Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-tn8tq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-29T11:33:56.233Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Identifying structural brain markers of resilience to adversity in young people using voxel-based morphometry

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 July 2023

Harriet Cornwell
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath, UK
Nicola Toschi
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedicine and Prevention, University of Rome “Tor Vergata”, Rome, Italy Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging and Harvard Medical School, Boston, USA
Catherine Hamilton-Giachritsis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath, UK
Marlene Staginnus
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath, UK
Areti Smaragdi
Affiliation:
Child Development Institute, Toronto, Canada
Karen Gonzalez-Madruga
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Middlesex University, London, UK
Jack Rogers
Affiliation:
Centre for Human Brain Health, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK
Anne Martinelli
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Frankfurt, Goethe University, Frankfurt am Main, Germany Fresenius University of Applied Sciences, School of Psychology, Frankfurt, Germany
Gregor Kohls
Affiliation:
Child Neuropsychology Section, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University Hospital, RWTH Aachen, Aachen, Germany Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Nora Maria Raschle
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University of Basel, Psychiatric University Hospital, Basel, Switzerland Jacobs Center for Productive Youth Development at the University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
Kerstin Konrad
Affiliation:
Child Neuropsychology Section, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University Hospital, RWTH Aachen, Aachen, Germany JARA-Brain Institute II, Molecular Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, RWTH Aachen and Research Centre Juelich, Juelich, Germany
Christina Stadler
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University of Basel, Psychiatric University Hospital, Basel, Switzerland
Christine Freitag
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Frankfurt, Goethe University, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
Stephane De Brito
Affiliation:
Centre for Human Brain Health, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK
Graeme Fairchild*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath, UK
*
Corresponding author: G. Fairchild; Email: gf353@bath.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

There is increasing evidence that resilience in youth may have a neurobiological basis. However, the existing literature lacks a consistent way of operationalizing resilience, often relying on arbitrary judgments or narrow definitions (e.g., not developing PTSD) to classify individuals as resilient. Therefore, this study used data-driven, continuous resilience scores based on adversity and psychopathology to investigate associations between resilience and brain structure in youth. Structural MRI data from 298 youth aged 9–18 years (Mage = 13.51; 51% female) who participated in the European multisite FemNAT-CD study were preprocessed using SPM12 and analyzed using voxel-based morphometry. Resilience scores were derived by regressing data on adversity exposure against current/lifetime psychopathology and quantifying each individual’s distance from the regression line. General linear models tested for associations between resilience and gray matter volume (GMV) and examined whether associations between resilience and GMV differed by sex. Resilience was positively correlated with GMV in the right inferior frontal and medial frontal gyri. Sex-by-resilience interactions were observed in the middle temporal and middle frontal gyri. These findings demonstrate that resilience in youth is associated with volume in brain regions implicated in executive functioning, emotion regulation, and attention. Our results also provide evidence for sex differences in the neurobiology of resilience.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Epidemiological surveys conducted across 21 countries estimate that more than one third of the population have been exposed to at least one adverse childhood experience such as maltreatment or parental loss (Green et al., Reference Green, McLaughlin, Berglund, Gruber, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Kessler2010; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, McLaughlin, Green, Gruber, Sampson, Zaslavsky, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alhamzawi, Alonso and Angermeyer2010). Although there are well-documented associations between childhood adversity and poor mental health, many young people who experience adversity do not go on to develop mental health problems (Collishaw et al., Reference Collishaw, Pickles, Messer, Rutter, Shearer and Maughan2007). In fact, many young people show resilience which, broadly speaking, means that they remain free of or recover quickly from mental health problems following exposure to adversity (Kalisch et al., Reference Kalisch, Baker, Basten, Boks, Bonanno, Brummelman, Chmitorz, Fernandez, Fiebach and Galatzer-Levy2017, Reference Kalisch, Köber, Binder, Ahrens, Basten, Chmitorz, Choi, Fiebach, Goldbach and Neumann2021). There have been calls for resilience research to take a multilevel approach to elucidate mechanisms that give rise to resilience versus vulnerability following adversity exposure (Southwick et al., Reference Southwick, Bonanno, Masten, Panter-Brick and Yehuda2014). It is well established that adolescence is a period characterized not only by increased stress exposure but also greater neural plasticity (Leal & Silvers, Reference Leal and Silvers2021). Therefore, it is critical that we understand the neurobiological processes of resilience operating in this developmental phase, as this research could inform resilience-promoting interventions and prevention programs that reduce the negative impact of childhood adversity across the life span (Gee, Reference Gee2021).

To our knowledge, just six neuroimaging studies have investigated the structural brain basis of resilience in young people to date, and these studies were based on just four independent data sets (see Eaton et al., Reference Eaton, Cornwell, Hamilton-Giachritsis and Fairchild2022 for a review). Definitions of resilience varied markedly between studies, making it difficult to compare them. For example, one study did not explicitly define resilience but instead investigated whether psychopathology mediated the relationship between childhood maltreatment and brain development (Whittle et al., Reference Whittle, Dennison, Vijayakumar, Simmons, Yücel, Lubman, Pantelis and Allen2013). The authors found that young people who remained resilient during the follow-up period (by virtue of remaining free of psychopathology despite maltreatment) showed accelerated hippocampal, but slower amygdala, growth. Furthermore, three studies from the Healthy Childhood Brain Development Program defined resilience as the absence of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) following childhood maltreatment. All three studies compared an overlapping sample of maltreated youth with PTSD, maltreated youth without PTSD (i.e., resilient youth), and non-maltreated controls. The first two studies found that resilience was associated with larger cerebral and cerebellar gray matter volumes (GMV; De Bellis et al., Reference De Bellis, Hooper, Chen, Provenzale, Boyd, Glessner, MacFall, Payne, Rybczynski and Woolley2015) and greater left amygdala and right hippocampal volumes (Morey et al., Reference Morey, Haswell, Hooper and De Bellis2016). The third study examined structural networks in the brain and found that the resilient group exhibited greater centrality in the left anterior cingulate and right frontal pole than the PTSD or control groups (Sun et al., Reference Sun, Haswell, Morey and De Bellis2018). Higher centrality is thought to indicate that a cortical region is of high importance within its respective network.

Another study defined resilience as being low in mood and anxiety disorder symptoms despite exposure to traumatic stress and found that resilient youth showed smaller total brain volumes than those classified as susceptible (stress-exposed youth with elevated anxiety/depressive symptoms; Barzilay et al., Reference Barzilay, Rosen, Moore, Roalf, Satterthwaite, Calkins, Ruparel, Patrick, Scott and Wolf2020). Similar effects were observed in the basal ganglia and parietal cortex (resilient youth showed lower volumes in these regions). Finally, research from the European multisite IMAGEN study found that resilient adolescents (defined as being high in “competence” despite adversity) had increased GMV in the right middle frontal and right superior frontal gyri compared to individuals low in adversity exposure and those high in adversity exposure but low in competence (Burt et al., Reference Burt, Whelan, Conrod, Banaschewski, Barker, Bokde, Bromberg, Büchel, Fauth-Bühler and Flor2016). Competence was defined based on being low in internalizing symptoms, but also showing intact or superior academic and social functioning. Overall, these studies suggest that youth resilience is related to greater GMV in the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala, although existing findings are mixed or even contradictory. These brain regions are implicated in executive functions and emotion processing and regulation.

Although it is important to understand the structural brain basis of resilience in young people, resilience research is characterized by several limitations and conceptual challenges. The overarching issue is that different researchers and studies have defined, measured, and operationalized resilience in different ways (Kalisch et al., Reference Kalisch, Baker, Basten, Boks, Bonanno, Brummelman, Chmitorz, Fernandez, Fiebach and Galatzer-Levy2017). Firstly, many studies rely on narrow definitions of resilience, such as not developing PTSD following trauma exposure or childhood maltreatment. However, individuals may not reach formal thresholds for a diagnosis of PTSD but may have developed other forms of psychopathology and/or functional impairment. As it is well established that adversity increases risk for multiple classes of psychopathology, rather than just PTSD or internalizing symptoms specifically (Bauer et al., Reference Bauer, Fairchild, Hammerton, Murray, Santos, Rodrigues, Munhoz, Barros, Matijasevich and Halligan2022; Green et al., Reference Green, McLaughlin, Berglund, Gruber, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Kessler2010; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, McLaughlin, Green, Gruber, Sampson, Zaslavsky, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alhamzawi, Alonso and Angermeyer2010), it is important to take a broad perspective in measuring resilient outcomes, capturing many different forms of psychopathology rather than just one categorical disorder or class of symptoms.

Secondly, in studies that have taken a broader approach to resilience, different cutoffs are used to define who in a sample is deemed resilient versus vulnerable, which renders comparisons between studies challenging. For example, two separate studies based on the IMAGEN cohort used different cutoffs for significant adversity exposure (four versus six negative life events) and assessed these in relation to different outcomes (low risk of mental disorders versus low levels of internalizing symptoms and competence across multiple life domains) to classify participants as resilient (Burt et al., Reference Burt, Whelan, Conrod, Banaschewski, Barker, Bokde, Bromberg, Büchel, Fauth-Bühler and Flor2016; Galinowski et al., Reference Galinowski, Miranda, Lemaitre, Paillère, Artiges, Vulser, Goodman, Penttilä, Struve and Barbot2015). As a consequence of the challenges arising from the use of different definitions and ways of operationalizing resilience, it has been suggested that researchers should move away from using categorical measures of resilience and instead use quantitative, continuous measures (Kalisch et al., Reference Kalisch, Müller and Tüscher2015).

Therefore, in the broader resilience field, researchers have begun to operationalize resilience by regressing different types of adversity exposure (e.g., bullying, socioeconomic disadvantage) against outcomes of interest (e.g., emotional difficulties, psychopathological symptoms) and calculating how much each individual deviates from the expected positive relationship between adversity and psychopathology (e.g., Bowes et al., Reference Bowes, Maughan, Caspi, Moffitt and Arseneault2010; Miller-Lewis et al., Reference Miller-Lewis, Searle, Sawyer, Baghurst and Hedley2013; van Harmelen et al., Reference van Harmelen, Kievit, Ioannidis, Neufeld, Jones, Bullmore, Dolan, Fonagy and Goodyer2017). This “residuals” approach allows researchers to derive data-driven, dimensional resilience scores for each participant in their sample and allows even those with diagnosable disorders to be considered resilient; for example, if an individual has had experienced very high levels of adversity, yet only just meets the threshold for a psychiatric diagnosis. However, to our knowledge, this approach is yet to be applied when studying the neurobiology of resilience. Therefore, in the present study, we generated continuous resilience scores for the participants in our sample and, for the first time, used them to perform a structural MRI data analysis.

There have also been calls to investigate whether there are sex differences in the neurobiological basis of resilience (Moreno-López et al., Reference Moreno-López, Ioannidis, Askelund, Smith, Schueler and van Harmelen2020). It has long been recognized that many forms of childhood psychopathology are more prevalent in one sex than the other (Eme, Reference Eme1979). For example, early-onset neurodevelopmental conditions (e.g., autism spectrum conditions and externalizing disorders) are more prevalent in males, whereas adolescent-onset mood and anxiety disorders are more prevalent in females (Martel, Reference Martel2013; Rutter et al., Reference Rutter, Caspi and Moffitt2003). Furthermore, previous research has identified sex differences in the neurobiological basis of psychiatric disorders (e.g., Helpman et al., Reference Helpman, Zhu, Suarez-Jimenez, Lazarov, Monk and Neria2017; Smaragdi et al., Reference Smaragdi, Cornwell, Toschi, Riccelli, Gonzalez-Madruga, Wells, Clanton, Baker, Rogers and Martin-Key2017). However, to our knowledge, no studies have investigated sex differences in the neurobiology of resilience in young people.

Thus, our primary aim was to derive continuous, multidimensional resilience scores using a data-driven approach that captures many different forms of adversity (from relatively normative to extreme) and multiple classes of psychopathology (both internalizing and externalizing, and current and lifetime symptoms). Our secondary aim was to examine whether these resilience scores were related to brain structure using voxel-based morphometry, an unbiased approach that tests for alterations in GMV across the entire brain. We also investigated whether the relationship between resilience and GMV differs by sex. As we were applying a novel approach to defining resilience to investigate relationships between resilience and brain structure, we initially ran exploratory, whole-brain analyses. We then conducted region-of-interest (ROI) analyses, defining the amygdala, hippocampus, anterior cingulate, and key prefrontal regions as ROIs based on a recent systematic review (Eaton et al., Reference Eaton, Cornwell, Hamilton-Giachritsis and Fairchild2022). We predicted that resilience scores would be positively correlated with GMV in the amygdala and hippocampus, middle frontal and superior frontal regions, and the orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate cortices and frontal pole. We did not have any predictions about sex differences in the relationship between resilience and GMV; this was an exploratory analysis, as no previous neuroimaging studies have tested for sex-by-resilience interactions.

Methods

The FemNAT-CD study

This study utilized secondary data from 298 participants who took part in the neuroimaging substudy of a large European multisite project (Freitag et al., Reference Freitag, Konrad, Stadler, De Brito, Popma, Herpertz, Herpertz-Dahlmann, Neumann, Kieser and Chiocchetti2018). The aim of the original FemNAT-CD study was to investigate sex differences in conduct disorder (CD) in children and adolescents aged 9–18 years. Therefore, over half of the full sample had a current diagnosis of CD as assessed using the Kiddie-Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia – Present and Lifetime version (K-SADS-PL; Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Rao, Flynn, Moreci, Williamson and Ryan1997), which in the majority of cases was completed with a parent/caregiver as well as the participant themselves. Participants with CD were also allowed to have other current or lifetime DSM-IV disorders such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The remaining participants were healthy controls (HCs) and were free of current DSM-IV disorders. A further exclusion criterion for this group was having a lifetime diagnosis of CD, oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) or ADHD, as assessed using the K-SADS-PL. However, it should be noted that the healthy controls could have subthreshold levels of psychopathology (e.g., mild symptoms of depression or anxiety). Exclusion criteria for both groups were IQ < 70, and the presence of genetic syndromes, autism spectrum conditions, bipolar disorder/mania, schizophrenia, and neurological disorders. Participants were recruited from five sites across three European countries (Table S1), with neuroimaging data, including structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data, collected within a single scanning session lasting around one hour.

Details regarding the recruitment strategy and clinical assessments, as well as inclusion and exclusion criteria, for the full FemNAT-CD study were reported in a previous paper (Konrad et al., Reference Konrad, Kohls, Baumann, Bernhard, Martinelli, Ackermann, Smaragdi, Gonzalez-Madruga, Wells, Rogers, Pauli, Clanton, Baker, Kersten, Prätzlich, Oldenhof, Jansen, Kleeven, Bigorra and Freitag2022). Briefly, participants were recruited from the community (e.g., schools) and clinical and forensic settings (e.g., mental health clinics and youth offending services). Once their eligibility to take part in the study was confirmed, participants took part in three separate data collection sessions. During these sessions, multilevel data (e.g., phenotypic, neuropsychological, neuroimaging, and genetic data) were collected from participants. Measures relevant to this substudy are described in more detail below.

Ethical approval

The current study was granted ethical approval by the University of Bath Psychology Research Ethics Committee (Ref: 18–322). All participants and their parents/caregivers gave written informed consent and/or informed assent to take part in the FemNAT-CD study, which was approved by the European Commission and local ethics committees at each site.

Participants

The current study included 298 young people aged 9–18 years (M age = 13.51, SD age = 2.57; 51% females; 39% of the 767 participants who had undergone a structural MRI scan). All of those included had usable structural MRI data and a resilience score. The remaining participants (61%) who had undergone a structural MRI scan were excluded from the current study due to either missing and/or incomplete data for many of the key exposure and psychopathology measures required to derive the resilience scores, poor-quality structural MRI data, or missing demographic data. A flowchart detailing the full sample selection process can be found in Figure S1. Table S2 shows the demographic and clinical characteristics of participants included (n = 298) versus excluded (n = 454) from the analysis. Participants included in the present analysis were significantly younger, had higher estimated IQs, had experienced fewer traumatic events, and had fewer current CD symptoms than the excluded participants.

As the FemNAT-CD study’s original aim was to investigate the neurobiology of CD, of the 298 participants included, 78 (26%) had a diagnosis of CD, while the other 220 (74%) were free of current DSM-IV disorders and past disruptive behavior disorders.

Measures

Demographic and clinical data were also collected as part of the FemNAT-CD study. Sex was self-reported; no information was collected regarding gender identity. IQ was estimated using the vocabulary and matrix reasoning subscales of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999) at the UK sites, or the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2003) at the other sites. Pubertal development was measured using the Pubertal Development Scale (PDS; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Crockett, Richards and Boxer1988). Supplement 1 describes the methods used to impute missing data on measures relevant to the current study.

Resilience scores

The resilience scores used in this study were calculated using the “residuals” approach (Miller-Lewis et al., Reference Miller-Lewis, Searle, Sawyer, Baghurst and Hedley2013). Briefly, we calculated our resilience scores using a range of self- and parent-report measures of exposure to adversity and traumatic experiences and psychopathology. We selected a subset of the most relevant adversity exposure and psychopathology variables, partly because data were missing for many participants due to parents not completing questionnaires or ethical constraints which meant that we were not allowed to collect data on maltreatment exposure from participants aged below 16 years (i.e., the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; (Bernstein et al., Reference Bernstein, Stein, Newcomb, Walker, Pogge, Ahluvalia, Stokes, Handelsman, Medrano, Desmond and Zule2003)) at the UK sites, or because there was considerable overlap between measures in terms of item content.

A total of 45 variables measuring exposure to adversity and traumatic experiences, such as childhood maltreatment, exposure to violent crimes, and dysfunctional relationships with parents, were retained. They were collected using a parent-report interview (the Children’s Bad Experiences interview; Arseneault et al., Reference Arseneault, Cannon, Fisher, Polanczyk, Moffitt and Caspi2011), a self-report questionnaire (the Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse; Bifulco et al., Reference Bifulco, Bernazzani, Moran and Jacobs2005), and the PTSD trauma screening subsection of the K-SADS-PL (completed by both informants). It should be noted that the variables measured the occurrence of adversity and traumatic experiences rather than their severity or perceived impact.

Additionally, a total of 130 psychopathology variables measuring current and lifetime psychopathology were retained. They were measured using the other sections of the K-SADS-PL and a parent-report questionnaire assessing dimensional psychopathology (the Child Behaviour Checklist; Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1991). These measures assessed a broad range of internalizing and externalizing symptoms covering depression and anxiety disorders and CD, ADHD, and ODD (among other disorders). Rather than using data on categorical diagnoses, symptom-level data from the K-SADS-PL were used. Furthermore, parent and child responses for all K-SADS-PL data were integrated to create a composite score for each K-SADS-PL variable such that the highest score was taken (i.e., a symptom was considered present if endorsed at threshold by either informant).

Additional steps were then taken to ensure that the data were suitable for conducting a factor analysis. For example, ensuring the data matrix was full rank, identifying which columns were linearly dependent and making sure the correlation matrix was positive definite. This meant ensuring that we did not have any columns that could have been derived from a combination of others (e.g., a composite score from item level data), as if this was the case, the matrix would be rank deficient (as opposed to full rank), and we may still have had missing data.

Next, two exploratory factor analyses based on principal axis factoring with direct oblimin rotation were run. An exploratory factor analysis was deemed more suitable than a confirmatory factor analysis as there were no a priori hypotheses regarding the latent dimensions that would be identified by this procedure (Field, Reference Field2013). We chose to use principal axis factoring as it is the most commonly used factor analysis approach in social and behavioral science research (Warner, Reference Warner2013). Given that childhood adversities (McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Green, Gruber, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Kessler2012), and different forms of psychopathology (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Chiu, Demler, Merikangas and Walters2005) often co-occur, we chose to use the direct oblimin rotation method which allows factors to be correlated (Field, Reference Field2013).

Based on Field’s (2013) recommendations, as our sample size exceeded 250, it was deemed appropriate to use Kaiser’s criterion and retain all factors with eigenvalues greater than 1. In order to ensure additional robustness in the number of factors selected, a parallel analysis (O’Connor, Reference O’Connor2000) was also run to determine the number of factors to retain (Goldberg & Velicer, Reference Goldberg, Velicer and Strack2006). Once the final set of exposure to adversity (Table S3) and psychopathology (Table S4) factors were extracted, factor scores were then calculated using the regression method (recommended by Tabachnick & Fidell, Reference Tabachnick and Fidell2007). To aid understanding and interpretation, some factors were inverted such that higher scores for all factors represented higher exposure to adversity/trauma and more severe levels of psychopathology.

To derive a resilience score for each participant, factor scores were weighted by variance explained, normalized individually using robust scaling between the 1st and 99th percentile and then aggregated (separately for exposure to adversity and psychopathology factors) using the median operator to reduce the influence of outliers. Six psychopathology factors which are thought to have a stronger genetic basis (and which therefore may be less under environmental influence, that is, those related to neurodevelopmental disorders and psychosis (Carroll & Owen, Reference Carroll and Owen2009)), were removed (Supplement 2). We then derived a resilience score for each participant by regressing their aggregated adversity exposure score against their aggregated psychopathology score and calculating the individual distance from the regression line along the psychopathology dimension. The positive relationship between exposure to adversity and psychopathology was fitted to linear, cubic, and quadratic models, comparing model appropriateness using Akaike’s information criterion (AIC; Bozdogan, Reference Bozdogan1987). The best model (i.e., lowest AIC) was linear (Figure S2). Participants who were lower than expected in psychopathology based on their level of adversity exposure had higher resilience scores, whereas participants who were higher in psychopathology than expected given their level of adversity exposure had lower resilience scores. Resilience scores could range from 1 to -1, with higher scores indicating higher resilience.

MRI data acquisition and preprocessing

Structural MRI data were obtained at each site using Siemens 3T (Tim-Trio and Prisma) or Philips 3T (Achieva) scanners. T1-weighted scans were collected using a magnetization-prepared-rapid-acquisition gradient-echo (MP-RAGE) sequence. Details regarding site qualification procedures and acquisition parameters are reported in Supplement 3 and Table S5. An MRI operator assessed the T1-weighted image quality after the scan and repeated scans as necessary.

Prior to preprocessing and analysis, we visually inspected the quality of all images in MRIcron (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/mricron/) following a published protocol (Supplement 4; (Backhausen et al., Reference Backhausen, Herting, Buse, Roessner, Smolka and Vetter2016). In total, 16 scans were excluded due to poor image quality or gross neuroanatomical abnormalities, leaving a final sample of 298 youth.

Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM12; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) was used to preprocess and analyze the data. We rechecked the quality and orientation of each T1-weighted scan in SPM12, and reoriented images where necessary to aid normalization. T1-weighted images were segmented into gray matter (GM), white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid and the resulting GM tissue class images were used to construct a study-specific DARTEL template. We then normalized the template to MNI space and warped and smoothed the modulated GM images using an isotropic Gaussian kernel of 6-mm full-width-at-half-maximum (Ashburner & Friston, Reference Ashburner and Friston2000). Finally, we used the proportional scaling method with total intracranial volume values to control for individual differences in global brain size.

VBM analysis

The final statistical analyses were performed using general linear models in SPM12. We first explored associations between resilience and regional GMV. For completeness, we also tested for sex differences in GMV. We also computed a sex-by-resilience interaction term by multiplying demeaned resilience scores by the dichotomous sex variable and included the resulting variable in the model to explore whether resilience–GMV relationships differed by sex. Diagnostic group (CD vs. healthy controls), sex, age, and scanner site were included as covariates of no interest (the latter using the “one-hot encoding” approach which involves creating a different regressor for each site; Hancock & Khoshgoftaar, Reference Hancock and Khoshgoftaar2020).

We used two approaches in our analysis: an exploratory whole-brain approach and a ROI approach. Given the exploratory nature of our study, in the former, we report findings at a threshold of p ≤ .001, with a cluster extent threshold of k = 41 voxels that was empirically determined according to random field theory (Hayasaka & Nichols, Reference Hayasaka and Nichols2004; Worsley et al., Reference Worsley, Marrett, Neelin, Vandal, Friston and Evans1996). For our ROI approach, we used the automated anatomical labeling atlas, version 3 (Rolls et al., Reference Rolls, Huang, Lin, Feng and Joliot2022). We analyzed the left and right hemispheres separately for all regions (the amygdala, hippocampus, anterior cingulate, middle frontal and superior frontal gyri, and orbitofrontal cortex) except the frontal pole. For our ROI analysis, only clusters that survived family-wise error (FWE) correction at p < .05 for multiple comparisons in small volumes (i.e., small-volume correction, SVC) are reported (Friston, Reference Friston1997). In our sensitivity analyses, we report findings at a threshold of p ≤ .001 and only report clusters that met the relevant cluster extent threshold. Finally, parameter estimates were exported from SPM12 and correlated (using a Spearman correlation) with resilience scores in IBM SPSS Statistics 26 to provide an estimate of the strength of the correlations (https://www.ibm.com/support/pages/ibm-spss-statistics-26-documentation), although all inferential statistics assessing for associations between resilience scores and GMV or sex-by-resilience interactions were performed in SPM12 using GLMs. It should be noted that these Spearman correlations were run using parameter estimates extracted from the local cluster maxima rather than an independently defined ROI image, which is not consistent with best practice in MRI data analysis as outlined in the COBIDAS report (Nichols et al., Reference Nichols, Das, Eickhoff, Evans, Glatard, Hanke, Kriegeskorte, Milham, Poldrack, Poline, Proal, Thirion, Van Essen, White and Yeo2017).

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics

Table 1 shows the demographic and clinical characteristics of participants included in the VBM analysis. The participants’ resilience scores ranged from –0.59 to 0.22 (where higher values indicate higher resilience). Figure S3 in the Supplement shows the distribution of resilience scores in females and males. Of the four groups included in the study, male healthy controls had the highest resilience scores, although there was substantial variance in resilience scores in all four groups. The number of current CD symptoms ranged from 0 to 11 (out of a possible 15), reflecting the inclusion of both healthy controls and participants with CD diagnoses. Comparisons between the CD and healthy control groups are reported in Table S6 for completeness.

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants included in the voxel-based morphometry analysis (N = 298)

Number of Traumatic Events and Number of Current CD Symptoms are measured by the Kiddie-Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia - Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL); CD = conduct disorder; HC = healthy controls; IQ = intelligent quotient; SD = standard deviation.

Factor analysis

Factor analysis assumptions were tested and are reported in Supplement 5. When comparing each eigenvalue from both factor analyses to the corresponding 95th percentile of the distribution for that eigenvalue generated by the parallel analysis, it was found that none of the factors from the adversity exposure or psychopathology factor analyses were random. However, a final decision was made to retain 11 factors from the adversity exposure factor analysis and 36 factors from the psychopathology factor analysis based on Kaiser’s criterion (although six psychopathology factors were removed prior to computing the resilience scores - see above for further details). All the factors retained made sense theoretically (i.e., they indexed normative and severe adversities or various common forms of psychopathology) and cumulatively explained > 60% of the total variance in each factor analysis. Based on recommendations from Osborne and colleagues (2011), only factor loadings > .3 were interpreted.

Whole-brain analyses

We then tested for associations between resilience scores and GMV using VBM, with diagnostic group, sex, age, and scanner site included as covariates of no interest. To reiterate, as there were significant sex differences in total intracranial volume (Females M TIV = 1.33, Males M TIV = 1.45, p < .001), we used the proportional scaling method to control for individual differences in global brain size.

Correlations between resilience and GMV

We observed positive correlations between resilience scores and GMV in the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and right medial frontal gyrus (Fig. 1), as well as the right superior parietal lobule, right postcentral, right precentral, and bilateral paracentral gyri (Table 2). No negative correlations between resilience scores and GMV were observed.

Figure 1. Positive correlations between resilience scores and gray matter volume in (a) the right inferior frontal gyrus (rs = .15) and (b) the right medial frontal gyrus (rs = .15) in the whole-brain analysis.

Table 2. Coordinates and cluster sizes for the correlations between resilience scores and gray matter volume, and sex-by-resilience score interactions in the whole-brain analysis (N = 298)

All correlations and sex-by resilience score interactions were significant at a threshold of p ≤ .001, with a cluster extent threshold of k = 41 voxels. BA = Brodmann’s area; MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute.

Main effects of sex

Main effects of sex were observed in several cortical and subcortical regions (Table S7). Males had higher GMV in the fusiform, inferior occipital and inferior temporal gyri compared to females, whereas females had higher caudate nucleus and hippocampal GMV.

Sex-by-resilience interactions

We identified three areas where the relationship between resilience scores and GMV differed significantly between male and female youth. Firstly, resilience scores were positively correlated with GMV in the left middle temporal gyrus in males, but negatively correlated with GMV in this area in females (Table 2 and Fig. 2a). In contrast, resilience scores were positively correlated with GMV in the right middle frontal gyrus (Fig. 2b) and bilateral cerebellar tonsil in females, but negatively correlated with GMV in these regions in males.

Figure 2. Sex-by-resilience score interactions for gray matter volume in (a) the left middle temporal gyrus (Females, rs = −.21; males, rs = .21) and (b) the right middle frontal gyrus (Females, rs = .21; males, rs = −.10) in the whole-brain analysis.

ROI analysis

We identified positive and negative correlations between resilience scores and GMV in several ROIs, although none survived FWE small-volume correction. In line with our whole-brain analyses, a sex-by-resilience interaction was detected in the right middle frontal gyrus (p < .05, FWE SVC), driven by a positive correlation between resilience and GMV in females, but a negative correlation in males.

Sensitivity analyses

To ensure that we were not overcontrolling for diagnostic group by including data related to psychopathology when deriving our resilience scores as well as including diagnostic group as a regressor in the VBM analysis, we ran a sensitivity analysis including just the healthy controls (n = 220). The results remained broadly similar in terms of showing positive associations between resilience and GMV (Table S8), although most of the significant clusters were now in the left hemisphere. There was also an additional positive correlation with resilience in the left precuneus, and a negative correlation with resilience in the left precentral gyrus. Further, in the sex-by-resilience interaction analysis, resilience scores were positively correlated with GMV in males, but negatively correlated with GMV in females, in the left angular gyrus, left insula, and right inferior parietal lobule. An additional interaction was observed in the left superior temporal gyrus (positive correlation between resilience scores and GMV in females, but a negative correlation in males).

Furthermore, to focus on a more developmentally homogenous subsample, we ran an additional sensitivity analysis where only those classified as mid-, late-, or postpubertal on the PDS (n = 225) were included. Again, the results were broadly similar to those obtained with the entire sample (Table S9). However, additional negative correlations between resilience scores and GMV in bilateral dorsal caudate nucleus (extending into the ventral caudate; Figure S4) and left pars orbitalis were observed in this case.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to use data-driven, continuous resilience scores to investigate the brain structural correlates of resilience in young people. We hypothesized that resilience would be positively associated with GMV in the orbitofrontal gyrus, middle and superior frontal regions, frontal pole, anterior cingulate, amygdala, and hippocampus. We found that, across the whole sample, higher levels of resilience were associated with greater GMV in frontal and parietal areas of the brain, including the inferior and medial frontal gyri, mainly in the right hemisphere. Furthermore, and contrary to our hypotheses, resilience was not associated with GMV in the frontal pole, anterior cingulate, amygdala or hippocampus.

We also examined whether the relationship between resilience and brain structure differed by sex. We identified three areas where this was the case: the left middle temporal and right middle frontal gyri, and bilateral cerebellar tonsil. Resilience was positively associated with GMV in the right middle frontal gyrus and cerebellar tonsil in females, but negatively associated with GMV in males. In contrast, resilience scores were positively correlated with GMV in the left middle temporal gyrus in males, whereas a correlation in the opposite direction was observed in females. Overall, our findings provide preliminary evidence for sex differences in the relationship between resilience and brain structure in young people and suggest that future studies should also test for sex-specific associations between resilience and brain structure.

Correlations between resilience and GMV

As mentioned above, higher levels of resilience were associated with greater GMV in the right IFG, an area implicated in executive functions, such as response inhibition and attentional control (Hampshire et al., Reference Hampshire, Chamberlain, Monti, Duncan and Owen2010), and emotion regulation (Dixon et al., Reference Dixon, Thiruchselvam, Todd and Christoff2017). Interestingly, Luby et al. (Reference Luby, Barch, Whalen, Tillman and Belden2017) found that early adversity was linked to reduced IFG volume in childhood, and that smaller IFG volumes mediated the association between early adversity and depression severity in adolescence. In a subsequent study, Barch et al. (Reference Barch, Belden, Tillman, Whalen and Luby2018) found that early adversity was related to connectivity between the IFG region observed in the previous study and several other brain regions (e.g., premotor cortex, posterior parietal cortex, and cerebellum). Further to this, IFG connectivity with these brain regions also predicted externalizing disorder symptom severity in childhood and early adolescence. Although previous structural MRI studies of resilience have not identified this region, a longitudinal functional MRI study found that young people who remained resilient during the follow-up period (by remaining free of psychopathology despite being at high familial risk for mood disorders) showed lower right IFG activation when processing fearful faces compared to individuals at high familial risk who did develop psychopathology (Nimarko et al., Reference Nimarko, Garrett, Carlson and Singh2019). Taken together, these findings suggest that the IFG may play an important role in resilience.

Our data also support the proposal that emotion regulation is a key psychological resilience factor (Kalisch et al., Reference Kalisch, Cramer, Binder, Fritz, Leertouwer, Lunansky, Meyer, Timmer, Veer and Van Harmelen2019), although it should be noted that the right IFG region we identified is also implicated in the coordination of attention to external stimuli. Hartwigsen et al. (Reference Hartwigsen, Neef, Camilleri, Margulies and Eickhoff2019) revealed five functional clusters within the right IFG; the functional cluster that corresponds to the location of our IFG cluster (“cluster 5”) is in a relatively posterior part of the IFG (y = 11; Brodmann’s Area 44) and interestingly is functionally connected to the other regions that we found were positively associated with resilience: the right superior parietal lobule, the right middle frontal gyrus and the right precentral gyrus. This cluster is part of the dorsal attention network, which is implicated in spatial attention, memory encoding, attentional control, and action execution, and therefore plays a key role in the coordination of attention to external stimuli. The other regions we identified, such as the right precentral and postcentral gyri and paracentral lobule, are implicated in motor and sensory functions (particularly hand movements). These findings could be interpreted as evidence that resilient youth do not show the decreases in volume in sensory and motor regions that are observed in maltreated individuals or those exposed to significant adversity (Teicher et al., Reference Teicher, Samson, Anderson and Ohashi2016). We also found that resilience was positively associated with GMV in the medial frontal gyrus and superior parietal lobule. The medial frontal gyrus is implicated in cognitive processes such as decision-making (Talati & Hirsch, Reference Talati and Hirsch2005), while the superior parietal lobule has been implicated in working memory and attention (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Yang, Fan, Xu, Li, Liu, Fox, Eickhoff, Yu and Jiang2015), as well as tactile perception (Stoeckel et al., Reference Stoeckel, Weder, Binkofski, Choi, Amunts, Pieperhoff, Shah and Seitz2004). Taken together, these findings suggest that higher GMV in regions implicated in decision-making and working memory is a structural marker of resilience in young people. However, as the superior parietal lobule finding was not predicted, this region should be explored in future studies using a hypothesis-driven approach.

Sex-by-resilience interactions

We also observed sex differences in the relationship between resilience and brain structure. Higher levels of resilience in male youth were associated with increased GMV in the left middle temporal gyrus, whereas female youth showed the opposite relationship. Relevant to this, Samplin et al. (Reference Samplin, Ikuta, Malhotra, Szeszko and DeRosse2013) reported sex differences in the effects of childhood emotional abuse on hippocampal volume: while abused males showed reduced hippocampal volume, there was a trend toward larger hippocampal volume in abused females versus non-abused females. It is possible that there are sex differences in vulnerability to adversity-related changes in middle temporal gyrus structure (volume is reduced in maltreated males but increased or no different in maltreated females). Coupled with the idea that resilient individuals might be resistant to adversity’s effects on the brain, this potentially gives rise to the sex-by-resilience interaction observed in this region. We recognize this explanation is speculative, but there is evidence for effects of maltreatment on middle temporal gyrus volume (De Brito et al., Reference De Brito, Viding, Sebastian, Kelly, Mechelli, Maris and McCrory2013; Jackowski et al., Reference Jackowski, Perera, Abdallah, Garrido, Tang, Martinez, Mathew, Gorman, Rosenblum and Smith2011), sex differences in the effects of maltreatment on volume in adjacent regions such as the hippocampus (Samplin et al., Reference Samplin, Ikuta, Malhotra, Szeszko and DeRosse2013), and the idea that resilience is associated with resistance to the effects of maltreatment on brain structure (De Bellis et al., Reference De Bellis, Hooper, Chen, Provenzale, Boyd, Glessner, MacFall, Payne, Rybczynski and Woolley2015).

Although the middle temporal gyrus has not been implicated in resilience specifically, a meta-analysis found that trauma-exposed adults with PTSD had reduced GMV in the temporal pole/middle temporal gyrus compared to trauma-exposed adults without PTSD (i.e., a group that could be considered resilient; Kühn & Gallinat, Reference Kühn and Gallinat2013). The left middle temporal gyrus is implicated in autobiographical memory (Holland et al., Reference Holland, Addis and Kensinger2011) and McCrory et al. (Reference McCrory, Puetz, Maguire, Mechelli, Palmer, Gerin, Kelly, Koutoufa and Viding2017) found that maltreated children showed abnormally increased middle temporal gyrus activation during an autobiographical memory test relative to non-maltreated controls. Abuse in childhood is related to autobiographical memory disturbances, such as over-general memory (Puetz et al., Reference Puetz, Viding, Hoffmann, Gerin, Sharp, Rankin, Maguire, Mechelli and McCrory2021; Valentino et al., Reference Valentino, Toth and Cicchetti2009). These findings suggest that the effects of maltreatment and adversity on this region may differ by sex, and resilience may be characterized by an absence of such maltreatment-related effects on middle temporal gyrus structure or function. Along these lines, it would be interesting to investigate whether resilient individuals are protected from the effects of childhood maltreatment on autobiographical memory specificity. However, as this region has not previously been linked to youth resilience, we recommend that it is included as a ROI in future studies.

In contrast, we found that resilience was positively correlated with GMV in the right middle frontal gyrus in females, whereas males showed a weak correlation in the opposite direction. The right middle frontal gyrus was also included as an a priori ROI and a sex-by-resilience interaction in the same direction in this region survived correction for multiple comparisons. The largest study to date with an adolescent sample also found that resilience was associated with greater GMV in the right middle frontal gyrus (Burt et al., Reference Burt, Whelan, Conrod, Banaschewski, Barker, Bokde, Bromberg, Büchel, Fauth-Bühler and Flor2016), although the authors did not test for sex differences. Previous research has found the middle frontal gyrus to be involved in attentional reorienting (Japee, Holiday, Satyshur, Mukai, & Ungerleider, Reference Japee, Holiday, Satyshur, Mukai and Ungerleider2015). Thus, it could be speculated that an important resilience mechanism for female youth is their ability to reorient their attention away from negative stimuli or adverse events toward other more positive aspects of their life. This fits with theory and research on resilience to chronic pain (Goubert & Trompetter, Reference Goubert and Trompetter2017).

Finally, we also observed a sex-by-resilience interaction in the cerebellar tonsil, such that resilience was positively associated with GMV in females, but negatively associated in males. Childhood maltreatment has been linked to reduced cerebellar GM volume (Edmiston et al., Reference Edmiston, Wang, Mazure, Guiney, Sinha, Mayes and Blumberg2011). In female adolescents specifically, there was an inverse relationship between childhood trauma and cerebellar volume, as well as other areas involved in emotion regulation. Thus, female youth who do not show trauma-related reductions in cerebellum volume (or even display compensatory increases in this region) may be protected from developing psychopathology as a result. Taken together, these findings suggest that greater cerebellar volume may be a neural marker of resilience in youth exposed to adversity, particularly in females.

The sex differences identified in the current study support the notion that females and males might become resilient partly via different pathways (Fallon et al., Reference Fallon, Tanner, Greenwood and Baratta2020). For example, based on our findings, it could be inferred that avoiding adversity-related impairments in autobiographical memory or dysfunction in the neural substrates of autobiographical memory is an important resilience factor for male youth, whereas an important resilience factor for female youth is their ability to reorient their attention away from adverse/traumatic experiences. Alternatively, it could be that sex differences in adversity exposure and/or risk for psychopathology are driving these differences. For example, depression (Breslau et al., Reference Breslau, Gilman, Stein, Ruder, Gmelin and Miller2017) and experiences of sexual abuse (Briere & Elliott, Reference Briere and Elliott2003) are both more common in female youth. Therefore, remaining free of depression is arguably stronger evidence for resilience in female youth than male youth. Overall, based on our exploratory findings, future research should investigate factors that might underpin sex differences in the relationship between resilience and brain structure.

Although not a major focus of the study, the fact that we observed sex differences in brain structure that are consistent with prior findings (i.e., total intracranial volume was ∼ 10% higher in males than females, and the amygdala was larger in males, while the caudate nucleus was larger in females, even after accounting for sex differences in total intracranial volume; Eliot et al., Reference Eliot, Ahmed, Khan and Patel2021) supports the validity of our VBM results. Interestingly, a recent meta-analytic review by Eliot et al. (Reference Eliot, Ahmed, Khan and Patel2021) speculated that increases in caudate nucleus volume in females compared to males might be confined to adolescents as adult-only studies have not reported sex differences in caudate volumes, that is, our study might have been better placed to identify a sex difference in the caudate nucleus than many others, due to the age of the sample (M age = 13.5 years) and the fact that girls typically go through puberty earlier than boys, which may cause increases in caudate volume.

Strengths and limitations

Overall, this study had several strengths. Firstly, our resilience scores were data-driven and continuous and were derived based on each participant’s exposure to multiple types of adversity, as well as their current and lifetime psychopathology. We also used information about maltreatment and trauma exposure and psychopathology from the participants themselves and their parents or caregivers. This meant that we took a much broader perspective of resilience than many previous studies and were capturing “transdiagnostic” resilience – although current externalizing disorders and past ODD symptoms were the most influential psychopathology factors of those studied, given the original study’s focus on CD. These two factors alone (out of 36) accounted for > 20% of the variance in the psychopathology factor analysis.

Secondly, although some of the MRI subsample had to be excluded from the VBM analysis because we were missing data on key adversity measures that fed into the resilience score calculations, our sample was relatively large for a neuroimaging study (N = 298). We also included similar numbers of male and female youth which allowed us to investigate sex differences in the relationship between resilience and brain structure. Furthermore, we controlled for several potentially important covariates in our analyses including diagnostic status, age, and scanner site – as well as individual differences in total intracranial volume. We also screened the structural MRI data carefully, using an established quality control pipeline (Backhausen et al., Reference Backhausen, Herting, Buse, Roessner, Smolka and Vetter2016), to ensure that only high-quality images were included. Finally, a limitation of some of the earlier studies in this field is that they have not corrected for multiple comparisons (Eaton et al., Reference Eaton, Cornwell, Hamilton-Giachritsis and Fairchild2022). Although in our whole-brain analysis we report findings at a threshold of p ≤ .001, we only reported clusters that exceeded an extent threshold that was empirically determined based on random field theory (Hayasaka & Nichols, Reference Hayasaka and Nichols2004; Worsley et al., Reference Worsley, Marrett, Neelin, Vandal, Friston and Evans1996).

Despite these strengths, the current study had several limitations. Firstly, different scanners were used to collect the structural MRI data, which may have introduced noise into the data. However, this issue should have been partly mitigated by the site qualification procedures that were used and the fact that we controlled for site in our analyses.

Secondly, participants included in the analysis were significantly younger and had higher estimated IQs, experienced fewer traumatic events, and had fewer current CD symptoms compared to those excluded from the analysis. Although these differences indicate that the present sample is not fully representative of the original FemNAT-CD study sample (which was focused on case–control comparisons), it could be argued that the current study sample is more representative of the general population given that participants included in the analysis had experienced significantly lower levels of adversity and had fewer CD symptoms than the excluded participants. Importantly for the purposes of this study, almost the full range of traumatic events (0–7) and CD symptoms (0–11) was observed in the present sample, which meant that it was suitable for addressing research questions related to resilience to adversity.

Additionally, we included participants across a wide age range. However, our sensitivity analysis focusing on a more developmentally homogenous group revealed broadly similar findings, but also negative correlations between resilience and caudate nucleus and pars orbitalis GMV. A systematic review found that sexual abuse and emotional neglect, but not physical or emotional abuse, were related to decreased caudate nucleus volumes (Cassiers et al., Reference Cassiers, Sabbe, Schmaal, Veltman, Penninx and Van Den Eede2018). Previous research also found that adolescents’ scores on the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire were negatively correlated with GMV in the striatum (Edmiston et al., Reference Edmiston, Wang, Mazure, Guiney, Sinha, Mayes and Blumberg2011). Furthermore, our data were cross-sectional rather than longitudinal. This means that we cannot tell whether the observed differences in brain structure reflect post-trauma adaptations in the brain (i.e., resilience effects) or whether they represent preexisting differences that may have buffered the effects of adversity exposure. Future research should adopt longitudinal designs to differentiate between preexisting differences versus post-trauma modifications in brain structure.

A further limitation was that the exposure to adversity and traumatic experiences variables used to derive the resilience scores assessed whether traumatic or negative events had occurred, rather than measuring the severity, or the perceived impact, of these experiences. There are several aspects of adversity and trauma exposure that can influence resilient outcomes, such as whether it is a one-off incident, whether the perpetrator was known to the victim, and whether the traumatic experience was deliberately intended or accidental (Bolger & Patterson, Reference Bolger, Patterson and Luthar2003; Diamond et al., Reference Diamond, Airdrie, Hiller, Fraser, Hiscox, Hamilton-Giachritsis and Halligan2022; Hyman & Williams, Reference Hyman and Williams2001; Marriott et al., Reference Marriott, Hamilton-Giachritsis and Harrop2014). Future research using the residuals approach could incorporate these additional details about participants’ exposure to adversity and traumatic experiences, but these data were not systematically collected in the original FemNAT-CD study.

Finally, in terms of our analysis method, a limitation of VBM is that it conflates different cortical properties (i.e., surface area, cortical thickness and folding) that have distinct developmental trajectories and etiologies into one composite measure, namely GMV (Panizzon et al., Reference Panizzon, Fennema-Notestine, Eyler, Jernigan, Prom-Wormley, Neale, Jacobson, Lyons, Grant and Franz2009). Therefore, future research could use surface-based morphometry to determine whether the global volumetric effects observed here are driven by specific cortical properties (e.g., cortical thickness).

Conclusion

Overall, this study suggests that higher levels of resilience in youth are associated with greater GMV in frontal and parietal regions. It also emphasizes the importance of taking sex into account when investigating the brain basis of resilience, as we observed sex differences in the relationship between resilience and brain structure. However, across both males and females, many of the brain areas found to be related to resilience in young people are involved in emotion regulation, executive functions, and attention, which supports the idea that these are key processes involved in resilience that should be targeted in treatment and preventative interventions (Greenberg, Reference Greenberg2006).

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000718.

Funding statement

This work was supported in part by grant MR/N0137941/1 to the GW4 BIOMED MRC DTP, awarded to the Universities of Bath, Bristol, Cardiff and Exeter from the Medical Research Council (MRC)/UKRI, and grant ES/P000630/1 for the SWDTP, awarded to the Universities of Bath, Bristol, Exeter, Plymouth and West of England (UWE) from the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC)/UKRI. N.M.R. has received grant support as a Jacobs Foundation Early Career Fellow (grant number 2016 1217 13). The FemNAT-CD study was funded by the European Commission’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013, grant number 602407; coordinator C.M. Freitag).

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the child behavior checklist/4-18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Arseneault, L., Cannon, M., Fisher, H. L., Polanczyk, G., Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (2011). Childhood trauma and children’s emerging psychotic symptoms: A genetically sensitive longitudinal cohort study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(1), 6572. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10040567 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (2000). Voxel-based morphometry—The methods. NeuroImage, 11(6 Pt 1), 805821. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0582 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Backhausen, L. L., Herting, M. M., Buse, J., Roessner, V., Smolka, M. N., & Vetter, N. C. (2016). Quality control of structural MRI images applied using freeSurfer—A hands-on workflow to rate motion artifacts. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 10. Article 558. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2016.00558 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barch, D. M., Belden, A. C., Tillman, R., Whalen, D., & Luby, J. L. (2018). Early childhood adverse experiences, inferior frontal gyrus connectivity, and the trajectory of externalizing psychopathology. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 57(3), 183190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.12.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barzilay, R., Rosen, A. F., Moore, T. M., Roalf, D. R., Satterthwaite, T. D., Calkins, M. E., Ruparel, K., Patrick, A., Scott, J. C., Wolf, D. H. (2020). Structural brain patterns associated with traumatic stress resilience and susceptibility to mood and anxiety symptoms in youths. Adversity and Resilience Science, 1(3), 112. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42844-020-00014-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, A., Fairchild, G., Hammerton, G., Murray, J., Santos, I. S., Rodrigues, L. T., Munhoz, T. N., Barros, A. J., Matijasevich, A., Halligan, S. L. (2022). Associations between childhood trauma and childhood psychiatric disorders in Brazil: A population-based, prospective birth cohort study. The Lancet Psychiatry, 9(12), 969977. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(22)00337-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernstein, D. P., Stein, J. A., Newcomb, M. D., Walker, E., Pogge, D., Ahluvalia, T., Stokes, J., Handelsman, L., Medrano, M., Desmond, D., & Zule, W (2003). Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27(2), 169190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bifulco, A., Bernazzani, O., Moran, P., & Jacobs, C. (2005). The childhood experience of care and abuse questionnaire (CECA. Q): Validation in a community series. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44(4), 563581. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466505X35344 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolger, K., & Patterson, C. (2003). Sequelae of child maltreatment: Vulnerability and resilience. In Luthar, S. (Eds.), Resilience and Vulnerability: Adaptation in the Context of Childhood Adversities (pp. 156181). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511615788.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowes, L., Maughan, B., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Arseneault, L. (2010). Families promote emotional and behavioural resilience to bullying: Evidence of an environmental effect. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(7), 809817. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02216.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bozdogan, H. (1987). Model selection and Akaike’s information criterion (AIC): The general theory and its analytical extensions. Psychometrika, 52(3), 345370. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02294361 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breslau, J., Gilman, S., Stein, B., Ruder, T., Gmelin, T., & Miller, E. (2017). Sex differences in recent first-onset depression in an epidemiological sample of adolescents. Translational Psychiatry, 7(5), e1139e1139. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2017.105 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briere, J., & Elliott, D. M. (2003). Prevalence and psychological sequelae of self-reported childhood physical and sexual abuse in a general population sample of men and women. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27(10), 12051222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2003.09.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burt, K. B., Whelan, R., Conrod, P. J., Banaschewski, T., Barker, G. J., Bokde, A. L., Bromberg, U., Büchel, C., Fauth-Bühler, M., Flor, H. (2016). Structural brain correlates of adolescent resilience. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(11), 12871296. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12552 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, L. S., & Owen, M. J. (2009). Genetic overlap between autism, schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Genome Medicine, 1(10), 102. https://doi.org/10.1186/gm102 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cassiers, L. L., Sabbe, B. G., Schmaal, L., Veltman, D. J., Penninx, B. W., & Van Den Eede, F. (2018). Structural and functional brain abnormalities associated with exposure to different childhood trauma subtypes: A systematic review of neuroimaging findings. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9. Article 329, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00329 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collishaw, S., Pickles, A., Messer, J., Rutter, M., Shearer, C., & Maughan, B. (2007). Resilience to adult psychopathology following childhood maltreatment: Evidence from a community sample. Child Abuse & Neglect, 31(3), 211229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.02.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Bellis, M. D., Hooper, S. R., Chen, S. D., Provenzale, J. M., Boyd, B. D., Glessner, C. E., MacFall, J. R., Payne, M. E., Rybczynski, R., Woolley, D. P. (2015). Posterior structural brain volumes differ in maltreated youth with and without chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 27(4pt2), 15551576. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415000942 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Brito, S. A., Viding, E., Sebastian, C. L., Kelly, P. A., Mechelli, A., Maris, H., & McCrory, E. J. (2013). Reduced orbitofrontal and temporal grey matter in a community sample of maltreated children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(1), 105112. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02597.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, P. R., Airdrie, J. N., Hiller, R., Fraser, A., Hiscox, L. V., Hamilton-Giachritsis, C., & Halligan, S. L. (2022). Change in prevalence of post-traumatic stress disorder in the two years following trauma: A meta-analytic study. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 13(1), 2066456. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2022.2066456 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixon, M. L., Thiruchselvam, R., Todd, R., & Christoff, K. (2017). Emotion and the prefrontal cortex: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 143(10), 10331081. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000096 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eaton, S., Cornwell, H., Hamilton-Giachritsis, C., & Fairchild, G. (2022). Resilience and young people’s brain structure, function and connectivity: A systematic review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 132, 936956. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.11.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edmiston, E. E., Wang, F., Mazure, C. M., Guiney, J., Sinha, R., Mayes, L. C., & Blumberg, H. P. (2011). Corticostriatal-limbic gray matter morphology in adolescents with self-reported exposure to childhood maltreatment. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 165(12), 10691077. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.565 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eliot, L., Ahmed, A., Khan, H., & Patel, J. (2021). Dump the “dimorphism”: Comprehensive synthesis of human brain studies reveals few male-female differences beyond size. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 125, 667697. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.02.026 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eme, R. F. (1979). Sex differences in childhood psychopathology: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 86(3), 574595. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.574 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fallon, I. P., Tanner, M. K., Greenwood, B. N., & Baratta, M. V. (2020). Sex differences in resilience: Experiential factors and their mechanisms. European Journal of Neuroscience, 52(1), 25302547. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.14639 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, A. (2013). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics (4th edn). Sage.Google Scholar
Freitag, C. M., Konrad, K., Stadler, C., De Brito, S. A., Popma, A., Herpertz, S. C., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Neumann, I., Kieser, M., Chiocchetti, A. G. (2018). Conduct disorder in adolescent females: Current state of research and study design of the femNAT-cD consortium. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27(9), 10771093. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-018-1172-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. J. (1997). Testing for anatomically specified regional effects. Human Brain Mapping, 5(2), 133136. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1097-0193(1997)5:2 3.0.CO;2-4>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galinowski, A., Miranda, R., Lemaitre, H., Paillère, M. M., Artiges, E., Vulser, H., Goodman, R., Penttilä, J., Struve, M., Barbot, A. (2015). Resilience and corpus callosum microstructure in adolescence. Psychological Medicine, 45(11), 22852294. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715000239 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gee, D. G. (2021). Early life trauma and resilience: Insights from developmental neuroscience for policy. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 6(2), 141143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.07.005 Google ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, L. R., & Velicer, W. F. (2006). Principles of exploratory factor analysis. In Strack, S. (Ed.): Differentiating normal and abnormal personality (2nd ed, pp. 209–237) Springer.Google Scholar
Goubert, L., & Trompetter, H. (2017). Towards a science and practice of resilience in the face of pain. European Journal of Pain, 21(8), 13011315. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejp.1062 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, J. G., McLaughlin, K. A., Berglund, P. A., Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., & Kessler, R. C. (2010). Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication I: Associations with first onset of DSM-iV disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(2), 113123. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.186 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenberg, M. T. (2006). Promoting resilience in children and youth: Preventive interventions and their interface with neuroscience. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094(1), 139150. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1376.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampshire, A., Chamberlain, S. R., Monti, M. M., Duncan, J., & Owen, A. M. (2010). The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: Inhibition and attentional control. NeuroImage, 50(3), 13131319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.109 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hancock, J. T., & Khoshgoftaar, T. M. (2020). Survey on categorical data for neural networks. Journal of Big Data, 7(1), Article28. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40537-020-00305-w CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartwigsen, G., Neef, N. E., Camilleri, J. A., Margulies, D. S., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2019). Functional segregation of the right inferior frontal gyrus: Evidence from coactivation-based parcellation. Cerebral Cortex, 29(4), 15321546. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy049 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayasaka, S., & Nichols, T. E. (2004). Combining voxel intensity and cluster extent with permutation test framework. NeuroImage, 23(1), 5463. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.035 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helpman, L., Zhu, X., Suarez-Jimenez, B., Lazarov, A., Monk, C., & Neria, Y. (2017). Sex differences in trauma-related psychopathology: A critical review of neuroimaging literature (2014-2017. Current Psychiatry Reports, 19(12), Article104. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-017-0854-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holland, A. C., Addis, D. R., & Kensinger, E. A. (2011). The neural correlates of specific versus general autobiographical memory construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia, 49(12), 31643177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyman, B., & Williams, L. (2001). Resilience among women survivors of child sexual abuse. Affilia, 16(2), 198219. https://doi.org/10.1177/08861090122094226 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackowski, A., Perera, T. D., Abdallah, C. G., Garrido, G., Tang, C. Y., Martinez, J., Mathew, S. J., Gorman, J. M., Rosenblum, L. A., Smith, E. L. (2011). Early-life stress, corpus callosum development, hippocampal volumetrics, and anxious behavior in male nonhuman primates. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 192(1), 3744. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.11.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Japee, S., Holiday, K., Satyshur, M. D., Mukai, I., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2015). A role of right middle frontal gyrus in reorienting of attention: A case study. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 9, Article23. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2015.00023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalisch, R., Baker, D. G., Basten, U., Boks, M. P., Bonanno, G. A., Brummelman, E., Chmitorz, A., Fernandez, G., Fiebach, C. J., Galatzer-Levy, I. (2017). The resilience framework as a strategy to combat stress-related disorders. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(11), 784790. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0200-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalisch, R., Cramer, A. O., Binder, H., Fritz, J., Leertouwer, Ij, Lunansky, G., Meyer, B., Timmer, J., Veer, I. M., Van Harmelen, A.-L. (2019). Deconstructing and reconstructing resilience: A dynamic network approach. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(5), 765777. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619855637 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalisch, R., Köber, G., Binder, H., Ahrens, K. F., Basten, U., Chmitorz, A., Choi, K. W., Fiebach, C. J., Goldbach, N., Neumann, R. J. (2021). The frequent stressor and mental health monitoring-paradigm: A proposal for the operationalization and measurement of resilience and the identification of resilience processes in longitudinal observational studies. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. Article 710493. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.710493 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalisch, R., Müller, M. B., & Tüscher, O. (2015). A conceptual framework for the neurobiological study of resilience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38, e92. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1400082X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U., Flynn, C., Moreci, P., Williamson, D., & Ryan, N. (1997). Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-sADS-pL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(7), 980988. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W. T., Demler, O., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6):617–27. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Green, J. G., Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alhamzawi, A. O., Alonso, J., Angermeyer, M. (2010). Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO world mental health surveys. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 197(5), 378385. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.110.080499 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Konrad, K., Kohls, G., Baumann, S., Bernhard, A., Martinelli, A., Ackermann, K., Smaragdi, A., Gonzalez-Madruga, K., Wells, A., Rogers, J. C., Pauli, R., Clanton, R., Baker, R., Kersten, L., Prätzlich, M., Oldenhof, H., Jansen, L., Kleeven, A., Bigorra, A., …Freitag, C. M. (2022). Sex differences in psychiatric comorbidity and clinical presentation in youths with conduct disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 63(2), 218228. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13428 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kühn, S., & Gallinat, J. (2013). Gray matter correlates of posttraumatic stress disorder: A quantitative meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 73(1), 7074. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.06.029 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leal, A. S. M., & Silvers, J. A. (2021). Neurobiological markers of resilience to early life adversity during adolescence. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 6(2), 238247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.08.004 Google Scholar
Luby, J. L., Barch, D., Whalen, D., Tillman, R., & Belden, A. (2017). Association between early life adversity and risk for poor emotional and physical health in adolescence: A putative mechanistic neurodevelopmental pathway. JAMA Pediatrics, 171(12), 11681175. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2017.3009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marriott, C., Hamilton-Giachritsis, C., & Harrop, C. (2014). Factors promoting resilience following childhood sexual abuse: A structured, narrative review of the literature. Child Abuse Review, 23(1), 1734. https://doi.org/10.1002/car.225 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martel, M. M. (2013). Sexual selection and sex differences in the prevalence of childhood externalizing and adolescent internalizing disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 139(6):12211259. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032247 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrory, E. J., Puetz, V. B., Maguire, E. A., Mechelli, A., Palmer, A., Gerin, M. I., Kelly, P. A., Koutoufa, I., & Viding, E. (2017). Autobiographical memory: A candidate latent vulnerability mechanism for psychiatric disorder following childhood maltreatment. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 211(4), 216222. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.117.201798 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Green, J. G., Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., & Kessler, R. C. (2012). Childhood adversities and first onset of psychiatric disorders in a national sample of US adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(11), 11511160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller-Lewis, L. R., Searle, A. K., Sawyer, M. G., Baghurst, P. A., & Hedley, D. (2013). Resource factors for mental health resilience in early childhood: An analysis with multiple methodologies. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 7(1), 6. https://doi.org/10.1186/1753-2000-7-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moreno-López, L., Ioannidis, K., Askelund, A. D., Smith, A. J., Schueler, K., & van Harmelen, A.-L. (2020). The resilient emotional brain: A scoping review of the medial prefrontal cortex and limbic structure and function in resilient adults with a history of childhood maltreatment. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 5(4), 392402. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.12.008 Google Scholar
Morey, R. A., Haswell, C. C., Hooper, S. R., & De Bellis, M. D. (2016). Amygdala, hippocampus, and ventral medial prefrontal cortex volumes differ in maltreated youth with and without chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology, 41(3), 791801. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.205 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nichols, T. E., Das, S., Eickhoff, S. B., Evans, A. C., Glatard, T., Hanke, M., Kriegeskorte, N., Milham, M. P., Poldrack, R. A., Poline, J. B., Proal, E., Thirion, B., Van Essen, D. C., White, T., & Yeo, B. T. (2017). Best practices in data analysis and sharing in neuroimaging using MRI. Nature Neuroscience, 20(3):299303. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4500 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nimarko, A. F., Garrett, A. S., Carlson, G. A., & Singh, M. K. (2019). Neural correlates of emotion processing predict resilience in youth at familial risk for mood disorders. Development and Psychopathology, 31(3), 10371052. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000579 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Connor, B. P. (2000). SPSS and SAS programs for determining the number of components using parallel analysis and Velicer’s MAP test. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 32(3), 396402. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03200807 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panizzon, M. S., Fennema-Notestine, C., Eyler, L. T., Jernigan, T. L., Prom-Wormley, E., Neale, M., Jacobson, K., Lyons, M. J., Grant, M. D., Franz, C. E. (2009). Distinct genetic influences on cortical surface area and cortical thickness. Cerebral Cortex, 19(11), 27282735. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp026 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, A. C., Crockett, L., Richards, M., & Boxer, A. (1988). A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17(2), 117133. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01537962 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Puetz, V. B., Viding, E., Hoffmann, F., Gerin, M. I., Sharp, M., Rankin, G., Maguire, E. A., Mechelli, A., & McCrory, E. J. (2021). Autobiographical memory as a latent vulnerability mechanism following childhood maltreatment: Association with future depression symptoms and prosocial behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 33(4), 13001307. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420000504 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, E. T., Huang, C.-C., Lin, C.-P., Feng, J., & Joliot, M. (2020). Automated anatomical labelling atlas 3. NeuroImage, 206, Article116189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116189 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Using sex differences in psychopathology to study causal mechanisms: Unifying issues and research strategies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(8), 10921115. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00194 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samplin, E., Ikuta, T., Malhotra, A. K., Szeszko, P. R., & DeRosse, P. (2013). Sex differences in resilience to childhood maltreatment: Effects of trauma history on hippocampal volume, general cognition and subclinical psychosis in healthy adults. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47(9), 11741179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.05.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smaragdi, A., Cornwell, H., Toschi, N., Riccelli, R., Gonzalez-Madruga, K., Wells, A., Clanton, R., Baker, R., Rogers, J., Martin-Key, N. (2017). Sex differences in the relationship between conduct disorder and cortical structure in adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(8), 703712. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.05.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Southwick, S. M., Bonanno, G. A., Masten, A. S., Panter-Brick, C., & Yehuda, R. (2014). Resilience definitions, theory, and challenges: Interdisciplinary perspectives. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 5(1). https://doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v5.25338 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoeckel, M., Weder, B., Binkofski, F., Choi, H., Amunts, K., Pieperhoff, P., Shah, N., & Seitz, R. (2004). Left and right superior parietal lobule in tactile object discrimination. European Journal of Neuroscience, 19(4), 10671072. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03185.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, D., Haswell, C. C., Morey, R. A., & De Bellis, M. D. (2018). Brain structural covariance network centrality in maltreated youth with PTSD and in maltreated youth resilient to PTSD. Development and Psychopathology, 31(2), 557571. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418000093 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.) Allyn & Bacon/Pearson Education.Google Scholar
Talati, A., & Hirsch, J. (2005). Functional specialization within the medial frontal gyrus for perceptual go/no-go decisions based on, what,”“when, and, where, related information: An fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(7), 981993. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929054475226 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teicher, M.S., Samson, J.A., Anderson, C.M., & Ohashi, K. (2016). The effects of childhood maltreatment on brain structure, function and connectivity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(10), 652666. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.111 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valentino, K., Toth, S. L., & Cicchetti, D. (2009). Autobiographical memory functioning among abused, neglected, and nonmaltreated children: The overgeneral memory effect. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(8), 10291038. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02072.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Harmelen, A-L., Kievit, R. A., Ioannidis, K., Neufeld, S., Jones, P. B., Bullmore, E., Dolan, R., NSPN Consortium, Fonagy, P., & Goodyer, I. (2017). Adolescent friendships predict later resilient functioning across psychosocial domains in a healthy community cohort. Psychological Medicine, 47(13), 23122322. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717000836CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, J., Yang, Y., Fan, L., Xu, J., Li, C., Liu, Y., Fox, P. T., Eickhoff, S. B., Yu, C., & Jiang, T. (2015). Convergent functional architecture of the superior parietal lobule unraveled with multimodal neuroimaging approaches. Human Brain Mapping, 36(1), 238257. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22626 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warner, R. M. (2013). Applied statistics: From bivariate through multivariate techniques. SAGE Publications.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated intelligence scale (WASI). The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler intelligence scale for children-fourth edition (WISC-IV). The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Whittle, S., Dennison, M., Vijayakumar, N., Simmons, J. G., Yücel, M., Lubman, D. I., Pantelis, C., & Allen, N. B. (2013). Childhood maltreatment and psychopathology affect brain development during adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(9), 940952. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.06.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Worsley, K. J., Marrett, S., Neelin, P., Vandal, A. C., Friston, K. J., & Evans, A. C. (1996). A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation. Human Brain Mapping, 4(1), 5873. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1996)4:1 3.0.CO;2-O>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants included in the voxel-based morphometry analysis (N = 298)

Figure 1

Figure 1. Positive correlations between resilience scores and gray matter volume in (a) the right inferior frontal gyrus (rs = .15) and (b) the right medial frontal gyrus (rs = .15) in the whole-brain analysis.

Figure 2

Table 2. Coordinates and cluster sizes for the correlations between resilience scores and gray matter volume, and sex-by-resilience score interactions in the whole-brain analysis (N = 298)

Figure 3

Figure 2. Sex-by-resilience score interactions for gray matter volume in (a) the left middle temporal gyrus (Females, rs = −.21; males, rs = .21) and (b) the right middle frontal gyrus (Females, rs = .21; males, rs = −.10) in the whole-brain analysis.

Supplementary material: File

Cornwell et al. supplementary material

Cornwell et al. supplementary material

Download Cornwell et al. supplementary material(File)
File 375.4 KB