Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-k7p5g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-10T13:30:46.090Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Replies to Lombrozo, Piccinini, and Poirier and Beaulac

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 June 2011

Édouard Machery*
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Book Symposium/Tribune du livre
Copyright
Copyright © Canadian Philosophical Association 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ahn, W., and Kim, N. S. 2008Causal Theories of Mental Disorder Concepts.” Psychological Science Agenda 22: 38.Google Scholar
Allen, S. W., and Brooks, L. R. 1991Specializing the Operation of an Explicit Rule.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 120: 319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, G. D., and Converse, S. A. 1986Illness Representations: A Prototype Approach.” Health Psychology 5: 95114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carmichael, C., and Hayes, B. K. 2001Prior Knowledge and Exemplar Encoding in Children’s Concept Acquisition.” Child Development 72: 10711090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. 1993 Associative Engines. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, I. M., and Quillian, M. R. 1969Retrieval Time from Semantic Memory.” Journal of verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 8: 240248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R., and Tsivkin, S. 1999Sources of Mathematical Thinking: Behavioral and Brain-Imaging Evidence.” Science 284: 970974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. St. B. T. 2008Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment and social cognition.” Annual Review of Psychology 59: 255278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. St. B. T., and Frankish, K., Eds. 2009 In Two Minds. Dual Processes and Beyond. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. 1975 The Language of Thought. New York: Crowell.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. 1998 Concepts, Where Cognitive Science Went Wrong. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. 2008 LOT 2: The language of thought revisited. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., and Regier, T. P. 1996How Do We Tell an Association from a Rule?Psychological Bulletin 119: 2326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M. 1997What Memory Is For.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20: 155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffiths, T. L., Sanborn, A. N., Canini, K. R., and Navarro, D. J. 2008 “Categorization as Nonparametric Bayesian Density Estimation.” In The Probabilistic Mind: Prospects for Rational Models of Cognition. Edited by Oaksford, M. and Chater, N., pp. 303328. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hampton, J. A. 1979Polymorphous Concepts in Semantic Memory.” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18: 441461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hampton, J. A. 1981An Investigation of the Nature of Abstract Concepts.” Memory and Cognition 9: 149156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, B. K., and Kearney, L. 2010Defending the Concept of ‘Concepts’.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33: 214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Labov, W. 1973 “The Boundaries of Words and Their Meanings.” In New Ways of Analyzing Variation in English. Edited by Baily, C. J. & Shuy, R., pp. 340373. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Lombrozo, T. 2009Explanation and Categorization: How ‘Why?’” Informs “What?” Cognition 110: 248253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machery, E. 2005Concepts Are Not a Natural Kind.” Philosophy of Science 72: 444467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machery, E. 2006How to Split Concepts. Reply To Piccinini and Scott.” Philosophy of Science 73: 410418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machery, E. 2007100 Years of Psychology of Concepts: The Theoretical Notion of Concept and Its Operationalization.” Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 38: 6384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Machery, E. 2010The Heterogeneity of Knowledge Representation and the Elimination of Concept.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33: 231244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malt, B. C. 1989An On-Line Investigation Of Prototype And Exemplar Strategies In Classification.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 15: 539555.Google ScholarPubMed
Margolis, E., and Laurence, S. 2010Concepts and Theoretical Unification.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33: 219220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mercier, H. 2010How to Cut a Concept? Review of Doing without Concepts by Edouard Machery.” Biology and Philosophy 25: 269277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, G. L., and Medin, D. L. 1985The Role of Theories in Conceptual Coherence.” Psychological Review 92: 289316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, G., Eva, K., Brooks, L., and Hamstra, S.. 2006 “Expertise in Medicine and Surgery.” In The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance, Edited by Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Feltovich, P. J., and Hoffman, R. R. , pp. 339353. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosofsky, R. M. 1992 “Exemplar-Based Approach to Relating Categorization, Identification, and Recognition.” In Multidimensional Models of Perception and Cognition, Edited by Ashby, F. G., pp. 363393. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Peacocke, C. 1992 A Study of Concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Piccinini, G., and Scott, S. 2006Splitting Concepts.” Philosophy of Science 73: 390409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rehder, B. 2006When Causality and Similarity Compete in Category-Based Property Induction.” Memory and Cognition 34: 316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rehder, B., and Murphy, G. L. 2003A Knowledge-Resonance (KRES) Model of Category Learning.” Psychological Bulletin & Review 10: 789794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rey, G. 1983Concepts and Stereotypes.” Cognition 15: 237262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rey, G. 1985Concepts and Conceptions: A Reply to Smith, Medin and Rips. Cognition 19: 297303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E., and Mervis, C. B. 1975Family Resemblance: Studies in the Internal Structure of Categories.” Cognitive Psychology 7: 573605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rupert, R. D. Forthcoming “Concepts and Scientific Unity.” Metascience.Google Scholar
Samuels, R. 2009 “The Magical Number Two, Plus or Minus: Dual Process Theory as a Theory of Cognitive Kinds.” In In two minds: Dual process and beyond. Edited by Evans, J. & Frankish, K., pp. 129146. New York: Oxford University press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., and O’Connor, C. 1987Emotion Knowledge: Further Exploration of a Prototype Approach.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52: 10611086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, E. E., Patalano, A. L., and Jonides, J. 1998Alternative Strategies of Categorization.” Cognition 65: 167196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, J. D., and Minda, J. P. 1998Prototypes in the Mist: The Early Epochs of Category Learning.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 24: 14111436.Google Scholar
Strohminger, N., and Moore, B. W. 2010Banishing the Thought.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33: 225226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Temmerman, R. 2000 Towards New Ways of Terminology Description: The Sociocognitive-Approach. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodward, J., and Allman, J. 2008Moral Intuition: Its Neural Substrates and Normative Significance.” Journal of Physiology-Paris 101: 179202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar