Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-m8s7h Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-20T02:51:09.402Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Evolution and dysfunction of human cognitive and social traits: A transcriptional regulation perspective

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2022

Roman Zug*
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
Tobias Uller
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: roman.zug@biol.lu.se

Abstract

Evolutionary changes in brain and craniofacial development have endowed humans with unique cognitive and social skills, but also predisposed us to debilitating disorders in which these traits are disrupted. What are the developmental genetic underpinnings that connect the adaptive evolution of our cognition and sociality with the persistence of mental disorders with severe negative fitness effects? We argue that loss of function of genes involved in transcriptional regulation represents a crucial link between the evolution and dysfunction of human cognitive and social traits. The argument is based on the haploinsufficiency of many transcriptional regulator genes, which makes them particularly sensitive to loss-of-function mutations. We discuss how human brain and craniofacial traits evolved through partial loss of function (i.e. reduced expression) of these genes, a perspective compatible with the idea of human self-domestication. Moreover, we explain why selection against loss-of-function variants supports the view that mutation-selection-drift, rather than balancing selection, underlies the persistence of psychiatric disorders. Finally, we discuss testable predictions.

Type
Perspective
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Social media summary: Loss of function of transcriptional regulator genes links evolution and dysfunction of human cognitive and social traits.

Introduction

What sets us apart from other species is one of humanity's big questions. The most obvious candidate traits are related to human cognition and sociality, including problem solving, social cognition and communication (Laland & Seed, Reference Laland and Seed2021). The extraordinary cognitive and social abilities that evolved in the human lineage are largely based on changes in brain development, structure and function (Sousa et al., Reference Sousa, Meyer, Santpere, Gulden and Sestan2017a; Chin et al., Reference Chin, Chang and Holmes2022), accompanied by changes in craniofacial development and morphology, which enabled the evolution of the human face with its unique possibilities for social interaction and communication (Wilkins, Reference Wilkins2017; Lacruz et al., Reference Lacruz, Stringer, Kimbel, Wood, Harvati, O'Higgins and Arsuaga2019).

However, this remarkable success story also has a dark side. The genetic and developmental changes that enabled the evolution of human cognitive and social traits appear also to be associated with the risk of debilitating disorders in which these traits are disrupted, such as intellectual disability, schizophrenia and autism (Doan et al., Reference Doan, Shin and Walsh2018; Pattabiraman et al., Reference Pattabiraman, Muchnik and Sestan2020). Given that natural selection has been so powerful in shaping our unique cognition and sociality, why has it been unable to eliminate the genetic variants that predispose to the associated mental disorders with severe fitness costs?

Here we argue that loss of function (LOF) of genes involved in transcriptional regulation (TR) of brain and face development represents a crucial link between the adaptive evolution and the dysfunction of human cognitive and social traits. The argument is based on a crucial property of TR genes, namely their haploinsufficiency, which makes them particularly sensitive to LOF mutations. After a short overview of human disorders associated with brain and face development (neurodevelopmental disorders and neurocristopathies), we show that many of them are caused by LOF of haploinsufficient TR genes. We then discuss evidence showing that human brain and craniofacial traits evolved owing to partial LOF (reduced expression) of TR genes, and we show that this perspective is compatible with an evolutionary scenario of human self-domestication, including a key role of the neural crest. Further, we explain why purifying selection against LOF variants in TR genes makes mutation-selection-drift a more likely explanation than balancing selection for the evolutionary persistence of mental disorders. Finally, we list predictions from this theory that can be tested using powerful experimental model systems such as brain organoids, and comparative genomics involving ancient humans, non-human primates and domesticated species.

Human brain and face development and associated disorders

Human brain and face development is governed by complex processes involving cell proliferation, migration and differentiation. These processes have been covered comprehensively in excellent reviews of brain (Taverna et al., Reference Taverna, Götz and Huttner2014; Molnár et al., Reference Molnár, Clowry, Sestan, Alzu'bi, Bakken, Hevner and Kriegstein2019; Kelley & Paşca, Reference Kelley and Paşca2022) and craniofacial development (Minoux & Rijli, Reference Minoux and Rijli2010; Cordero et al., Reference Cordero, Brugmann, Chu, Bajpai, Jame and Helms2011; Murillo-Rincón & Kaucka, Reference Murillo-Rincón and Kaucka2020). Moreover, brain and craniofacial development are intimately connected (Marcucio et al., Reference Marcucio, Hallgrimsson and Young2015; LaMantia, Reference LaMantia2020; Naqvi et al., Reference Naqvi, Sleyp, Hoskens, Indencleef, Spence, Bruffaerts and Claes2021) and tightly linked to the neural crest (NC), a transient embryonic cell population. Neural crest cells actively migrate throughout the developing embryo and differentiate into a large number of cell types and tissues. For example, cranial NC cells give rise to craniofacial cartilage and bones, thus forming large parts of the head and face (Minoux & Rijli, Reference Minoux and Rijli2010; Cordero et al., Reference Cordero, Brugmann, Chu, Bajpai, Jame and Helms2011; Murillo-Rincón & Kaucka, Reference Murillo-Rincón and Kaucka2020).

The cellular processes that underlie brain and face development are orchestrated by gene regulatory networks (GRNs). In recent years, there has been great progress in elucidating the genetics and GRNs underlying brain (Nord et al., Reference Nord, Pattabiraman, Visel and Rubenstein2015; Trevino et al., Reference Trevino, Müller, Andersen, Sundaram, Kathiria, Shcherbina and Greenleaf2021) and craniofacial development (White et al., Reference White, Indencleef, Naqvi, Eller, Hoskens, Roosenboom and Claes2021; Naqvi et al., Reference Naqvi, Hoskens, Wilke, Weinberg, Shaffer, Walsh and Claes2022). Key players in these GRNs are transcription factors (TFs) that bind to cell type-specific cis-regulatory elements (CREs), in particular enhancers, thus driving gene expression programmes that control cell fate determination, migration, and maturation. Cell type-specific gene expression is assisted by transcriptional cofactors and additional regulatory proteins involved in chromatin remodelling and DNA methylation. Here, we subsume the action of TFs, cofactors, chromatin remodellers and other proteins that regulate cell type-specific gene expression under the term transcriptional regulation (TR).

Disruption of the GRNs underlying brain and face development can lead to a plethora of disorders and syndromes. Those that result from disrupted brain development are called neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs). These include intellectual disability (ID), autism spectrum disorders (ASD), schizophrenia (SCZ), epilepsy, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), bipolar disorder (BD), major depressive disorder (MDD) and speech and language disorders (for references, see Table 1 in Zug, Reference Zug2022).

Disorders arising from defects in NC cell specification and migration are called neurocristopathies (NCPs; Vega-Lopez et al., Reference Vega-Lopez, Cerrizuela, Tribulo and Aybar2018; Sato et al., Reference Sato, Handa, Priya, Watal, Becker and Sato2019). Neurocristopathies are highly diverse and affect disparate tissues; here we focus on those that involve the cranial NC and hence result in craniofacial abnormalities. Given the tight integration of brain and craniofacial development, many human disorders and syndromes show features of both NDDs and NCPs.

Many disorders of brain and face development are caused by loss of function of haploinsufficient TR genes

In the past 10 years or so, our understanding of the genetic architecture of NDDs such as ASD and SCZ has improved considerably. It has become clear that, despite high genetic heterogeneity, NDD risk genes converge on shared functional pathways, such as synaptic function and TR (Moyses-Oliveira et al., Reference Moyses-Oliveira, Yadav, Erdin and Talkowski2020; Parenti et al., Reference Parenti, Rabaneda, Schoen and Novarino2020; Mossink et al., Reference Mossink, Negwer, Schubert and Nadif Kasri2021). Hence, there are many NDDs that are mainly caused by rare, large-effect variants in TR genes (modified by the polygenic background of common variants of small effect). For example, TR genes associated with ASD risk include ARID1B, BCL11A, CHD8, FOXP1, KMT2C, MBD5, POGZ, TBR1, TCF4 and TCF20 (Sestan & State, Reference Sestan and State2018; Iakoucheva et al., Reference Iakoucheva, Muotri and Sebat2019; Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, De Rubeis and Schaefer2019; Lord et al., Reference Lord, Brugha, Charman, Cusack, Dumas, Frazier and Veenstra-VanderWeele2020; Satterstrom et al., Reference Satterstrom, Kosmicki, Wang, Breen, De Rubeis, An and Buxbaum2020; Zug, Reference Zug2022). TR genes associated with SCZ risk include AUTS2, CHD4, CHD8, EP300, KDM2B, KMT2C, KMT2D, SETD1A, SMARCC2 and TCF4 (Girard et al., Reference Girard, Gauthier, Noreau, Xiong, Zhou, Jouan and Rouleau2011; McCarthy et al., Reference McCarthy, Gillis, Kramer, Lihm, Yoon, Berstein and Corvin2014; Jia et al., Reference Jia, Chen, Fanous and Zhao2018; Doostparast Torshizi et al., Reference Doostparast Torshizi, Armoskus, Zhang, Forrest, Zhang, Souaiaia and Wang2019; Howrigan et al., Reference Howrigan, Rose, Samocha, Fromer, Cerrato, Chen and Neale2020).

A striking feature of these and many other TR genes is their haploinsufficiency (HI): a single functional allele is insufficient to sustain normal gene function; in other words, these genes do not tolerate heterozygous LOF (Figure 1; see Boxes 1 and 2; Zug, Reference Zug2022). Note that we use the term HI in a broader sense, describing both the property of genes intolerant to heterozygous LOF and the resulting condition. Owing to the HI of many TR genes, NDD risk variants affecting these genes are predominantly rare inherited or de novo heterozygous LOF mutations. This has been shown for ASD (De Rubeis et al., Reference De Rubeis, He, Goldberg, Poultney, Samocha, Cicek and Buxbaum2014; Iossifov et al., Reference Iossifov, O'Roak, Sanders, Ronemus, Krumm, Levy and Wigler2014; Krumm et al., Reference Krumm, Turner, Baker, Vives, Mohajeri, Witherspoon and Eichler2015; Ji et al., Reference Ji, Kember, Brown and Bućan2016; Sestan & State, Reference Sestan and State2018; Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, De Rubeis and Schaefer2019), SCZ (Girard et al., Reference Girard, Gauthier, Noreau, Xiong, Zhou, Jouan and Rouleau2011; Gulsuner et al., Reference Gulsuner, Walsh, Watts, Lee, Thornton, Casadei and McClellan2013; McCarthy et al., Reference McCarthy, Gillis, Kramer, Lihm, Yoon, Berstein and Corvin2014; Pardiñas et al., Reference Pardiñas, Holmans, Pocklington, Escott-Price, Ripke, Carrera and Walters2018; Howrigan et al., Reference Howrigan, Rose, Samocha, Fromer, Cerrato, Chen and Neale2020) and other NDDs (Samocha et al., Reference Samocha, Robinson, Sanders, Stevens, Sabo, McGrath and Daly2014; Kataoka et al., Reference Kataoka, Matoba, Sawada, Kazuno, Ishiwata, Fujii and Kato2016; Deciphering Developmental Disorders Study, 2017; Kosmicki et al., Reference Kosmicki, Samocha, Howrigan, Sanders, Slowikowski, Lek and Daly2017; Brunet et al., Reference Brunet, Jech, Brugger, Kovacs, Alhaddad, Leszinski and Wagner2021; Wainberg et al., Reference Wainberg, Merico, Huguet, Zarrei, Jacquemont, Scherer and Tripathy2022). Furthermore, NDD risk variants also affect non-coding CREs, resulting in regulatory LOF, such as enhancer deletion, disruption and disconnection (see Box 2; D'haene & Vergult, Reference D'haene and Vergult2021). Frequently, affected CREs regulate those TR genes that are themselves associated with disease risk (Short et al., Reference Short, McRae, Gallone, Sifrim, Won, Geschwind and Hurles2018; Turner & Eichler, Reference Turner and Eichler2019; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Park, Theesfeld, Wong, Yuan, Scheckel and Troyanskaya2019). These findings demonstrate that heterozygous LOF of haploinsufficient TR genes is an important cause of NDDs. A more detailed account of NDDs caused by HI of TR genes is given in Table S1.

Figure 1. Heterozygous loss of function (LOF) of haploinsufficient genes involved in transcriptional regulation (TR) of brain and face development leads to neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs) and neurocristopathies (NCPs). Only a subset of NDDs are shown, including attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), bipolar disorder (BD), intellectual disability (ID) and schizophrenia (SCZ). CRE, cis-regulatory element.

Box 1: Why are so many TR genes haploinsufficient?

It has long been known that many NDDs, NCPs and other developmental disorders are due to HI of TR genes. However, it is not well understood how and why TR HI leads to disease. Building upon earlier work (Wilkie Reference Wilkie1994; Veitia, Reference Veitia2002; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Nguyen and Veitia2019), we have recently proposed a hypothesis that is based on the crucial role of TR genes in determining and maintaining cell fate and identity (Zug, Reference Zug2022). We argue (a) that the GRNs that determine cell identity comprise bistable switches, involving positive feedback and cooperativity, (b) that these features make dosage sensitivity of TR genes an inherent property of fate decisions, and (c) that disorders caused by TR HI result from disrupted positive feedback or cooperativity (Zug, Reference Zug2022).

HI is closely related to the concept of gene essentiality. A gene is defined as essential when loss of its function is associated with a profound loss of fitness; in other words, essential genes do not tolerate loss-of-function variants, and there is strong negative (purifying) selection against those variants (Bartha et al., Reference Bartha, di Iulio, Venter and Telenti2018). Since haploinsufficient genes do not even tolerate the loss of one of the two alleles, HI can be regarded as a particularly strict form of gene essentiality. A recent analysis found that essential genes act predominantly in TR, chromatin modification and lineage specification (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Zhang, Jiang, Li, Li, Zhao and Bo2020), corroborating that developmentally important TR genes tend to be essential.

Box 2: Loss-of-function mutations and their evolutionary implications

Loss of function can result from reduced gene dosage, expression or protein activity (Wilkie, Reference Wilkie1994), and be caused by variation in both coding and non-coding sequences:

  1. (1) Structural variation (e.g. gene deletions, translocations) can reduce gene dosage.

  2. (2) Nonsense and frameshift mutations introduce premature stop codons, which usually results in nonsense-mediated mRNA decay, leading to reduced gene expression.

  3. (3) Missense mutations, splice site mutations, and small in-frame indels that inactivate functional protein domains will lead to reduced protein activity.

  4. (4) Variation in regulatory sequences can also lead to reduced gene expression via deletion, disruption or disconnection of CREs (so-called regulatory LOF).

The LOF can be complete (gene function eliminated; so-called amorphic alleles) or partial (gene function reduced; hypomorphic alleles). Hence, any mutation that causes downregulation of a particular gene results in partial LOF.

Loss-of-function mutations have long been regarded as relatively unimportant for adaptive evolution. This is mainly because most LOF mutations are recessive to the wild type and hence phenotypically neutral. However, in haploinsufficient genes, LOF mutations are dominant and thus easily visible to selection, because their effect on fitness is seen in heterozygotes. Therefore, HI counters the argument that LOF mutations cannot play a major role in adaptive evolution (Murray, Reference Murray2020). In fact, the idea that LOF mutations do contribute to adaptation and evolutionary novelties is gaining momentum (Olson, Reference Olson1999; Oh et al., Reference Oh, Choi, Goh and Hahn2015; Albalat & Cañestro, Reference Albalat and Cañestro2016; Murray, Reference Murray2020; Monroe et al., Reference Monroe, McKay, Weigel and Flood2021). In the main text, we present evidence supporting this idea with regard to the evolution of human cognitive and social traits, which we argue has been driven by selection for reduced expression (partial LOF) of haploinsufficient TR genes.

Another key finding concerning the genetic architecture of NDDs, which is related to their functional convergence, is the fact that seemingly diverse psychiatric disorders share common risk genes (The Brainstorm Consortium, 2018; Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, 2019; Myers et al., Reference Myers, Challman, Bernier, Bourgeron, Chung, Constantino and Ledbetter2020; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Feng and Smoller2021; Rees et al., Reference Rees, Creeth, Hwu, Chen, Tsuang, Glatt and O'Donovan2021). Many of these shared risk genes are involved in TR (Zug, Reference Zug2022). For example, as described above, HI of CHD8, KMT2C and TCF4 is associated with both ASD and SCZ risk. Taken together, many NDD risk genes are involved in TR and characterized by HI and widespread pleiotropy.

Loss of function of haploinsufficient TR genes is also the cause of many NCPs that affect craniofacial development (Table S1). In addition to heterozygous LOF mutations affecting the coding sequences of NCP-associated genes, recent studies have revealed the important role of regulatory LOF in causing NCPs (Laugsch et al., Reference Laugsch, Bartusel, Rehimi, Alirzayeva, Karaolidou, Crispatzu and Rada-Iglesias2019; Long et al., Reference Long, Osterwalder, Welsh, Hansen, Davies, Liu and Wysocka2020; Sánchez-Gaya et al., Reference Sánchez-Gaya, Mariner-Fauli and Rada-Iglesias2020). In sum, many NDDs and NCPs are caused by heterozygous LOF of haploinsufficient TR genes through coding or non-coding, regulatory variants of large effect size (modified by common variants of small effect). The fact that many of these TR genes are involved in both brain and face development underscores their pleiotropic nature.

The role of LOF of TR genes in the evolution of human cognitive and social traits and associated disorders

Human-specific selection has probably acted on CREs of TR genes whose HI causes NDDs/NCPs

Research into the evolution of human-specific traits has benefited from the increasing availability of genome sequences of other great apes, such as the chimpanzee, and of archaic humans, such as Neanderthals and Denisovans (Pääbo, Reference Pääbo2014). These advances have increased our understanding of the genetic peculiarities that distinguish anatomically modern humans (AMHs) from their closest relatives, both living and extinct. For example, comparative genomic analyses have identified hundreds of so-called human accelerated regions (HARs) – genomic loci that are highly conserved among vertebrates yet show accelerated sequence divergence in the human lineage. Importantly, most HARs appear to be driven by positive selection, rather than by non-adaptive processes such as GC-biased gene conversion (Kostka et al., Reference Kostka, Hubisz, Siepel and Pollard2012; Hubisz & Pollard, Reference Hubisz and Pollard2014; Levchenko et al., Reference Levchenko, Kanapin, Samsonova and Gainetdinov2018). Moreover, almost all HARs are non-coding CREs (Pollard et al., Reference Pollard, Salama, King, Kern, Dreszer, Katzman and Haussler2006; Prabhakar et al., Reference Prabhakar, Noonan, Pääbo and Rubin2006; Bird et al., Reference Bird, Stranger, Liu, Thomas, Ingle, Beazley and Dermitzakis2007; Lindblad-Toh et al., Reference Lindblad-Toh, Garber, Zuk, Lin, Parker, Washietl and Kellis2011; Capra et al., Reference Capra, Erwin, McKinsey, Rubenstein and Pollard2013; Gittelman et al., Reference Gittelman, Hun, Ay, Madeoy, Pennacchio, Noble and Akey2015), in line with the hypothesis that adaptive divergence in human evolution, particularly with regard to cognitive traits, is primarily driven by regulatory changes (King & Wilson, Reference King and Wilson1975; Haygood et al., Reference Haygood, Babbitt, Fedrigo and Wray2010; Enard et al., Reference Enard, Messer and Petrov2014; Peyrégne et al., Reference Peyrégne, Boyle, Dannemann and Prüfer2017; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Mosti and Silver2021). Strikingly, many HARs have been found to regulate genes that are involved in the TR of brain and face development and for which HI leads to NDDs/NCPs (Table 1; Doan et al., Reference Doan, Shin and Walsh2018; Levchenko et al., Reference Levchenko, Kanapin, Samsonova and Gainetdinov2018; Kozlenkov et al., Reference Kozlenkov, Vermunt, Apontes, Li, Hao, Sherwood and Dracheva2020; Girskis et al., Reference Girskis, Stergachis, DeGennaro, Doan, Qian, Johnson and Walsh2021). Indeed, the vast majority of haploinsufficient TR genes implicated in the evolution of human brain/face development show changes in CREs, rather than coding changes (Table 1). Finally, among 29 human tissues or cell types scanned for signals of positive selection on TF binding sites in CREs (on the basis of predicted binding affinity changes), brain-related cell types show the highest proportion of positive selection (Liu & Robinson-Rechavi, Reference Liu and Robinson-Rechavi2020; see also Babbitt et al., Reference Babbitt, Haygood, Nielsen and Wray2017). Together, these findings underscore the importance of positive selection on the regulatory architecture of haploinsufficient brain- and face-related TR genes in the evolution of human-specific traits.

Table 1. Haploinsufficient transcriptional regulation (TR) genes implicated both in neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs)/neurocristopathies (NCPs) and in the evolution of human brain and/or face development

Notes: 1, If available, the OMIM phenotype number is given in parentheses. Listed clinical features do not necessarily appear in all (or even most) patients. ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; BD, bipolar disorder; ID, intellectual disability; NCP, neurocristopathy; NDD, neurodevelopmental disorder; SCZ, schizophrenia. 2, Changes occurred in the evolutionary lineage leading to anatomically modern humans (AMHs), either compared with archaic humans or with non-human primates. CRE, cis-regulatory element; HAR, human accelerated region. 3, References for human-specific evolutionary changes of the particular gene. For references for the NDD/NCP caused by haploinsufficiency of the gene, see Table S1 in the Supplementary Material.

Evidence of reduced TR gene expression during human evolution

That selection appears to have acted predominantly on CREs, and that the TR genes regulated by those CREs are exquisitely dosage sensitive (as evidenced by their HI), suggests that the associated human-specific traits evolved, at least in part, owing to changes in the genes’ expression level. Indeed, with respect to craniofacial traits, there is good support for this idea, in particular, for a reduction in gene expression (i.e. partial LOF). As described in detail below, several studies indicate (1) that the phenotypic changes in craniofacial morphology that occurred from archaic to modern humans are due to reduced TR gene expression and (2) that these phenotypic changes seen during human evolution resemble the phenotypic changes seen in HI-associated disorders, which are likewise due to reduced TR gene expression. In other words, there seems to be a parallelism between the evolutionary and the pathological consequences of reduced TR gene expression for craniofacial features (Figure 2). For example, reduced expression of the haploinsufficient TF gene NFIX causes Malan syndrome, whose clinical features include a high forehead and a prominent chin (Priolo et al., Reference Priolo, Schanze, Tatton-Brown, Mulder, Tenorio, Kooblall and Hennekam2018). Intriguingly, the same genetic and phenotypic changes (reduced NFIX expression, resulting in a more prominent forehead and chin) are likely to have also occurred in the evolution from archaic to modern humans (Gokhman et al., Reference Gokhman, Nissim-Rafinia, Agranat-Tamir, Housman, García-Pérez, Lizano and Carmel2020). A similar correspondence between the evolutionary and the pathological changes in craniofacial traits following TR gene downregulation has been suggested for BAZ1B (reduced facial bones; Williams–Beuren syndrome; Zanella et al., Reference Zanella, Vitriolo, Andirko, Martins, Sturm, O'Rourke and Testa2019), and SATB2 (smaller jaws, flatter face, higher forehead; SATB2-associated syndrome; Weiss et al., Reference Weiss, Harshman, Inoue, Fraser, Petrov, Ahituv and Gokhman2021); both cases are discussed in more detail in the next section. Taken together, these findings suggest that downregulation of haploinsufficient TR genes might have played an important role in the evolution of human craniofacial traits.

Figure 2. The parallelism between the evolutionary and the pathological consequences of reduced TR gene expression for craniofacial development. The figure shows several craniofacial features of archaic humans, healthy anatomically modern humans (AMHs), and AMHs with a haploinsufficiency-associated disorder. Both the evolutionary changes from archaic humans to AMHs and the pathological changes from healthy AMHs to individuals with the disorder are due to reduced TR gene expression. Adapted from Gokhman et al. (Reference Gokhman, Nissim-Rafinia, Agranat-Tamir, Housman, García-Pérez, Lizano and Carmel2020).

What do we know about TR gene expression changes in the evolution of the human brain? A promising approach to address this question is to compare gene expression patterns in brain samples from humans and other primates. In the past few years, it has become possible to compare brain transcriptomes at single-cell resolution, which enables the detection of cell type-specific gene expression differences. Interestingly, comparisons between primates show that, in the human lineage, gene expression divergence is higher in non-neuronal cell types such as oligodendrocytes than in neurons (Berto et al., Reference Berto, Mendizabal, Usui, Toriumi, Chatterjee, Douglas and Konopka2019; Hu et al., Reference Hu, Li and Wang2020; Khrameeva et al., Reference Khrameeva, Kurochkin, Han, Guijarro, Kanton, Santel and Khaitovich2020; Pembroke et al., Reference Pembroke, Hartl and Geschwind2021). Strikingly, genes that showed human-specific downregulation in oligodendrocytes were enriched for functions related to TR; among those downregulated TR genes were ARID1B, ARID2 and TCF20 (Berto et al., Reference Berto, Mendizabal, Usui, Toriumi, Chatterjee, Douglas and Konopka2019). Other brain-related TR genes that probably experienced reduced expression during human evolution are FOXP2 and CUX1. Maricic et al. (Reference Maricic, Günther, Georgiev, Gehre, Ćurlin, Schreiweis and Pääbo2013) found a regulatory change in FOXP2 specific to modern humans that diminishes binding of the TF POU3F2, thus driving reduced reporter gene expression compared with the ancestral allele. Finally, using allele-specific expression data from human–chimpanzee hybrid cortical spheroids, Starr et al. (Reference Starr, Gokhman and Fraser2022) recently found that expression of CUX1 from the human allele is lower than expression from the chimpanzee allele. These results suggest that reduced TR gene expression has played a role in human brain evolution. We do not know the specific effects that downregulation of these TR genes had on human brain development. However, this knowledge is crucial to understand how reduced expression affected the evolution of human cognition and social behaviour. Therefore, we cannot yet determine the relationship between the evolutionary and the pathological consequences of reduced TR gene expression for brain-related traits (remember that, for craniofacial traits, there is tentative evidence of a parallel relationship). The fact that TR HI in NDDs generally leads to ID and other impairments of brain function (Table 1) argues against a simple parallel relationship between the pathological and the evolutionary consequences of reduced TR gene expression with respect to brain-related traits, because cognitive function has increased, rather than decreased, during human evolution.

While few TR genes have so far been studied in detail, the findings described above suggest that reduced TR gene expression was involved in the evolution of both human craniofacial and brain-related traits. Thus, the results also add to the long-standing discussion about the developmental mechanisms that contributed to the evolution of human-specific traits. While many researchers have argued for a major role of heterochrony (or more specifically, delayed gene expression) in human evolution (Gould, Reference Gould1977; Somel et al., Reference Somel, Franz, Yan, Lorenc, Guo, Giger and Khaitovich2009), the findings presented here emphasize the role of reduced gene expression (i.e. heterometry; see Arthur, Reference Arthur2000). Nevertheless, the conclusion that reduced TR gene expression was important during human evolution does not preclude the possibility that, in other developmental contexts, it was increased TR gene expression that fuelled the evolution of human-specific traits. Moreover, the effects of TR gene expression changes on development and evolution of craniofacial traits are not necessarily the same as those on development and evolution of brain-related traits (see Box 3). Also, our conclusion is currently based only on a handful of cases and hence somewhat speculative, while several crucial questions remain open. Nevertheless, we think that HI of TR genes predisposes these genes to LOF mutations that do have evolutionary implications (see Box 2). Therefore, we believe that future research will unearth further evidence supporting the significance of TR gene downregulation in the evolution of human-specific traits.

Box 3: Evolution and development of human brain and face – evidence of partial independence between both structures

Modern humans have a large, globular brain and a short, retracted face that distinguish them from their extinct Homo relatives. Based on recent morphometric analyses of Homo sapiens fossils, the evolution of modern human brain and face can be roughly divided into an early and a later stage. Hominin fossils from around 300,000 years ago suggest that early H. sapiens already showed key features of modern human craniofacial morphology (Hublin et al., Reference Hublin, Ben-Ncer, Bailey, Freidline, Neubauer, Skinner and Gunz2017; Lacruz et al., Reference Lacruz, Stringer, Kimbel, Wood, Harvati, O'Higgins and Arsuaga2019). Brain sizes in early H. sapiens also already fell within the range of those of present-day humans. In contrast, brain shape changed only considerably later, resulting in the typical globular modern shape between about 100,000 and 35,000 years ago. Intriguingly, this later stage involving brain globularization parallels the emergence of behavioural modernity, the suite of behavioural and cognitive traits that distinguishes modern humans from other hominins (Neubauer et al., Reference Neubauer, Hublin and Gunz2018). It seems likely that the emergence of these traits in modern humans was facilitated by the two major features contributing to brain globularization: bulging of the parietal cortex and of the cerebellum (Boeckx, Reference Boeckx2017; Kochiyama et al., Reference Kochiyama, Ogihara, Tanabe, Kondo, Amano, Hasegawa and Akazawa2018; Neubauer et al., Reference Neubauer, Hublin and Gunz2018; Pereira-Pedro et al., Reference Pereira-Pedro, Bruner, Gunz and Neubauer2020). Both brain areas are engaged in a broad range of sensorimotor, cognitive and social functions (Sokolov et al., Reference Sokolov, Miall and Ivry2017; Freedman & Ibos, Reference Freedman and Ibos2018). Hence, parietal and cerebellar bulging seem to be particularly important for the emergence of behavioural modernity.

These findings suggest that modern human cognition and behaviour evolved later than, and partially independently from, the modern human face. Indeed, this partial independence of human brain and face becomes apparent not only during evolution, but also in development. Naqvi et al. (Reference Naqvi, Sleyp, Hoskens, Indencleef, Spence, Bruffaerts and Claes2021) searched for genomic loci that influence both human face and brain shape and found that many of the shared brain-face loci include TR genes involved in craniofacial development. However, they also found that only a subset of shared loci indeed affects behavioural–cognitive traits and neuropsychiatric disorders. For example, shared TR genes that are involved in both NC and brain development (e.g. TCF4, ZEB2) do affect behavioural–cognitive and neuropsychiatric traits, while other shared TR genes are only involved in NC development, with no effects on brain-related traits (e.g. PAX3, TWIST1). Hence, genes affecting craniofacial traits are not necessarily the same as those affecting cognitive and behavioural traits.

Selection for prosociality and against fear and aggression: the human self-domestication hypothesis

In light of the evidence of reduced TR gene expression during human evolution, an important question is: what selective forces were responsible for this downregulation and the associated emergence of human-specific traits? One hypothesis is that these traits evolved in part through human self-domestication (HSD) (Hare, Reference Hare2017; strictly speaking, the term is a misnomer – in contrast to animal domestication, HSD did not involve any deliberate selection or goal-directedness; Sánchez-Villagra & van Schaik, Reference Sánchez-Villagra and van Schaik2019). According to the HSD hypothesis, selection during human evolution favoured in-group prosociality over aggression and fear. In the wake of this selection, humans evolved traits similar to those characteristic of other domestic animals. In domesticated mammals, this suite of behavioural, physiological and morphological traits that emerged via selection for tameness is referred to as the ‘domestication syndrome’ (Wilkins et al., Reference Wilkins, Wrangham and Fitch2014). Several craniofacial traits that distinguish modern humans from their archaic ancestors, such as a short, retracted face and a reduced brow ridge (Figure 2), fit the domestication syndrome, which can be seen as support for the HSD hypothesis (Hare, Reference Hare2017). In their seminal paper, Wilkins et al. (Reference Wilkins, Wrangham and Fitch2014) propose that the domestication syndrome ultimately results from multiple partial LOF mutations affecting dosage-sensitive NC genes (the ‘neural crest/domestication syndrome’ hypothesis; for an up-to-date account, see Wilkins et al., Reference Wilkins, Wrangham and Fitch2021). Indeed, many haploinsufficient TR genes involved in NC development, including BAZ1B, CHD7, MITF, PAX3, PAX9, RAI1, SATB2, SOX9, SOX10 and ZEB2, are not only known to cause human NCPs (Table S1), but also are candidate genes for the domestication syndrome (Wilkins et al., Reference Wilkins, Wrangham and Fitch2014; Pendleton et al., Reference Pendleton, Shen, Taravella, Emery, Veeramah, Boyko and Kidd2018). Hence, the neural crest/domestication syndrome hypothesis provides an appealing mechanism underlying HSD. Yet what is the evidence for this hypothesis with respect to human evolution?

In order to test the neural crest/domestication syndrome hypothesis with regard to HSD, Zanella et al. (Reference Zanella, Vitriolo, Andirko, Martins, Sturm, O'Rourke and Testa2019) focused on Williams–Beuren syndrome, whose characteristic features include a reduced, retracted face and pronounced friendliness and sociality – traits that fit the domestication syndrome. HI of the chromatin regulator BAZ1B has long been known as the causal factor for most of the characteristic features of Williams–Beuren syndrome (Lu et al., Reference Lu, Meng, Morris and Keating1998; Peoples et al., Reference Peoples, Cisco, Kaplan and Francke1998; Lalli et al., Reference Lalli, Jang, Park, Wang, Guzman, Zhou and Kosik2016). Zanella et al. (Reference Zanella, Vitriolo, Andirko, Martins, Sturm, O'Rourke and Testa2019) showed that BAZ1B is a master regulator of NC genes involved in craniofacial development, and Kuhlwilm and Boeckx (Reference Kuhlwilm and Boeckx2019) found nearly fixed mutations in the CREs of BAZ1B in the genomes of modern, but not archaic, humans. These mutations are probably partial LOF mutations causing slight reductions in gene expression and, ultimately, in craniofacial morphology, corresponding to the facial retraction from archaic to modern humans (Wilkins, Reference Wilkins2020). Strikingly, very similar findings were recently described for another NC gene, the TF SATB2. HI of SATB2 causes SATB2-associated syndrome (also termed Glass syndrome), whose features include small jaws, flat face and a friendly personality – again, traits that match the domestication syndrome. Weiss et al. (Reference Weiss, Harshman, Inoue, Fraser, Petrov, Ahituv and Gokhman2021) found a fixed mutation in the CRE of SATB2 in modern, but not archaic, humans, which probably reduced SATB2 expression and contributed to the retracted face characteristic of modern humans. In light of the correspondence between the pathological and the evolutionary changes in craniofacial traits following reduced TR gene expression (see above), these findings provide tentative support for the neural crest/domestication syndrome hypothesis as a mechanism underlying HSD: selection for prosociality led to reduced expression of cranial NC-associated genes (e.g. BAZ1B, SATB2), and this NC hypofunction caused, pleiotropically, the craniofacial changes that distinguish modern humans (and which are paralleled both in the associated HI syndromes and in the domestication syndrome of domesticated mammals). Moreover, the neural crest/domestication syndrome hypothesis as a possible mechanism for HSD has also been linked to the evolution of human cognition and language, based on a set of genes (many of them encoding TRs) that are involved both in NC development and in brain globularization (Benítez-Burraco et al., Reference Benítez-Burraco, Theofanopoulou and Boeckx2018; Thomas & Kirby, Reference Thomas and Kirby2018).

Given the appeal of the HSD hypothesis as an explanation for the emergence of human cognitive and social traits, it is not surprising that researchers have used it to understand the origin and persistence of psychiatric disorders. SCZ, for example, is viewed as a hyper-domestication syndrome, resulting from NC hypofunction and hence from the same selective pressures that triggered HSD (Benítez-Burraco et al., Reference Benítez-Burraco, Di Pietro, Barba and Lattanzi2017; Šimić et al., Reference Šimić, Vukić, Kopić, Krsnik and Hof2021). In fact, this idea is only one of several evolutionary hypotheses that trace the existence of psychiatric disorders to the action of natural selection. In contrast, in the next section, we argue that the important role of LOF of TR genes in the etiology of psychiatric disorders is more compatible with the view that these disorders persist despite, not because of, natural selection.

Evolutionary models for the persistence of psychiatric disorders

NDDs such as ASD and SCZ seem to present an evolutionary paradox: they have a strong genetic component and are associated with considerable fitness costs, and yet they are surprisingly prevalent. Given the high heritability and high fitness costs of these disorders, why has natural selection not eliminated the genetic variants that predispose to them? This ‘paradox of common, harmful, heritable mental disorders’ (Keller & Miller, Reference Keller and Miller2006) has long puzzled evolutionary geneticists and psychiatrists alike (e.g. Huxley et al., Reference Huxley, Mayr, Osmond and Hoffer1964). In essence, it boils down to a fundamental question in evolutionary genetics: what causes genetic variation in fitness-related traits to persist, given that selection is expected to minimize it?

Simply put, there are two classes of evolutionary models to explain genetic variation in fitness-related traits and, by extension, in the liability to psychiatric disorders: mutation-selection balance (or more precisely, mutation-selection-drift) and balancing selection (Keller, Reference Keller2018). The mutation-selection-drift model explains genetic variation as a balance between the loss of deleterious alleles through purifying selection and the emergence of new ones through mutation; in addition, random events cause some deleterious alleles with small effects to drift to high frequencies. Under the alternative model, balancing selection, allelic variation at a given locus is maintained by the balance between positive and negative selective forces. In this scenario, risk alleles associated with psychiatric disorders confer some benefit at least under some conditions; hence, selection for the beneficial trait can maintain disorder risk as a by-product. For example, Crow (Reference Crow1997) famously argued that SCZ was ‘the price that Homo sapiens pays for language’. Indeed, the idea that SCZ represents a maladaptive by-product of positive selection during human evolution has been expressed repeatedly (Burns, Reference Burns2004; Crespi et al., Reference Crespi, Summers and Dorus2007; Sikela & Searles Quick, Reference Sikela and Searles Quick2018), and the HSD approach to SCZ is one version of this idea (see above). Similar hypotheses have been suggested for ASD (Baron-Cohen, Reference Baron-Cohen2003; Ploeger & Galis, Reference Ploeger and Galis2011) and bipolar disorder (Wilson, Reference Wilson1998; Greenwood, Reference Greenwood2020). A major difference between mutation-selection balance and balancing selection concerns the role of selection: under mutation-selection balance, mental disorders exist and persist despite natural selection, whereas under balancing selection, they do so because of natural selection (Durisko et al., Reference Durisko, Mulsant, McKenzie and Andrews2016). Which model, then, best explains the paradox of harmful yet common psychiatric disorders?

As our understanding of the genetic architecture of ASD, SCZ and other highly heritable psychiatric disorders is increasing, much research is consistent with a simple mutation-selection-drift model (Keller & Miller, Reference Keller and Miller2006; Keller, Reference Keller2018). First, there is broad support for the important role that rare deleterious mutations with large effect size play in the etiology of NDDs: as described above, a considerable part of ASD and SCZ risk is due to rare heterozygous LOF variants in TR genes that do not tolerate such LOF because of their HI. In addition, disorder risk also depends on a large number of deleterious variants with small effect sizes (background polygenic risk). In fact, the effects of many deleterious mutations will be so small that they may drift to high frequencies. Since the extremely polygenic nature of complex traits (Boyle et al., Reference Boyle, Li and Pritchard2017) implies a large mutational target size of psychiatric disorders, this helps to explain their high prevalence (Keller, Reference Keller2018). Second, there is compelling evidence that, on average, risk alleles associated with these disorders are under weak to strong purifying selection (Rees et al., Reference Rees, Moskvina, Owen, O'Donovan and Kirov2011; Mullins et al., Reference Mullins, Ingason, Porter, Euesden, Gillett, Ólafsson and Stefansson2017; Keller, Reference Keller2018; Pardiñas et al., Reference Pardiñas, Holmans, Pocklington, Escott-Price, Ripke, Carrera and Walters2018; Huang & Siepel, Reference Huang and Siepel2019; Esteller-Cucala et al., Reference Esteller-Cucala, Maceda, Børglum, Demontis, Faraone, Cormand and Lao2020; Rapaport et al., Reference Rapaport, Boisson, Gregor, Béziat, Boisson-Dupuis, Bustamante and Patin2021; Wendt et al., Reference Wendt, Pathak, Overstreet, Tylee, Gelernter, Atkinson and Polimanti2021). These lines of evidence converge in recent population genetic analyses which apply a simple mutation-selection balance model (either as a deterministic approximation or explicitly incorporating drift) to provide direct estimates of the strength of selection against heterozygous LOF in humans (Cassa et al., Reference Cassa, Weghorn, Balick, Jordan, Nusinow, Samocha and Sunyaev2017; Weghorn et al., Reference Weghorn, Balick, Cassa, Kosmicki, Daly, Beier and Sunyaev2019; Agarwal et al., Reference Agarwal I., Myers and & Przeworski2022). Strikingly, negative selection was strongest for LOF-intolerant TR genes (Cassa et al., Reference Cassa, Weghorn, Balick, Jordan, Nusinow, Samocha and Sunyaev2017).

Recent studies provide further interesting insights into the nature of negative selection at LOF-intolerant genes. First, purifying selection not only acts on coding sequences, but also on the promoters of LOF-intolerant genes, presumably to safeguard promoter hypomethylation and to curb coding mutation rates (Boukas et al., Reference Boukas, Razi, Bjornsson and Hansen2022). Second, negative selection at LOF-intolerant genes comes not only in the form of natural selection (against variants increasing pre-reproductive mortality or decreasing fertility), but also in the form of sexual selection. Gardner et al. (Reference Gardner, Neville, Samocha, Barclay, Kolk, Niemi and Hurles2022) found that LOF variants in these genes reduce reproductive success much more in males than in females, and that this reduction is mediated primarily through cognitive and behavioural traits, rendering male carriers of such variants less likely to find mating partners (see also Liu et al., Reference Liu, Akimova, Ding, Jukarainen, Vartiainen, Kiiskinen and Ganna2022).

Lastly, we would like to add two further points. First, while mutation-selection-drift offers a convincing explanation for the persistence of highly heritable psychiatric disorders such as ASD and SCZ, other NDDs, and especially those with lower heritability, might require different types of evolutionary explanation. For example, many symptoms of depression, which has much lower heritability than other psychiatric disorders, can be seen as adaptive defences to minimize fitness loss in the face of adverse life situations (Keller, Reference Keller2018; Syme & Hagen, Reference Syme and Hagen2020). Second, acceptance of mutation-selection-drift as the major explanation of the persistence of psychiatric disorders does not preclude a role of positive selection in providing the conditions for the evolutionary origin of these disorders. One can easily imagine a scenario in which mutations that altered brain development of early H. sapiens were selected for, became fixed, and thus helped to establish human-specific cognitive and behavioural abilities (e.g. mutations that gave rise to HARs). At the same time, these mutations could have, if only indirectly, made human brain development more susceptible to disruption and allowed disorders such as ASD and SCZ to arise (we thank an anonymous reviewer for drawing our attention to such a scenario). However, since these mutations became fixed early on in human evolution, such a scenario does not explain individual differences in genetic risk to psychiatric disorders in modern human populations (Keller, Reference Keller2018).

Taken together, positive selection might have been involved in the emergence of psychiatric disorders, and we also cannot completely rule out balancing selection as a possible explanation for the persistence of some risk alleles associated with these disorders. Nevertheless, mutation-selection-drift is likely to be a more general explanation for the observed genetic variation in disorder risk (Keller, Reference Keller2018). Both the evidence for disease-causing LOF alleles in LOF-intolerant TR genes and for strong purifying selection against such alleles are fully in line with this conclusion.

Predictions and suggestions for future work

Based on the evidence discussed in this article, we hypothesize that human-specific cognitive and social traits evolved due to reduced expression (partial LOF) of haploinsufficient TR genes, possibly through selection for lower aggressiveness and higher sociality. Although it is difficult to test this hypothesis directly, there are several ways to obtain indirect evidence. For example, comparative functional genomics provides a powerful tool to investigate the evolutionary differences in brain and face development between humans and their closest relatives, both living and extinct, thus advancing our understanding of the molecular basis of uniquely human traits (Konopka et al., Reference Konopka, Friedrich, Davis-Turak, Winden, Oldham, Gao and Geschwind2012; Enard, Reference Enard2016; Reilly & Noonan, Reference Reilly and Noonan2016; Kuhlwilm & Boeckx, Reference Kuhlwilm and Boeckx2019). As described above, such comparative studies have already found evidence of reduced expression of TR genes (ARID1B, ARID2, BAZ1B, CUX1, FOXP2, NFIX, SATB2, TCF20) in modern, but not archaic, humans, some of which probably contributed to the retracted face characteristic of present-day humans (Zanella et al., Reference Zanella, Vitriolo, Andirko, Martins, Sturm, O'Rourke and Testa2019; Gokhman et al., Reference Gokhman, Nissim-Rafinia, Agranat-Tamir, Housman, García-Pérez, Lizano and Carmel2020; Weiss et al., Reference Weiss, Harshman, Inoue, Fraser, Petrov, Ahituv and Gokhman2021). In this vein, comparative genomics is likely to provide further evidence of partial LOF of haploinsufficient TR genes as an evolutionary driver of human-specific brain and craniofacial traits.

A particularly powerful experimental model system that has recently emerged to tackle previously intractable questions of human evolution are brain organoids generated from induced pluripotent stem cells (iPSCs) (Dannemann & Gallego Romero, Reference Dannemann and Gallego Romero2022; Mora-Bermúdez et al., Reference Mora-Bermúdez, Taverna and Huttner2022). For example, Benito-Kwiecinski et al. (Reference Benito-Kwiecinski, Giandomenico, Sutcliffe, Riis, Freire-Pritchett, Kelava and Lancaster2021) used brain organoids to show that heterozygous LOF of ZEB2 results in an enlarged neuroepithelium, consistent with brain expansion during human evolution. Hence, future work could study the effects of LOF of other brain-related TR genes in human brain organoids and compare them with organoids derived from other hominids, including both archaic humans and non-human primates. Another recently developed technique to study gene regulatory divergence in the evolution of human brain and face development involves fusing human and chimpanzee iPSCs to generate interspecies hybrids (Agoglia et al., Reference Agoglia, Sun, Birey, Yoon, Miura, Sabatini and Fraser2021; Gokhman et al., Reference Gokhman, Agoglia, Kinnebrew, Gordon, Sun, Bajpai and Fraser2021). Since in hybrids both alleles experience the same environment, including trans-acting regulators, this approach is particularly well-suited for identifying cis-regulatory changes between species. Recently, this method was used to detect reduced CUX1 expression in humans (Starr et al., Reference Starr, Gokhman and Fraser2022), and we expect future studies using interspecies hybrids to reveal further examples of human-specific TR gene downregulation.

The selective forces that drove the evolution of human cognition and sociality are not amenable to direct testing. However, if the HSD hypothesis has some merit, then a comparative approach involving domesticated animal species should provide insights. The hypothesis suggests that the evolution of domesticated species and modern humans was driven by selection against fear and aggression and for tameness/prosociality. For example, humans and domestic dogs have evolved similar social cognitive skills (in both cases adapted for social and communicative interactions with human beings), indicative of convergent evolution (Hare & Tomasello, Reference Hare and Tomasello2005). Therefore, comparative genomics across different domesticated species can increase our understanding of the selective conditions that promoted the evolution of human cognition and sociality. For example, genomic analyses of several domesticated species, including humans, found signs of positive selection in NC-related genes, in line with both the neural crest/domestication syndrome hypothesis and the HSD hypothesis (Theofanopoulou et al., Reference Theofanopoulou, Gastaldon, O'Rourke, Samuels, Messner, Martins and Boeckx2017; Pendleton et al., Reference Pendleton, Shen, Taravella, Emery, Veeramah, Boyko and Kidd2018). Specifically, we predict that NC-related TR genes in domesticated species exhibit reduced expression (partial LOF) compared with their wild ancestors (see also Wilkins et al., Reference Wilkins, Wrangham and Fitch2021). In light of the convergent evolutionary processes among domesticated species and modern humans, such findings would lend further support to the idea that human cognitive and social traits evolved (at least in part) in response to selection for prosociality and against fear and aggression.

Conclusions

Clearly, the question of what makes us human can be approached from different angles (Varki et al., Reference Varki, Geschwind and Eichler2008; Calcagno & Fuentes, Reference Calcagno and Fuentes2012). In this article, we focus on the evolution of human cognition and sociality and approach it from a TR perspective. In particular, we argue that loss of function of genes involved in TR of brain and face development is crucial to understand both the evolution and dysfunction of our unique cognitive and social skills. It is now possible for comparative and experimental studies to put the theory to the test.

Acknowledgements

We thank Cedric Boeckx, Juan Gefaell, Ed Hagen and Adam Wilkins for valuable feedback that improved the manuscript. We apologise to those authors whose work we could not cite due to space constraints.

Author contributions

RZ developed the conceptual framework, reviewed the literature, and led the writing. TU contributed to the writing and editing of the paper.

Financial support

This work was supported by a grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (ZU 405/1-1).

Conflict of interest

The authors are not aware of any conflicts of interest.

Research transparency and reproducibility

N/A

Data availability

N/A

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2022.42.

References

Acosta, S., Sidhaye, J., Fiore, L., Rollan, I., Iacono, G., Misharin, A. V., …, Oliver, G. (2019). A human accelerated region participates in early human forebrain patterning and expansion. bioRxiv, https://doi.org/10.1101/777235.Google Scholar
Agarwal I., Fuller, Myers, Z. L., & Przeworski, S., M. (2022). Relating pathogenic loss-of-function mutations in humans to their evolutionary fitness costs. bioRxiv, https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.08.11.503594.Google Scholar
Agoglia, R. M., Sun, D., Birey, F., Yoon, S.-J., Miura, Y., Sabatini, K., …, Fraser, H. B. (2021). Primate cell fusion disentangles gene regulatory divergence in neurodevelopment. Nature, 592, 421427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albalat, R., & Cañestro, C. (2016). Evolution by gene loss. Nature Reviews Genetics, 17, 379391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arthur, W. (2000). The concept of developmental reprogramming and the quest for an inclusive theory of evolutionary mechanisms. Evolution & Development, 2, 4957.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Babbitt, C. C., Haygood, R., Nielsen, W. J., & Wray, G. A. (2017). Gene expression and adaptive noncoding changes during human evolution. BMC Genomics 18: 435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (2003). The essential difference. Men, women, and the extreme male brain. Penguin.Google Scholar
Bartha, I., di Iulio, J., Venter, J. C., & Telenti, A. (2018). Human gene essentiality. Nature Reviews Genetics, 19, 5162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, C. G., Wilson, G. A., Butcher, L. M., Roos, C., Walter, L., & Beck, S. (2012). Human-specific CpG ‘beacons’ identify loci associated with human-specific traits and disease. Epigenetics, 7, 11881199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benítez-Burraco, A., Di Pietro, L., Barba, M., & Lattanzi, W. (2017). Schizophrenia and human self-domestication: An evolutionary linguistics approach. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 89, 162184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benítez-Burraco, A., Theofanopoulou, C., & Boeckx, C. (2018). Globularization and domestication. Topoi, 37, 265278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benito-Kwiecinski, S., Giandomenico, S. L., Sutcliffe, M., Riis, E. S., Freire-Pritchett, P., Kelava, I., …, Lancaster, M. A. (2021). An early cell shape transition drives evolutionary expansion of the human forebrain. Cell, 184, 20842102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berto, S., Mendizabal, I., Usui, N., Toriumi, K., Chatterjee, P., Douglas, C., …, Konopka, G. (2019). Accelerated evolution of oligodendrocytes in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116, 2433424342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bird, C. P., Stranger, B. E., Liu, M., Thomas, D. J., Ingle, C. E., Beazley, C., …, Dermitzakis, E. T. (2007). Fast-evolving noncoding sequences in the human genome. Genome Biology, 8, R118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boeckx, C. (2017). The language-ready head: Evolutionary considerations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24, 194199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boukas, L., Razi, A., Bjornsson, H. T., & Hansen, K. D. (2022). Natural selection acts on epigenetic marks. bioRxiv, https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.04.187880.Google Scholar
Boyle, E. A., Li, Y. I., & Pritchard, J. K. (2017). An expanded view of complex traits: From polygenic to omnigenic. Cell, 169, 11771186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunet, T., Jech, R., Brugger, M., Kovacs, R., Alhaddad, B., Leszinski, G., …, Wagner, M. (2021). De novo variants in neurodevelopmental disorders – Experiences from a tertiary care center. Clinical Genetics, 100, 1428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burns, J. K. (2004). An evolutionary theory of schizophrenia: Cortical connectivity, metarepresentation, and the social brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 831855.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calcagno, J. M., & Fuentes, A. (2012). What makes us human? Answers from evolutionary anthropology. Evolutionary Anthropology, 21, 182194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caporale, A. L., Gonda, C. M., & Franchini, L. F. (2019). Transcriptional enhancers in the FOXP2 locus underwent accelerated evolution in the human lineage. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 36, 24322450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capra, J. A., Erwin, G. D., McKinsey, G., Rubenstein, J. L. R., & Pollard, K. S. (2013). Many human accelerated regions are developmental enhancers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 368, 20130025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cassa, C. A., Weghorn, D., Balick, D. J., Jordan, D. M., Nusinow, D., Samocha, K. E., …, Sunyaev, S. R. (2017). Estimating the selective effects of heterozygous protein-truncating variants from human exome data. Nature Genetics, 49, 806810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castelijns, B., Baak, M. L., Timpanaro, I. S., Wiggers, C. R. M., Vermunt, M. W., Shang, P., …, Creyghton, M. P. (2020). Hominin-specific regulatory elements selectively emerged in oligodendrocytes and are disrupted in autism patients. Nature Communications, 11, 301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, H., Zhang, Z., Jiang, S., Li, R., Li, W., Zhao, C., …, Bo, X. (2020). New insights on human essential genes based on integrated analysis and the construction of the HEGIAP web-based platform. Briefings in Bioinformatics, 21, 13971410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chin, R., Chang, S. W. C., & Holmes, A. J. (2022). Beyond cortex: The evolution of the human brain. Psychological Review, https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cordero, D. R., Brugmann, S., Chu, Y., Bajpai, R., Jame, M., & Helms, J. A. (2011). Cranial neural crest cells on the move: Their roles in craniofacial development. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A, 155, 270279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crespi, B., Summers, K., & Dorus, S. (2007). Adaptive evolution of genes underlying schizophrenia. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 274, 28012810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (2019). Genomic relationships, novel loci, and pleiotropic mechanisms across eight psychiatric disorders. Cell, 179, 14691482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crow, T. J. (1997). Is schizophrenia the price that Homo sapiens pays for language? Schizophrenia Research, 28, 127141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dannemann, M., & Gallego Romero, I. (2022). Harnessing pluripotent stem cells as models to decipher human evolution. The FEBS Journal, 289, 29923010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deciphering Developmental Disorders Study (2017). Prevalence and architecture of de novo mutations in developmental disorders. Nature, 542, 433438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Rubeis, S., He, X., Goldberg, A. P., Poultney, C. S., Samocha, K., Cicek, A. E., …, Buxbaum, J. D. (2014). Synaptic, transcriptional and chromatin genes disrupted in autism. Nature, 515, 209215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'haene, E., & Vergult, S. (2021). Interpreting the impact of noncoding structural variation in neurodevelopmental disorders. Genetics in Medicine, 23, 3446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Di Pietro, L., Barba, M., Palacios, D., Tiberio, F., Prampolini, C., Baranzini, M., …, Lattanzi, W. (2021). Shaping modern human skull through epigenetic, transcriptional and post-transcriptional regulation of the RUNX2 master bone gene. Scientific Reports, 11, 21316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doan, R. N., Bae, B.-I., Cubelos, B., Chang, C., Hossain, A. A., Al-Saad, S., …, Walsh, C. A. (2016). Mutations in human accelerated regions disrupt cognition and social behavior. Cell, 167, 341354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doan, R. N., Shin, T., & Walsh, C. A. (2018). Evolutionary changes in transcriptional regulation: Insights into human behavior and neurological conditions. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 41, 185206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doostparast Torshizi, A., Armoskus, C., Zhang, H., Forrest, M. P., Zhang, S., Souaiaia, T., …, Wang, K. (2019). Deconvolution of transcriptional networks identifies TCF4 as a master regulator in schizophrenia. Science Advances, 5, eaau4139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dumas, G., Malesys, S., & Bourgeron, T. (2021). Systematic detection of brain protein-coding genes under positive selection during primate evolution and their roles in cognition. Genome Research, 31, 484496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durisko, Z., Mulsant, B. H., McKenzie, K., & Andrews, P. W. (2016). Using evolutionary theory to guide mental health research. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 61, 159165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enard, W. (2016). The molecular basis of human brain evolution. Current Biology, 26, R1109R1117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enard, D., Messer, P. W., & Petrov, D. A. (2014). Genome-wide signals of positive selection in human evolution. Genome Research, 24, 885895.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enard, W., Przeworski, M., Fisher, S. E., Lai, C. S. L., Wiebe, V., Kitano, T., …, Pääbo, S. (2002). Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature, 418, 869872.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erwin, G. D., Oksenberg, N., Truty, R. M., Kostka, D., Murphy, K. K., Ahituv, N., …, Capra, J. A. (2014). Integrating diverse datasets improves developmental enhancer prediction. PLOS Computational Biology, 10, e1003677.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Esteller-Cucala, P., Maceda, I., Børglum, A. D., Demontis, D., Faraone, S. V., Cormand, B., & Lao, O. (2020). Genomic analysis of the natural history of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder using Neanderthal and ancient Homo sapiens samples. Scientific Reports, 10, 8622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freedman, D. J., & Ibos, G. (2018). An integrative framework for sensory, motor, and cognitive functions of the posterior parietal cortex. Neuron, 97, 12191234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, E. J., Neville, M. D. C., Samocha, K. E., Barclay, K., Kolk, M., Niemi, M. E. K., …, Hurles, M. E. (2022). Reduced reproductive success is associated with selective constraint on human genes. Nature, 603, 858863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girard, S. L., Gauthier, J., Noreau, A., Xiong, L., Zhou, S., Jouan, L., …, Rouleau, G. A. (2011). Increased exonic de novo mutation rate in individuals with schizophrenia. Nature Genetics, 43, 860863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girskis, K. M., Stergachis, A. B., DeGennaro, E. M., Doan, R. N., Qian, X., Johnson, M. B., …, Walsh, C. A. (2021). Rewiring of human neurodevelopmental gene regulatory programs by human accelerated regions. Neuron, 109, 32393251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gittelman, R. M., Hun, E., Ay, F., Madeoy, J., Pennacchio, L., Noble, W. S., …, Akey, J. M. (2015). Comprehensive identification and analysis of human accelerated regulatory DNA. Genome Research, 25, 12451255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gokhman, D., Agoglia, R. M., Kinnebrew, M., Gordon, W., Sun, D., Bajpai, V. K., …, Fraser, H. B. (2021). Human–chimpanzee fused cells reveal cis-regulatory divergence underlying skeletal evolution. Nature Genetics, 53, 467476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gokhman, D., Nissim-Rafinia, M., Agranat-Tamir, L., Housman, G., García-Pérez, R., Lizano, E., …, Carmel, L. (2020). Differential DNA methylation of vocal and facial anatomy genes in modern humans. Nature Communications, 11, 1189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Green, R. E., Krause, J., Briggs, A. W., Maricic, T., Stenzel, U., Kircher, M., …, Pääbo, S. (2010). A draft sequence of the Neandertal genome. Science, 328, 710722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwood, T. A. (2020). Creativity and bipolar disorder: A shared genetic vulnerability. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 16, 239264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gulsuner, S., Walsh, T., Watts, A. C., Lee, M. K., Thornton, A. M., Casadei, S., …, McClellan, J. M. (2013). Spatial and temporal mapping of de novo mutations in schizophrenia to a fetal prefrontal cortical network. Cell, 154, 518529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 155186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 439444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haygood, R., Babbitt, C. C., Fedrigo, O., & Wray, G. A. (2010). Contrasts between adaptive coding and noncoding changes during human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 78537857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howrigan, D. P., Rose, S. A., Samocha, K. E., Fromer, M., Cerrato, F., Chen, W. J., …, Neale, B. M. (2020). Exome sequencing in schizophrenia-affected parent–offspring trios reveals risk conferred by protein-coding de novo mutations. Nature Neuroscience, 23, 185193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, G., Li, J., & Wang, G.-Z. (2020). Significant evolutionary constraints on neuron cells revealed by single-cell transcriptomics. Genome Biology and Evolution, 12, 300308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, Y.-F., & Siepel, A. (2019). Estimation of allele-specific fitness effects across human protein-coding sequences and implications for disease. Genome Research, 29, 13101321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubisz, M. J., & Pollard, K. S. (2014). Exploring the genesis and functions of Human Accelerated Regions sheds light on their role in human evolution. Current Opinion in Genetics & Development, 29, 1521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hublin, J.-J., Ben-Ncer, A., Bailey, S. E., Freidline, S. E., Neubauer, S., Skinner, M. M., …, Gunz, P. (2017). New fossils from Jebel Irhoud, Morocco and the pan-African origin of Homo sapiens. Nature, 546, 289292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hussain, I., Raza, R. Z., Ali, S., Abrar, A., & Abbasi, A. A. (2021). Molecular signatures of selection on the human GLI3 associated central nervous system specific enhancers. Development Genes and Evolution, 231, 2132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huxley, J., Mayr, E., Osmond, H., & Hoffer, A. (1964). Schizophrenia as a genetic morphism. Nature, 204, 220221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iakoucheva, L. M., Muotri, A. R., & Sebat, J. (2019). Getting to the cores of autism. Cell, 178, 12871298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iossifov, I., O'Roak, B. J., Sanders, S. J., Ronemus, M., Krumm, N., Levy, D., …, Wigler, M. (2014). The contribution of de novo coding mutations to autism spectrum disorder. Nature, 515, 216221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ji, X., Kember, R. L., Brown, C. D., & Bućan, M. (2016). Increased burden of deleterious variants in essential genes in autism spectrum disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113, 1505415059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jia, P., Chen, X., Fanous, A. H., & Zhao, Z. (2018). Convergent roles of de novo mutations and common variants in schizophrenia in tissue-specific and spatiotemporal co-expression network. Translational Psychiatry, 8, 105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, A. F., Nguyen, H. T., & Veitia, R. A. (2019). Causes and effects of haploinsufficiency. Biological Reviews, 94, 17741785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamm, G. B., Pisciottano, F., Kliger, R., & Franchini, L. F. (2013). The developmental brain gene NPAS3 contains the largest number of accelerated regulatory sequences in the human genome. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 30, 10881102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kataoka, M., Matoba, N., Sawada, T., Kazuno, A.-A., Ishiwata, M., Fujii, K., …, Kato, T. (2016). Exome sequencing for bipolar disorder points to roles of de novo loss-of-function and protein-altering mutations. Molecular Psychiatry, 21, 885893.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, M. C. (2018). Evolutionary perspective on genetic and environmental risk factors for psychiatric disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 14, 471493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, M. C., & Miller, G. (2006). Resolving the paradox of common, harmful, heritable mental disorders: Which evolutionary genetic models work best? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 385404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelley, K. W., & Paşca, S. P. (2022). Human brain organogenesis: Toward a cellular understanding of development and disease. Cell, 185, 4261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khrameeva, E., Kurochkin, I., Han, D., Guijarro, P., Kanton, S., Santel, M., …, Khaitovich, P. (2020). Single-cell-resolution transcriptome map of human, chimpanzee, bonobo, and macaque brains. Genome Research, 30, 776789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, M.-C., & Wilson, A. C. (1975). Evolution at two levels in humans and chimpanzees. Science, 188, 107116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochiyama, T., Ogihara, N., Tanabe, H. C., Kondo, O., Amano, H., Hasegawa, K., …, Akazawa, T. (2018). Reconstructing the Neanderthal brain using computational anatomy. Scientific Reports, 8, 6296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Konopka, G., Friedrich, T., Davis-Turak, J., Winden, K., Oldham, M. C., Gao, F., …, Geschwind, D. H. (2012). Human-specific transcriptional networks in the brain. Neuron, 75, 601617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kosmicki, J. A., Samocha, K. E., Howrigan, D. P., Sanders, S. J., Slowikowski, K., Lek, M., …, Daly, M. J. (2017). Refining the role of de novo protein-truncating variants in neurodevelopmental disorders by using population reference samples. Nature Genetics, 49, 504510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kostka, D., Hubisz, M. J., Siepel, A., & Pollard, K. S. (2012). The role of GC-biased gene conversion in shaping the fastest evolving regions of the human genome. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 29, 10471057.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kozlenkov, A., Vermunt, M. W., Apontes, P., Li, J., Hao, K., Sherwood, C. C., …, Dracheva, S. (2020). Evolution of regulatory signatures in primate cortical neurons at cell-type resolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 117, 2842228432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krumm, N., Turner, T. N., Baker, C., Vives, L., Mohajeri, K., Witherspoon, K., …, Eichler, E. E. (2105). Excess of rare, inherited truncating mutations in autism. Nature Genetics, 47, 582588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhlwilm, M., & Boeckx, C. (2019). A catalog of single nucleotide changes distinguishing modern humans from archaic hominins. Scientific Reports, 9, 8463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lacruz, R. S., Stringer, C. B., Kimbel, W. H., Wood, B., Harvati, K., O'Higgins, P., …, Arsuaga, J.-L. (2019) The evolutionary history of the human face. Nature Ecology and Evolution, 3, 726736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laland, K., & Seed, A. (2021). Understanding human cognitive uniqueness. Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 689716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lalli, M. A., Jang, J., Park, J.-H. C., Wang, Y., Guzman, E., Zhou, H., …, Kosik, K. S. (2016). Haploinsufficiency of BAZ1B contributes to Williams syndrome through transcriptional dysregulation of neurodevelopmental pathways. Human Molecular Genetics, 25, 12941306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaMantia, A.-S. (2020). Why does the face predict the brain? Neural crest induction, craniofacial morphogenesis, and neural circuit development. Frontiers in Physiology, 11, 610970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laugsch, M., Bartusel, M., Rehimi, R., Alirzayeva, H., Karaolidou, A., Crispatzu, G., …, Rada-Iglesias, A. (2019). Modeling the pathological long-range regulatory effects of human structural variation with patient-specific hiPSCs. Cell Stem Cell, 24, 736752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, P. H., Feng, Y.-C. A., & Smoller, J. W. (2021). Pleiotropy and cross-disorder genetics among psychiatric disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 89, 2031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levchenko, A., Kanapin, A., Samsonova, A., & Gainetdinov, R. R. (2018). Human accelerated regions and other human-specific sequence variations in the context of evolution and their relevance for brain development. Genome Biology and Evolution, 10, 166188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindblad-Toh, K., Garber, M., Zuk, O., Lin, M. F., Parker, B. J., Washietl, S., …, Kellis, M. (2011). A high-resolution map of human evolutionary constraint using 29 mammals. Nature, 478, 476482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, A., Akimova, E. T., Ding, X., Jukarainen, S., Vartiainen, P., Kiiskinen, T., …, Ganna, A. (2022). The relationship of major diseases with childlessness: A sibling matched case–control and population register study in Finland and Sweden. medRxiv, https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.03.25.22272822.Google Scholar
Liu, J., Mosti, F., & Silver, D. L. (2021). Human brain evolution: Emerging roles for regulatory DNA and RNA. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 71: 170177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, J., & Robinson-Rechavi, M. (2020). Robust inference of positive selection on regulatory sequences in the human brain. Science Advances, 6, eabc9863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Long, H. K., Osterwalder, M., Welsh, I. C., Hansen, K., Davies, J. O. J., Liu, Y. E., …, Wysocka, J. (2020). Loss of extreme long-range enhancers in human neural crest drives a craniofacial disorder. Cell Stem Cell, 27, 765783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C., Brugha, T. S., Charman, T., Cusack, J., Dumas, G., Frazier, T., …, Veenstra-VanderWeele, J. (2020). Autism spectrum disorder. Nature Reviews Disease Primers, 6, 5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, X., Meng, X., Morris, C. A., & Keating, M. T. (1998). A novel human gene, WSTF, is deleted in Williams syndrome. Genomics, 54, 241249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcucio, R., Hallgrimsson, B., & Young, N. M. (2015). Facial morphogenesis: Physical and molecular interactions between the brain and the face. Current Topics in Developmental Biology, 115, 299320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maricic, T., Günther, V., Georgiev, O., Gehre, S., Ćurlin, M., Schreiweis, C., …, Pääbo, S. (2013). A recent evolutionary change affects a regulatory element in the human FOXP2 gene. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 30, 844852.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCarthy, S. E., Gillis, J., Kramer, M., Lihm, J., Yoon, S., Berstein, Y., …, Corvin, A. (2014). De novo mutations in schizophrenia implicate chromatin remodeling and support a genetic overlap with autism and intellectual disability. Molecular Psychiatry, 19, 652658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minoux, M., & Rijli, F. M. (2010). Molecular mechanisms of cranial neural crest cell migration and patterning in craniofacial development. Development, 137, 26052621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molnár, Z., Clowry, G. J., Sestan, N., Alzu'bi, A., Bakken, T., Hevner, R. F., …, Kriegstein, A. (2019). New insights into the development of the human cerebral cortex. Journal of Anatomy, 235, 432451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monroe, J. G., McKay, J. K., Weigel, D., & Flood, P. J. (2021). The population genomics of adaptive loss of function. Heredity, 126, 383395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mora-Bermúdez, F., Taverna, E., & Huttner, W. B. (2022). From stem and progenitor cells to neurons in the developing neocortex: Key differences among hominids. The FEBS Journal, 289, 15241535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moriano, J., & Boeckx, C. (2020). Modern human changes in regulatory regions implicated in cortical development. BMC Genomics, 21, 304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mossink, B., Negwer, M., Schubert, D., & Nadif Kasri, N. (2021). The emerging role of chromatin remodelers in neurodevelopmental disorders: A developmental perspective. Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences, 78, 25172563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moyses-Oliveira, M., Yadav, R., Erdin, S., & Talkowski, M. E. (2020). New gene discoveries highlight functional convergence in autism and related neurodevelopmental disorders. Current Opinion in Genetics & Development, 65, 195206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mozzi, A., Forni, D., Cagliani, R., Pozzoli, U., Clerici, M., & Sironi, M. (2017). Distinct selective forces and Neanderthal introgression shaped genetic diversity at genes involved in neurodevelopmental disorders. Scientific Reports, 7, 6116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mullins, N., Ingason, A., Porter, H., Euesden, J., Gillett, A., Ólafsson, S., …, Stefansson, K. (2017). Reproductive fitness and genetic risk of psychiatric disorders in the general population. Nature Communications, 8, 15833.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murillo-Rincón, A. P., & Kaucka, M. (2020). Insights into the complexity of craniofacial development from a cellular perspective. Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology, 8, 620735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, A. W. (2020). Can gene-inactivating mutations lead to evolutionary novelty? Current Biology, 30, R465R471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myers, S. M., Challman, T. D., Bernier, R., Bourgeron, T., Chung, W. K., Constantino, J. N., …, Ledbetter, D. H. (2020). Insufficient evidence for ‘autism-specific’ genes. American Journal of Human Genetics, 106, 587595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naqvi, S., Hoskens, H., Wilke, F., Weinberg, S. M., Shaffer, J. R., Walsh, S., …, Claes, P. (2022). Decoding the human face: Progress and challenges in understanding the genetics of craniofacial morphology. Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics, https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-genom-120121-102607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naqvi, S., Sleyp, Y., Hoskens, H., Indencleef, K., Spence, J. P., Bruffaerts, R., …, Claes, P. (2021). Shared heritability of human face and brain shape. Nature Genetics, 53, 830839.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neubauer, S., Hublin, J.-J., & Gunz, P. (2018). The evolution of modern human brain shape. Science Advances, 4, eaao5961.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nord, A. S., Pattabiraman, K., Visel, A., & Rubenstein, J. L. R. (2015). Genomic perspectives of transcriptional regulation in forebrain development. Neuron, 85, 2747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, A. R., Ryu, A. H., Jamieson, K., Thomas, S., Shen, Y., Ahituv, N., …, Reiter, J. F. (2021). A human accelerated region is a Leydig cell GLI2 enhancer that affects male-typical behavior. bioRxiv, https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.01.27.428524.Google Scholar
Oh, H. J., Choi, D., Goh, C. J., & Hahn, Y. (2015). Loss of gene function and evolution of human phenotypes. BMB Reports, 48, 373379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oksenberg, N., Stevison, L., Wall, J. D., & Ahituv, N. (2013). Function and regulation of AUTS2, a gene implicated in autism and human evolution. PLOS Genetics, 9, e1003221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, M. V. (1999). When less is more: Gene loss as an engine of evolutionary change. American Journal of Human Genetics, 64, 1823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pääbo, S. (2014). The human condition – a molecular approach. Cell, 157, 216226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pardiñas, A. F., Holmans, P., Pocklington, A. J., Escott-Price, V., Ripke, S., Carrera, N., …, Walters, J. T. R. (2018). Common schizophrenia alleles are enriched in mutation-intolerant genes and in regions under strong background selection. Nature Genetics, 50, 381389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parenti, I., Rabaneda, L. G., Schoen, H., & Novarino, G. (2020). Neurodevelopmental disorders: From genetics to functional pathways. Trends in Neurosciences, 43, 608621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pattabiraman, K., Muchnik, S. K., & Sestan, N. (2020). The evolution of the human brain and disease susceptibility. Current Opinion in Genetics & Development, 65, 9197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pembroke, W. G., Hartl, C. L., & Geschwind, D. H. (2021). Evolutionary conservation and divergence of the human brain transcriptome. Genome Biology, 22, 52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pendleton, A. L., Shen, F., Taravella, A. M., Emery, S., Veeramah, K. R., Boyko, A. R., & Kidd, J. M. (2018). Comparison of village dog and wolf genomes highlights the role of the neural crest in dog domestication. BMC Biology, 16, 64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peoples, R. J., Cisco, M. J., Kaplan, P., & Francke, U. (1998). Identification of the WBSCR9 gene, encoding a novel transcriptional regulator, in the Williams–Beuren syndrome deletion at 7q11.23. Cytogenetics and Cell Genetics, 82, 238246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pereira-Pedro, A. S., Bruner, E., Gunz, P. & Neubauer, S. (2020). A morphometric comparison of the parietal lobe in modern humans and Neanderthals. Journal of Human Evolution, 142, 102770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peyrégne, S., Boyle, M. J., Dannemann, M., & Prüfer, K. (2017). Detecting ancient positive selection in humans using extended lineage sorting. Genome Research, 27, 15631572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ploeger, A., & Galis, F. (2011). Evolutionary approaches to autism – an overview and integration. McGill Journal of Medicine, 13, 3843.Google Scholar
Pollard, K. S., Salama, S. R., King, B., Kern, A. D., Dreszer, T., Katzman, S., …, Haussler, D. (2006). Forces shaping the fastest evolving regions in the human genome. PLOS Genetics, 2, e168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prabhakar, S., Noonan, J. P., Pääbo, S., & Rubin, E. M. (2006). Accelerated evolution of conserved noncoding sequences in humans. Science, 314, 786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prescott, S. L., Srinivasan, R., Marchetto, M. C., Grishina, I., Narvaiza, I., Selleri, L., …, Wysocka, J. (2015). Enhancer divergence and cis-regulatory evolution in the human and chimp neural crest. Cell, 163, 6883.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Priolo, M., Schanze, D., Tatton-Brown, K., Mulder, P. A., Tenorio, J., Kooblall, K., …, Hennekam, R. C. (2018). Further delineation of Malan syndrome. Human Mutation, 39, 12261237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapaport, F., Boisson, B., Gregor, A., Béziat, V., Boisson-Dupuis, S., Bustamante, J., …, Patin, E. (2021). Negative selection on human genes underlying inborn errors depends on disease outcome and both the mode and mechanism of inheritance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 118, e2001248118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rees, E., Creeth, H. D. J., Hwu, H.-G., Chen, W. J., Tsuang, M., Glatt, S. J., …, O'Donovan, M. C. (2021). Schizophrenia, autism spectrum disorders and developmental disorders share specific disruptive coding mutations. Nature Communications, 12, 5353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rees, E., Moskvina, V., Owen, M. J., O'Donovan, M. C., & Kirov, G. (2011). De novo rates and selection of schizophrenia-associated copy number variants. Biological Psychiatry, 70, 11091114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reilly, S. K., & Noonan, J. P. (2016). Evolution of gene regulation in humans. Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics, 17, 4567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samocha, K. E., Robinson, E. B., Sanders, S. J., Stevens, C., Sabo, A., McGrath, L. M., …, Daly, M. J. (2014). A framework for the interpretation of de novo mutation in human disease. Nature Genetics, 46, 944950.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sánchez-Gaya, V., Mariner-Fauli, M., & Rada-Iglesias, A. (2020). Rare or overlooked? Structural disruption of regulatory domains in human neurocristopathies. Frontiers in Genetics, 11, 688.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sánchez-Villagra, M. R., & van Schaik, C. P. (2019). Evaluating the self-domestication hypothesis of human evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 28, 133143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sato, T. S., Handa, A., Priya, S., Watal, P., Becker, R. M., & Sato, Y. (2019). Neurocristopathies: Enigmatic appearances of neural crest cell-derived abnormalities. Radiographics, 39, 20852102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Satterstrom, F. K., Kosmicki, J. A., Wang, J., Breen, M. S., De Rubeis, S., An, J.-Y., …, Buxbaum, J. D. (2020). Large-scale exome sequencing study implicates both developmental and functional changes in the neurobiology of autism. Cell, 180, 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sestan, N., & State, M. W. (2018). Lost in translation: Traversing the complex path from genomics to therapeutics in autism spectrum disorder. Neuron, 100, 406423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Short, P. J., McRae, J. F., Gallone, G., Sifrim, A., Won, H., Geschwind, D. H., …, Hurles, M. E. (2018). De novo mutations in regulatory elements in neurodevelopmental disorders. Nature, 555, 611616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sikela, J. M., & Searles Quick, V. B. (2018). Genomic trade-offs: Are autism and schizophrenia the steep price of the human brain? Human Genetics, 137, 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Šimić, G., Vukić, V., Kopić, J., Krsnik, Ž., & Hof, P. R. (2021). Molecules, mechanisms, and disorders of self-domestication: Keys for understanding emotional and social communication from an evolutionary perspective. Biomolecules, 11, 2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokolov, A. A., Miall, R. C., & Ivry, R. B. (2017). The cerebellum: Adaptive prediction for movement and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 313332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Somel, M., Franz, H., Yan, Z., Lorenc, A., Guo, S., Giger, T., …, Khaitovich, P. (2009). Transcriptional neoteny in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 57435748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sousa, A. M. M., Meyer, K. A., Santpere, G., Gulden, F. O., & Sestan, N. (2017a). Evolution of the human nervous system function, structure, and development. Cell, 170, 226247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sousa, A. M. M., Zhu, Y., Raghanti, M. A., Kitchen, R. R., Onorati, M., Tebbenkamp, A. T. N., …, Sestan, N. (2017b). Molecular and cellular reorganization of neural circuits in the human lineage. Science, 358, 10271032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Starr, A. L., Gokhman, D., & Fraser, H. B. (2022). Accounting for cis-regulatory constraint prioritizes genes likely to affect species-specific traits. bioRxiv, https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.03.29.486301.Google Scholar
Sullivan, J. M., De Rubeis, S., & Schaefer, A. (2019). Convergence of spectrums: Neuronal gene network states in autism spectrum disorder. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 59, 102111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Syme, K. L., & Hagen, E. H. (2020). Mental health is biological health: Why tackling ‘diseases of the mind’ is an imperative for biological anthropology in the 21st century. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 171, 87117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taverna, E., Götz, M., & Huttner, W. B. (2014). The cell biology of neurogenesis: Toward an understanding of the development and evolution of the neocortex. Annual Review of Cell and Developmental Biology, 30, 465502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
The Brainstorm Consortium (2018). Analysis of shared heritability in common disorders of the brain. Science, 360, eaap8757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Theofanopoulou, C., Gastaldon, S., O'Rourke, T., Samuels, B. D., Messner, A., Martins, P. T., …, Boeckx, C. (2017). Self-domestication in Homo sapiens: Insights from comparative genomics. PLOS ONE, 12, e0185306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, J., & Kirby, S. (2018). Self domestication and the evolution of language. Biology & Philosophy, 33, 9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trevino, A. E., Müller, F., Andersen, J., Sundaram, L., Kathiria, A., Shcherbina, A., …, Greenleaf, W. J. (2021). Chromatin and gene-regulatory dynamics of the developing human cerebral cortex at single-cell resolution. Cell, 184, 50535069.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, T. N., & Eichler, E. E. (2019). The role of de novo noncoding regulatory mutations in neurodevelopmental disorders. Trends in Neurosciences, 42, 115127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Varki, A., Geschwind, D. H., & Eichler, E. E. (2008). Explaining human uniqueness: Genome interactions with environment, behaviour and culture. Nature Reviews Genetics 9, 749763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vega-Lopez, G. A., Cerrizuela, S., Tribulo, C., & Aybar, M. J. (2018). Neurocristopathies: New insights 150 years after the neural crest discovery. Developmental Biology, 444, S110S143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veitia, R. A. (2002). Exploring the etiology of haploinsufficiency. Bioessays, 24, 175184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wainberg, M., Merico, D., Huguet, G., Zarrei, M., Jacquemont, S., Scherer, S. W., & Tripathy, S. J. (2022). Deletion of loss-of-function-intolerant genes and risk of 5 psychiatric disorders. JAMA Psychiatry, 79, 7881.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weghorn, D., Balick, D. J., Cassa, C., Kosmicki, J. A., Daly, M. J., Beier, D. R., & Sunyaev, S. R. (2019). Applicability of the mutation-selection balance model to population genetics of heterozygous protein-truncating variants in humans. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 36, 17011710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, C. V., Harshman, L., Inoue, F., Fraser, H. B., Petrov, D. A., Ahituv, N., & Gokhman, D. (2021). The cis-regulatory effects of modern human-specific variants. eLife, 10, e63713.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wendt, F. R., Pathak, G. A., Overstreet, C., Tylee, D. S., Gelernter, J., Atkinson, E. G., & Polimanti, R. (2021). Characterizing the effect of background selection on the polygenicity of brain-related traits. Genomics, 113, 111119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, J. D., Indencleef, K., Naqvi, S., Eller, R. J., Hoskens, H., Roosenboom, J., …, Claes, P. (2021). Insights into the genetic architecture of the human face. Nature Genetics, 53, 4553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkie, A. O. M. (1994). The molecular basis of genetic dominance. Journal of Medical Genetics, 31, 8998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkins, A. S. (2017). Making faces: The evolutionary origins of the human face. Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Wilkins, A. S. (2020). A molecular investigation of human self-domestication. Trends in Genetics, 36, 227228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkins, A. S., Wrangham, R. W., & Fitch, W. T. (2014). The ‘domestication syndrome’ in mammals: A unified explanation based on neural crest cell behavior and genetics. Genetics, 197, 795808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkins, A. S., Wrangham, R. W., & Fitch, W. T. (2021). The neural crest/domestication syndrome hypothesis, explained: Reply to Johnsson, Henriksen, and Wright. Genetics, 219, iyab098.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, D. R. (1998). Evolutionary epidemiology and manic depression. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 71, 375395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Won, H., Huang, J., Opland, C. K., Hartl, C. L., & Geschwind, D. H. (2019). Human evolved regulatory elements modulate genes involved in cortical expansion and neurodevelopmental disease susceptibility. Nature Communications, 10, 2396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zanella, M., Vitriolo, A., Andirko, A., Martins, P. T., Sturm, S., O'Rourke, T., …, Testa, G. (2019). Dosage analysis of the 7q11.23 Williams region identifies BAZ1B as a major human gene patterning the modern human face and underlying self-domestication. Science Advances, 5, eaaw7908.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, J., Park, C. Y., Theesfeld, C. L., Wong, A. K., Yuan, Y., Scheckel, C., …, Troyanskaya, O. G. (2019). Whole-genome deep-learning analysis identifies contribution of noncoding mutations to autism risk. Nature Genetics, 51, 973980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zug, R. (2022). Developmental disorders caused by haploinsufficiency of transcriptional regulators: A perspective based on cell fate determination. Biology Open, 11, bio058896.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Heterozygous loss of function (LOF) of haploinsufficient genes involved in transcriptional regulation (TR) of brain and face development leads to neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs) and neurocristopathies (NCPs). Only a subset of NDDs are shown, including attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), bipolar disorder (BD), intellectual disability (ID) and schizophrenia (SCZ). CRE, cis-regulatory element.

Figure 1

Table 1. Haploinsufficient transcriptional regulation (TR) genes implicated both in neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs)/neurocristopathies (NCPs) and in the evolution of human brain and/or face development

Figure 2

Figure 2. The parallelism between the evolutionary and the pathological consequences of reduced TR gene expression for craniofacial development. The figure shows several craniofacial features of archaic humans, healthy anatomically modern humans (AMHs), and AMHs with a haploinsufficiency-associated disorder. Both the evolutionary changes from archaic humans to AMHs and the pathological changes from healthy AMHs to individuals with the disorder are due to reduced TR gene expression. Adapted from Gokhman et al. (2020).

Supplementary material: File

Zug and Uller supplementary material

Zug and Uller supplementary material

Download Zug and Uller supplementary material(File)
File 22.9 KB