Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-tsvsl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-04T14:53:23.805Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Coercion without Lexical Decomposition: Type-Shifting Effects Revisited

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 June 2016

Roberto G. de Almeida
Affiliation:
Concordia University
Veena D. Dwivedi
Affiliation:
Brock University

Abstract

Recently, there has been a surge of psycholinguistic research on the interpretation of type-shifting constructions. We evaluate some of the theoretical and empirical research in this area and propose a reassessment of the nature of type-shifting operations.

We argue, in particular, for a structural gap in type-shifting constructions, motivated mainly by the restructuring nature of the verbs employed. We also argue that the putative underspecification (or indeterminacy) of type-shifting constructions is resolved via pragmatic inferences.

Résumé

Résumé

Ces dernières années, la recherche en psycholinguiste a témoigné d’un d’intérêt grandissant pour l’interprétation des constructions qui impliquent un changement de type. Nous évaluons ici les recherches théoriques et empiriques dans ce domaine et nous proposons une réévaluation de la nature de ces opérations.

Nous proposons en particulier une position vide dans ces constructions et nous faisons appel au processus de restructuration de certains verbes pour rendre compte de leur utilisation dans ces constructions. De plus, nous proposons que la soi-disante sous-spécification (ou l’indétermination) de ces constructions est résolue par des inférences pragmatiques.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Canadian Linguistic Association/Association canadienne de linguistique 2008 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aissen, Judith, and Perlmutter, David. 1976. Clause reduction in Spanish. In Proceedings of the Second Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (BLS 2), ed. Thompson, Henry, Edge, Vicki, Jaeger, Jeri, Javkin, Ronya, Petruck, Miriam, Smeall, Christopher, and Van Valin, Robert D. Jr., 1–30. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Asher, Nicholas, and Pustejovsky, James. 2000. The metaphysics of words in context. Ms., University of Texas, Austin.Google Scholar
Carston, Robyn. 2002. Thoughts and utterances. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Chierchia, Gennaro, and McConnell-Ginet, Sally. 1990. Meaning and grammar: An introduction to semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam 1995. The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
de Almeida, Roberto G. 1999a. The representation of lexical concepts: A psycholinguistic inquiry. Doctoral dissertation, Rutgers University.Google Scholar
de Almeida, Roberto G. 1999b. What do category-specific semantic deficits tell us about the representation of lexical concepts? Brain and Language 68:241–248.Google Scholar
de Almeida, Roberto G. 2004. The effect of context on the processing of type-shifting verbs. Brain and Language 90:249–261.Google Scholar
Dretske, F. 1981. Knowledge and the flow of information. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Evers, A. 1975. The transformational cycle of Dutch and German. Doctoral dissertation, University of Utrecht, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
Fodor, Jerry A. 1975. The language of thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, Jerry A. 1990. A theory of content and other essays. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, Jerry A., and Lepore, Ernest. 2002. The emptiness of the lexicon: Reflections on James Pustejovsky’s The Generative Lexicon. In The compositionality papers, ed. Fodor, Jerry A. and Lepore, Ernest, 89–119. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Freed, Alice F. 1979. The semantics of English aspectual complementation. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Frazier, Lyn, and Fodor, Janet D.. 1978. The sausage machine: A new two-stage parsing model. Cognition 6:291–325.Google Scholar
Gillon, Brendan S. 1992. Towards a common semantics for English count and mass nouns. Linguistics and Philosophy 15:597–640.Google Scholar
Gillon, Brendan S. 2004. Ambiguity, indeterminacy, deixis, and vagueness: evidence and theory. In Semantics: A reader, ed. Davis, S. and Gillon, Brendan S., 157–187. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Grimshaw, Jane 1990. Argument structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Grimshaw, Jane 2005. Semantic structure and semantic content in lexical representation. In Words and structure, ed. Grimshaw, Jane, 75–89. Stanford, CA: CSL1 Publications. [1993.]Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray 1997. The architecture of the Language Faculty. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kermes, K.A., and Kemper, S.. 1999. Aging and resolution of quantifier scope effects. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences 54:350–360.Google Scholar
Kuhn, Thomas 1962. The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Larson, Richard. 1988. On the double-object construction. Linguistic Inquiry 19:335–391.Google Scholar
McElree, Brian, Traxler, Matthew J., Pickering, Martin J., Seely, R.E., and Jackendoff, Ray. 2001. Reading time evidence for enriched composition. Cognition 78(1):B17B25.Google Scholar
Murphy, Greg L. 2002. The big book of concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Newmeyer, Frederick J. 1969. English aspectual verbs. Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois.Google Scholar
Partee, Barbara H. 1984. Compositionality. In Varieties of formal semantics, ed. Landman, F. and Veltman, F., 281–311. Dordretch: Foris.Google Scholar
Partee, Barbara H. 1987. Noun-phrase interpretation and type-shifting principles. In Studies in discourse representation theory and the theory of generalized quantifiers, ed. Groenendijk, J., de Jongh, D., and Stokhof, Martin, 115–143. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Partee, Barbara H., and Boroschev, V.. 1999. Possessives, favorite, and coercion. In Proceedings of ESCOL99, ed. Riehl, A. and Daly, R., 173–190. Ithaca, NY: CLC Publications, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Partee, Barbara H., and Rooth, Mats. 1983. Generalized conjunction and type ambiguity. In Meaning, use, and interpretation of language, ed. Schwarze, Bauerle, and Stechow, von, 361–383. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Pickering, Martin J., McElree, Brian, and Traxler, Matthew J.. 2005. The difficulty of coercion: A response to de Almeida. Brain and Language 93:1–9.Google Scholar
Pustejovsky, James 1995. The generative lexicon. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pylkkänen, Liina, and McElree, Brian. 2006. The syntax-semantics interface: On-line composition of sentence meaning. In Handbook of psycholinguistics, 2nd ed., ed. Traxler, Matthew and Gernsbacher, M.A., 537–577. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Rizzi, Luigi. 1978. A restructuring rule in Italian syntax. In Recent transformational studies in European languages, ed. Keyser, Samuel Jay, 113–158. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rochette, Anne. 1999. The selection properties of aspectual verbs. In Beyond principles and parameters, ed. Johnson, Kyle and Roberts, Ian, 145–165. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Traxler, Matthew J., Pickering, Martin J., and McElree, Brian. 2002. Coercion in sentence processing: Evidence from eye-movements and self-paced reading. Journal of Memory and Language 47:530–547.Google Scholar
Vinka, Mikael 1999. Predicative and non-predicative verb partiel constructions. In Proceedings of WCCFL 18, ed. Bird, S. and Carnie, A., 570–585. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
von Stechow, Arnim 1996. The different readings of Wieder ‘again’: A structural account. Journal of Semantics 13:87–138.Google Scholar
Wurmbrand, Susi. 2001. Infinitives: Restructuring and clause structure. Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
Wurmbrand, Susi. 2004. Two types of restructuring — Lexical vs. functional. Lingua 114: 991–1014.Google Scholar