26.350Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Representing words in a second language: Can the L2 dance on its own?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 September 2021

Judith F. Kroll*
Affiliation:
School of Education, University of California, Irvine
Nicole Vargas Fuentes
Affiliation:
School of Education, University of California, Irvine
Julio Torres
Affiliation:
Department of Spanish and Portuguese, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697 USA
*
Address for correspondence: Judith F. Kroll, School of Education, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697, Judith.kroll@uci.edu

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Peer Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ayçiçeği, A and Harris, C (2004) BRIEF REPORT Bilinguals’ recall and recognition of emotion words. Cognition and Emotion 18, 977987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baus, C, Costa, A and Carreiras, M (2013) On the effects of second language immersion on first language production. Acta Psychologica 142, 402409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bice, K and Kroll, JF (2015) Native language change during early stages of second language learning. NeuroReport 26, 966971.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bordag, D, Gor, K and Opitz, A (2021) Ontogenesis model of the L2 lexical representation. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728921000250CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colbeck, KL and Bowers, JS (2012) Blinded by taboo words in L1 but not L2. Emotion 12(2), 217222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T and Van Heuven, WJ (2002) The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 5, 175197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, JF and Stewart, E (1994) Category interference in translation and picture naming: evidence for asymmetric connections between bilinguals memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language 33, 149174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, JF and Tokowicz, N (2005) Models of bilingual representation and processing. In Kroll, JF and De Groot, AMB (eds), Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 531553.Google Scholar
Lagrou, E, Hartsuiker, RJ and Duyck, W (2011) Knowledge of a second language influences auditory word recognition in the native language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 37, 952965.Google ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, J, Osterhout, L and Kim, A (2004) Neural correlates of second-language word learning: minimal instruction produces rapid change. Nature Neuroscience 7, 703704.10.1038/nn1264CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Misra, M, Guo, T, Bobb, SC and Kroll, JF (2012) When bilinguals choose a single word to speak: Electrophysiological evidence for inhibition of the native language. Journal of Memory and Language 67, 224237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montrul, S (2016) The acquisition of heritage languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Perani, D and Abutalebi, J (2005) The neural basis of first and second language processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 15, 202206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutton, TM, Altarriba, J, Gianico, JL and Basnight-Brown, DM (2007) The automatic access of emotion: Emotional Stroop effects in Spanish–English bilingual speakers. Cognition and Emotion 21, 10771090.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torres, J and Vargas Fuentes, N (2021) Heritage language learners’ lexical performance across pair types and (non-)digital collaborative writing task environments. Languages 6, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar