Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-fmk2r Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-15T15:29:42.193Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Age-Related Differences in a Kinematic Analysis of Precued Movements

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 November 2010

Noreen L. Goggin
Affiliation:
The University of North Texas
George E. Stelmach
Affiliation:
Arizona State University

Abstract

An experiment is reported that examines kinematic measures, reaction time and response preparation differences between young and older adults, and movement response parameters in a precueing paradigm. Ten young and ten older adults performed 5 cm or 10 cm horizontal movements to the left or right with an electronic pen on a digitizing tablet. The precue (advance information) provided information about direction, extent, or both parameters. Dependent measures of reaction time (RT), movement time (MT), peak velocity, peak acceleration, peak deceleration, and their respective time values were analysed. The results indicate that young adults were significantly different on all dependent measures. Advance precue information was advantageous for all subjects as RT was faster when advance information was provided. In addition, both groups of subjects produced higher peak velocities when provided advance information about both direction and extent. Examination of the kinematics indicate that older adults produce movement patterns which display different response characteristics such as accelerating less rapidly and displaying a prolonged deceleration phase. Finally, older adults are less efficient than young adults at scaling velocity and acceleration over time between long and short movements when spatial accuracy constraints are minimal.

Résumé

RÉSUMÉ

Cet article rend compte d'une étude qui a examiné les différences entre adultes, jeunes et vieux, au niveau des mesures kinétiques, des réactions de temps (RT) et de la préparation à répondre ainsi que les paramètres de réponse de mouvement dans un cadre où les participants ont reçu des pré-indications. Dix jeunes adultes et dix plus âgés ont effectué des mouvements horizontaux mesurant 5 cm ou 10 cm vers la gauche ou vers la droite à l'aide d'une plume électronique sur une tablette digitalisée. Les pré-indications (renseignements reçus au préalable) portaient sur la direction, la longueur, ou bien les deux paramètres. Les différentes mesures dépendantes, dont le temps de réaction (RT), le temps de mouvement (MT), la vitesse maximum, l'accélération maximum, la délcélération maximum, et leurs valeurs de temps respectives ont été analysées. Selon les résultats, les jeunes ont réagi avec une différence marquée, et ce à tous les niveaux. Tous ont bénéficié des pré-indications puisque le RT était plus rapide lorsque des pré-indications étaient fournies. De plus, les deux groupes de sujets ont réussi à atteindre une meilleure vitesse maximale lorsqu'ils disposaient de pré-indications sur la direction et la longueur. Une étude kinématique démontre que les adultes âgés effectuent des mouvements qui dénotent des caractéristiques de réponse différentes, par exemple une accélération moins rapide et une phase de décélération prolongée. Enfin, les adultes âgés sont moins adeptes que les plus jeunes lorsqu'il s'agit d'ajuster pendant une période de temps la vélocité et l'accélération entre les mouvements longs et courts lorsque les contraintes de l'exactitude spatiale sont minimales.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Canadian Association on Gerontology 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

1

Department of Kinesiology, Health Promotion, and Recreation, The University of North Texas, Denton, TX 76203

*

This work was supported by grant number AG05154-01 from the U.S. Public Health Service awarded to George E. Stelmach. The research was completed while the first author was at The University of Wisconsin.

References

Atkeson, C.G., & Hollerbach, J.M. (1985). Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements. The Journal of Neuroscience, 5, 23182330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corcos, D.M., Gottlieb, G.L., & Agarwal, G.C. (1988). Accuracy constraints upon rapid elbow movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 20, 255272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freund, H.J., & Budingen, H.J. (1978). The relationship between speed and amplitude of the fastest voluntary contractions of human arm muscles. Experimental Brain Research, 31, 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gielen, C.C.A.M., van den Oosten, K., & Pull ter Gunne, F. (1985). Relation between EMG activation patterns and kinematic properties of aimed arm movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 17, 421442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottsdanker, R. (1980a). Aging and the maintaining of preparation. Experimental Aging Research, 6, 1327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottsdanker, R. (1980b). Aging and the use of advance probability information. Journal of Motor Behavior, 12, 133143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larish, D.D., & Stelmach, G.E. (1982). Preprogramming, programming, and reprogramming of aimed hand movements as a function of age. Journal of Motor Behavior, 14, 322340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marteniuk, R.G., MacKenzie, C.L., Jeannerod, M., Athenes, S., & Dugas, C. (1987). Constraints on human armmovement trajectories. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41, 365378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, D.E., Smith, J.E.K., & Wright, C.E. (1982). Models for the speed and accuracy of aimed movements. Psychological Review, 89, 449482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murrell, K.F., & Entwisle, D.G. (1960). Age differences in movement pattern. Nature, 185, 948949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rabbitt, P.M. (1979). How old and young subjects monitor and control responses for accuracy and speed. British Journal of Psychology, 70, 305311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rabbitt, P.M. (1982). Breakdown of control processes in old age. In Field, T.M., Huston, A., Quay, H.C., Troll, L., & Finley, G. (Eds.), Review of human development (pp. 540550). New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T.A. (1985). Speed of behavior and its implications for cognition. In Birren, J.E. & Schaie, K.W. (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (pp. 400426). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar
Sheridan, M.R. (1984). Response programming, response production, and fractionated reaction time. Psychological Research, 46, 3347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stelmach, G.E., Goggin, N.L., & Amrhein, P.C. (1988). Aging and the restructuring of precued movements. Psychology and Aging, 3, 151157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stelmach, G.E., Goggin, N.L., & Garcia-Colera, A. (1987). Movement specification time with age. Experimental Aging Research, 13, 3946.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warabi, T., Noda, H., & Kato, T. (1986). Effect of aging on sensorimotor functions of eye and hand movements. Experimental Neurology, 92, 686697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Welford, A.T. (1984a). Between bodily changes and performance: some possible reasons for slowing with age. Experimental Aging Research, 13, 7388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welford, A.T. (1984b). Psychomotor performance. In Eisdorfer, C. (Ed.), Annual review of Gerontology and Geriatrics (pp. 237273). New York: Springer Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Welford, A.T., Norris, A.H., & Shock, N.W. (1969). Speed and accuracy of movement and their changes with age. Ada Psychologica, 30, 315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed