Hostname: page-component-cc8bf7c57-5wl6q Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-12T01:31:24.900Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Hunter–Gatherer children's close-proximity networks: Similarities and differences with cooperative and communal breeding systems

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 January 2024

Nikhil Chaudhary*
Affiliation:
Leverhulme Centre for Human Evolutionary Studies, Department of Archaeology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 1QH, UK Department of Anthropology, University College London, London WC1H 0BW, UK
Abigail E. Page
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University College London, London WC1H 0BW, UK Department of Population Health, London School of Hygiene and Tropical and Medicine, London WC1E 7HT, UK
Gul Deniz Salali
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University College London, London WC1H 0BW, UK
Mark Dyble
Affiliation:
Leverhulme Centre for Human Evolutionary Studies, Department of Archaeology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 1QH, UK Department of Anthropology, University College London, London WC1H 0BW, UK
Daniel Major-Smith
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University College London, London WC1H 0BW, UK Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 2BN, UK
Andrea B. Migliano
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University College London, London WC1H 0BW, UK Department of Anthropology, University of Zurich, 8057 Zürich, Switzerland
Lucio Vinicius
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University College London, London WC1H 0BW, UK Department of Anthropology, University of Zurich, 8057 Zürich, Switzerland
James Thompson
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University College London, London WC1H 0BW, UK
Sylvain Viguier
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University College London, London WC1H 0BW, UK
*
Corresponding author: Nikhil Chaudhary; Email: nc542@cam.ac.uk

Abstract

Among vertebrates, allomothering (non-maternal care) is classified as cooperative breeding (help from sexually mature non-breeders, usually close relatives) or communal breeding (shared care between multiple breeders who are not necessarily related). Humans have been described with both labels, most frequently as cooperative breeders. However, few studies have quantified the relative contributions of allomothers according to whether they are (a) sexually mature and reproductively active and (b) related or unrelated. We constructed close-proximity networks of Agta and BaYaka hunter–gatherers. We used portable remote-sensing devices to quantify the proportion of time children under the age of 4 spent in close proximity to different categories of potential allomother. Both related and unrelated, and reproductively active and inactive, campmates had substantial involvement in children's close-proximity networks. Unrelated campmates, siblings and subadults were the most involved in both populations, whereas the involvement of fathers and grandmothers was the most variable between the two populations. Finally, the involvement of sexually mature, reproductively inactive adults was low. Where possible, we compared our findings with studies of other hunter–gatherer societies, and observed numerous consistent trends. Based on our results we discuss why hunter–gatherer allomothering cannot be fully characterised as cooperative or communal breeding.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Social media summary: Are we really cooperative breeders? Hunter–gatherer cooperation in childcare is unique from a cross-species perspective.

Introduction

Human reproduction is remarkably efficient, with women in natural fertility populations producing highly dependent large-brained offspring at higher fertility and survival rates than other apes (Kramer, Reference Kramer2010). It is well established that rearing multiple expensive offspring simultaneously is facilitated by extensive assistance from non-maternal helpers or ‘allomothers’ (Hrdy, Reference Hrdy2009; Sear & Mace, Reference Sear and Mace2008). Allomothering occurs in a diverse range of taxa and takes many forms such as food provisioning, babysitting, transporting and incubation (Clutton-Brock, Reference Clutton-Brock2002; Griesser et al., Reference Griesser, Drobniak, Nakagawa and Botero2017; Riehl, Reference Riehl2013). Among vertebrates, it is classified into systems of cooperative or communal breeding according to whether helpers are reproductively active, and the underlying evolutionary mechanisms (see next section) (Burkart et al., Reference Burkart, Van Schaik and Griesser2017; Lukas and Clutton-Brock, Reference Lukas and Clutton-Brock2012a). Humans have been described with both labels, and very frequently as cooperative breeders (Hrdy, Reference Hrdy2007; Kramer, Reference Kramer2010; Wu et al., Reference Wu, He, Deng, Wang, Mace, Ji and Tao2013). However, numerous scholars have challenged the applicability of these terms to our species (Bogin et al., Reference Bogin, Bragg and Kuzawa2014; Burkart et al., Reference Burkart, Van Schaik and Griesser2017; Strassmann and Kurapati, Reference Strassmann and Kurapati2010). Burkart et al. (Reference Burkart, Van Schaik and Griesser2017: 1) emphasise that to understand evolutionary pathways to cooperation we must avoid “looking for unity in diversity”. Therefore, it is important to establish the similarities and differences between allomothering across species, including humans. Below we outline how non-human allomothering systems are classified, we then briefly review existing research on hunter–gatherer allomothering, highlight obstacles to its classification, and explain how we aim to address these in the present study of Agta and BaYaka hunter–gatherers.

Classification of allomothering systems

Cooperative breeding

The specificity of criteria for the classification of cooperative breeding varies across the literature. A liberal definition is that it refers to social systems in which non-breeding allomothers help a breeding female or a male–female breeding pair in raising offspring (Burkart et al., Reference Burkart, Van Schaik and Griesser2017). However, most researchers restrict the use of the term to cases in which non-breeding allomothers are sexually mature, which begs the question of why they would remain non-reproductive and help another female raise her offspring (Emlen, Reference Emlen1982; Lukas & Clutton-Brock, Reference Lukas and Clutton-Brock2012b); we refer to this as the traditional definition. This occurs in less than 5% of mammals (Burkart et al., Reference Burkart, Van Schaik and Griesser2017; Lukas & Clutton-Brock, Reference Lukas and Clutton-Brock2016), and approximately 10% of bird species (Cockburn, Reference Cockburn2006).

The dominant explanation for cooperative breeding is that owing to ecological constraints on personal reproduction, e.g. low availability of suitable nesting sites, or benefits of philopatry, e.g. reduced predation, offspring delay dispersal from their natal group and become ‘helpers at the nest’ (Arnold & Owens, Reference Arnold and Owens1998; Emlen, Reference Emlen1982; Koenig et al., Reference Koenig, Pitelka, Carmen, Mumme and Stanback1992). There, they gain indirect fitness benefits by helping their parents raise the next brood/litter (Cornwallis et al., Reference Cornwallis, West and Griffin2009; Griesser et al., Reference Griesser, Drobniak, Nakagawa and Botero2017; Hatchwell, Reference Hatchwell2009; Lukas & Clutton-Brock, Reference Lukas and Clutton-Brock2012a). Allomothering evolves when the benefits that can be conferred to breeders are high, for example in unpredictable/harsh environments as seen in meerkats and babbling starlings, or in species with large litter sizes such as naked mole rats (Griesser et al., Reference Griesser, Drobniak, Nakagawa and Botero2017; Lukas & Clutton-Brock, Reference Lukas and Clutton-Brock2012b, Reference Lukas and Clutton-Brock2016; Rubenstein & Lovette, Reference Rubenstein and Lovette2007).

Mechanisms beyond kin selection have been shown to operate in some, but substantially fewer, cooperative breeders (Kokko et al., Reference Kokko, Johnstone and Wright2002; Piper et al., Reference Piper, Parker and Rabenold1995; Sherley, Reference Sherley1990). However, numerous phylogenetic analyses have demonstrated that the evolution of cooperative breeding requires high relatedness between potential providers and recipients of allomothering, and as such it is restricted to socially monogamous species with low promiscuity (Cornwallis et al., Reference Cornwallis, West and Griffin2009; Lukas & Clutton-Brock, Reference Lukas and Clutton-Brock2012a). Other drivers of helping may be supplementary to kin selection or contribute to the maintenance of the system once it has evolved.

Communal breeding

Communal breeding occurs when multiple breeding females, or in some cases multiple breeding male–female pairs, share care and/or provisioning responsibilities (Lukas & Clutton-Brock, Reference Lukas and Clutton-Brock2012a). This system is observed in a small minority of mammals, such as the banded mongoose, and birds such as the greater anis (Gilchrist et al., Reference Gilchrist, Otali and Mwanguhya2004; Pusey & Packer, Reference Pusey and Packer1994; Riehl, Reference Riehl2013).

The evolution of these systems does not rely on kin selection; co-breeding females may be unrelated and allomothering is often driven by reciprocity or mutualism (Baden et al., Reference Baden, Wright, Louis and Bradley2013; Bebbington et al., Reference Bebbington, Fairfield, Spurgin, Kingma, Dugdale, Komdeur and Richardson2018). Mothers often crèche their young together. This can provide greater protection from predators or infanticide, as seen in African lions, or allow mothers to increase foraging time if they take turns in caring for or feeding the crèche, as observed in free-living house mice (Auclair et al., Reference Auclair, König, Ferrari, Perony and Lindholm2014; Packer et al., Reference Packer, Pusey and Eberly2001). Communal breeding can also increase foraging efficiency, for instance female sperm whales share information and engage in cooperative hunting (Rendell et al., Reference Rendell, Cantor, Gero, Whitehead and Mann2019).

In summary, the most fundamental difference between cooperative and communal breeders is that allomothers are non-breeders in the former and breeders in the latter. This distinction is very important as it is only in cooperative breeding that it is necessary to explain why sexually mature allomothers do not reproduce even though they are physiologically capable of doing so. Accordingly, the evolution of cooperative breeding relies on kin selection. This is not the case in communal breeders as cooperation between unrelated individuals is frequent and driven by reciprocity and mutualism. Thus, identifying allomothers’ reproductive status and relatedness to the recipients of care is key for characterising an allomothering system.

Humans are frequently labelled as cooperative breeders since allomothering is normative in natural fertility populations (Burkart et al., Reference Burkart, Hrdy and Van Schaik2009; Hrdy, Reference Hrdy2007). However, many researchers would agree that allomothering in the vast majority of human societies does not neatly fit the traditional definition of cooperative breeding. The purpose of inter-specific classification schemes is to identify similarities across taxa which can then aid in uncovering fundamental evolutionary processes. Here, we aim to describe hunter–gatherer allomothering systems in a way that highlights the extent to which they resemble and differ from cooperative and communal breeding systems.

Hunter–gatherer allomothering

Given that our species lived as hunter–gatherers for the majority of our evolutionary history, studying contemporary hunter–gatherers can be informative for our understanding of the evolution of human allomothering systems. We emphasise that contemporary hunter–gatherers are modern populations who, just like any other extant human population, have undergone millennia of their own biological and cultural evolution; they are not ‘living fossils’ (Kelly, Reference Kelly2013). It is only because their mode of subsistence overlaps with that of ancestral populations that they can offer insight into human prehistory.

Much of the hunter–gatherer research has focused on the behaviour of fathers or post-reproductive grandmothers, emphasising their role as the primary allomother (Hawkes et al., Reference Hawkes, O'Connell, Blurton Jones, Alvarez and Charnov1998, Reference Hawkes, O'Connell and Blurton Jones2014; Marlowe, Reference Marlowe2003; Wood & Marlowe, Reference Wood and Marlowe2014). The traditional pair-bonding model argues that biparental care was fundamental for child survival over our species’ evolutionary history (Lovejoy, Reference Lovejoy1981), and highlights that in many foraging societies males produce the majority of calories (Hill & Hurtado, Reference Hill and Hurtado1996; Kaplan et al., Reference Kaplan, Hill, Lancaster and Hurtado2000; Marlowe, Reference Marlowe2003; Wood & Marlowe, Reference Wood and Marlowe2014). Fathers can also be important providers of hands-on childcare, for instance paternal care among Aka foragers is the highest recorded in any human society (Hewlett, Reference Hewlett1991). However, the role of fathers is contested and male hunting has been argued to be a form of ‘showing off’ phenotypic quality rather than paternal investment (Hawkes et al., Reference Hawkes, O'Connell and Blurton Jones2001, Reference Hawkes, O'Connell and Blurton Jones2014).

Many anthropologists have instead emphasised the primary importance of grandmothers as allomothers. For instance, Hadza grandmothers spend an extensive amount of time foraging, and their foraging effort is positively associated with their grandchildren's health (Hawkes et al., Reference Hawkes, O'Connell and Blurton Jones1997). Similarly, Martu aborigines are described as occupying a ‘grandmaternal niche’, and grandmothers provide considerably more childcare than any other allomother (Scelza, Reference Scelza2009). Grandmothers may also confer benefits via their ecological knowledge and expertise. For example, we found that among the BaYaka, medicinal plant knowledge increases with age and tends to be shared within families (Salali et al., Reference Salali, Chaudhary, Thompson, Grace, van der Burgt, Dyble and Migliano2016, Reference Salali, Dyble, Chaudhary, Sikka, Derkx, Keestra and Migliano2020). Some scholars also endorse the grandmother hypothesis, which argues that the inclusive fitness benefits of grandmothering are sufficiently large to have driven the evolution of the post-reproductive lifespan (Hawkes & Coxworth, Reference Hawkes and Coxworth2013).

The contribution of juveniles has also been studied, and some similar trends have been reported in several societies (Helfrecht et al., Reference Helfrecht, Roulette, Lane, Sintayehu and Meehan2020; Ivey, Reference Ivey2000; Kramer, Reference Kramer2005; Page et al., Reference Page, Emmott, Dyble, Smith, Chaudhary, Viguier and Migliano2021). Older children may supervise young in the context of mixed-age ‘playgroups’ (Lew-Levy et al., Reference Lew-Levy, Reckin, Lavi, Cristóbal-Azkarate and Ellis-Davies2017; Page et al., Reference Page, Emmott, Dyble, Smith, Chaudhary, Viguier and Migliano2021; Salali et al., Reference Salali, Chaudhary, Bouer, Thompson, Vinicius and Migliano2019). Juveniles also forage for foods such as fruits and plants which they have sufficient strength and skill to acquire (Crittenden et al., Reference Crittenden, Conklin-Brittain, Zes, Schoeninger and Marlowe2013; Salali et al., Reference Salali, Chaudhary, Bouer, Thompson, Vinicius and Migliano2019); this can help with provisioning when they target foods that adults do not (Kramer, Reference Kramer2014). Finally, they often play an important role in food processing, domestic tasks and collecting firewood and water; and in turn, mothers are able to engage in other activities (Hawkes et al., Reference Hawkes, O'Connell and Blurton Jones1997; Lew-Levy et al., Reference Lew-Levy, Reckin, Kissler, Pretelli, Boyette and Crittenden2022).

Requirements for classification

Studying the behaviour of a specific single category of allomother – such as fathers, grandmothers, or juveniles – when they are co-residing with a child overlooks the frequency with which they are actually living with an average child and also other potential sources of care. It is necessary to account for the presence and number of individuals in each category of potential allomother, i.e. how many individuals there are in each category living in the same camp as a child; herein, we refer to this as availability. For instance, if an average co-resident sibling provides much more care than an average unrelated campmate, we might infer that the allomothering system is largely driven by kin selection and resembles a ‘helpers at the nest’ system. However, if the average child has many more unrelated campmates than siblings, it may be that the aggregate contribution of all unrelated campmates combined is greater than that of all siblings combined. In this case the aforementioned inference would be misleading. Therefore, the taking of a child's-eye perspective, rather than that of any specific category of allomother, is required for charactering the allomothering system as a whole, and in turn for inter-specific comparison and classification.

Some child's-eye studies of allomaternal networks have been conducted (Helfrecht et al., Reference Helfrecht, Roulette, Lane, Sintayehu and Meehan2020; Marlowe, Reference Marlowe, Hewlett and Lamb2005b; Scelza, Reference Scelza2009). However, comparing hunter–gatherer allomothering with cooperative and communally breeding species requires identifying the relative contributions of all allomothers according to their (a) reproductive status and (b) relatedness to the recipients of care.

  1. (1) Few studies quantify the relative childcare contributions of subadult (not just siblings), adult, and post-reproductive (not just grandmothers) helpers; see Helfrecht et al. (Reference Helfrecht, Roulette, Lane, Sintayehu and Meehan2020) for an exception. We know of no study which presents all of these data and distinguishes between reproductively active and reproductively inactive adults, here we are using the term adult to refer to individuals who are of reproductive age. Reproductively inactive adults may constitute a non-trivial constituent of a child's allomaternal network (Ivey, Reference Ivey2000); and importantly, it is only their help that adheres to the traditional definition of cooperative breeding. Equally, communal breeding is defined by sharing of care among reproductively active adults, not adults in general.

  2. (2) There are numerous studies highlighting that food transfers between unrelated campmates are important for reducing unpredictability in resource access, and for provisioning reproductively active women. This has been demonstrated in African, Asian and South American hunter–gatherers (Dyble et al., Reference Dyble, Thompson, Smith, Salali, Chaudhary, Page and Migliano2016; Gurven et al., Reference Gurven, Hill, Kaplan, Hurtado and Lyles2000; Hill & Hurtado, Reference Hill and Hurtado2009). Surprisingly, very few studies have focused on the childcare contribution of unrelated helpers. Helpers who are not fathers, grandmothers or siblings are often lumped together as ‘other helpers’, therefore it is not possible to isolate the contribution of unrelated campmates. Examining the relative contribution of related and unrelated helpers is important for inter-specific comparison since the evolution of cooperative, but not communal, breeding is associated with high relatedness between potential allomothers and young. Existing childcare studies in which data on the contribution of unrelated campmates are presented, or can be calculated, are included in a summary table in our Results section (Table 6).

The current study

Here we examine the allomothering systems of two hunter–gatherer societies – the Agta from the Philippines and the BaYaka from Congo. We used remote-sensing devices (motes) to measure the amount of time potential allomothers spend in close proximity to infants and toddlers. Our fundamental aim is to characterise children's close-proximity networks in a manner that adds to our understanding of how hunter–gatherer allomothering resembles/differs from cooperative and communal breeding systems. As such, we take a child's-eye perspective and quantify the time spent in close proximity to all categories of potential allomother. Categories reflect potential allomothers’ reproductive status, and their relationship with a child including whether they are related or unrelated. Importantly, we quantify their involvement in the close-proximity networks at the individual and aggregate (time spent by all members of the category) levels, to account for helper availability.

The role of close proximity in allocare systems

Allomothering is typically divided into direct and indirect investment. The former is also referred to as allocare and includes all care that requires close proximity to an infant, and the latter to other forms of help such as territory defence or resource provisioning (Kleiman & Malcolm, Reference Kleiman, Malcolm, Gubernick and Klopfer1981). Studies of direct investment, herein childcare, often distinguish between care that is high cost/demand/investment vs. low cost/demand/investment. The specific term used varies by study; however, the distinctions being made are similar. High investment care involves focussed attention and interaction such as feeding or grooming, whereas low investment care does not necessarily require interaction or much energy expenditure e.g. supervision (Meehan, Reference Meehan2005; Scelza, Reference Scelza2009). Proximity is frequently measured in anthropological studies of childcare since it is a requirement for both high and low investment childcare (Hewlett & Lamb, Reference Hewlett, Lamb, Keller and Poortinga2009; Konner, Reference Konner2018; Marlowe, Reference Marlowe, Hewlett and Lamb2005b; Page et al., Reference Page, Emmott, Dyble, Smith, Chaudhary, Viguier and Migliano2021). It is sometimes highlighted as a form of low investment care in and of itself since it facilitates supervision of a child and the potential to intervene if required (Meehan et al., Reference Meehan, Quinlan and Malcom2013, Reference Meehan, Lackey, Hagen, Williams, Roulette, Helfrecht and McGuire2018; Page et al., Reference Page, Thomas, Smith, Dyble, Viguier, Chaudhary and Migliano2019). In turn, it provides greater opportunity for mothers to rest or engage in labour activities, such as foraging or food processing, without putting their child at risk (Page et al. Reference Page, Thomas, Smith, Dyble, Viguier, Chaudhary and Migliano2019). Indeed, this is one of the primary functions of what we call ‘babysitting’ in industrialised societies. Ethologists have noted that even when babysitting involves little active or costly care, it can confer major benefits in communally breeding species (Burkart et al., Reference Burkart, Van Schaik and Griesser2017; Lewis & Pusey, Reference Lewis, Pusey, Solomon and French1997).

The motes provide valuable high-resolution data on the time each potential allomother spends in close proximity to a child. However, it is important to acknowledge that we cannot discern the amount of high investment care a potential allomother may be providing, nor how much of the time they actually spend intervening as opposed to simply being in a position to intervene. In the Methods section we discuss the benefits and limitations of the motes in more detail.

We predict that Agta and BaYaka children rely on many allomothers, differing in reproductive status and relatedness, rather than any single category of helper; and as such, hunter–gatherers cannot be classified as communal or cooperative breeders. We expect that those categories of potential allomother who are the most available spend the most time in close proximity to children at the aggregate level. Different categories of helpers are available as a result of different forces – such as life-history schedules and residence patterns – some of which have parallels with the forces producing helper availability in cooperative and communally breeding non-human animals; we consider these in our discussion. Accordingly, by identifying the involvement of different helpers, it is possible to comment on the extent to which hunter–gatherer allomothering resembles and differs from cooperative and communal breeding systems.

Methods

This study has approval from the Ethics Committee of University College London (UCL Ethics code 3086/003), and the methods were carried out in accordance with the approved guidelines. Research permission was granted by the Republic of Congo's Ministry of Scientific Research and local government in the Philippines. Data were collected between February and October 2014. Informed consent was obtained for all participants. The purpose of the study and what it would entail was explained to all camp members in the local language by translators. They were then asked if they were interested in participating. Translators then read a prepared consent form to all potential participants in the local language, which willing participants signed via a pen mark or thumbprint according to their preference. If a child was under the age of 18, consent could be provided by either one of their parents. There was one child who lived in a household with her grandparents because both of her parents had moved to the city for labour opportunities; in her case, consent was provided by her grandmother who was a socially recognised guardian. Participating households were compensated with a gift of their choice out of thermal bottles, cooking pans and utensils or tools.

Study populations

The Agta and BaYaka are best described as immediate-return hunter–gatherers. This is an anthropological term referring to hunter–gatherer societies that do not accumulate material resources or store food and have mobile residence patterns and an egalitarian political organisation (Woodburn, Reference Woodburn1982). Allomothering is frequent in both populations (Page et al., Reference Page, Chaudhary, Viguier, Dyble, Thompson, Smith and Migliano2017, Reference Page, Emmott, Dyble, Smith, Chaudhary, Viguier and Migliano2021).

The Agta

The Agta communities that we worked with live in the coastal Palanan region of northeast Luzon, the Philippines. The average camp size is seven households and just under 50 individuals, average relatedness within camps is low and mobility between camps is high (Dyble et al., Reference Dyble, Salali, Chaudhary, Page, Smith, Thompson and Migliano2015; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Dyble, Major, Page, Chaudhary, Salali and Mace2019). Relationships are serially monogamous and polygyny does not occur. The Agta are bilocal, meaning that a couple is equally as likely to live with the man's relatives as the woman's relatives. However, it appears that Agta families that include young children may be more likely to stay with the mother's relatives. We found that nine of the Agta children in this study were living in the same camp as their maternal grandmother but only three were living with their paternal grandmother.

The communities we worked with rely heavily on spearfishing, inter-tidal foraging and the gathering of wild foods, and only occasionally hunt terrestrial game. Their foraged foods are supplemented by trading with local agricultural groups and in some cases wage labour (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Schlaepfer, Major, Dyble, Page, Thompson and Migliano2017; Dyble et al., Reference Dyble, Thorley, Page, Smith and Migliano2019). Hunting is a male activity despite famous reports of women hunting in neighbouring Agta populations (Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Griffin, Estioko-Griffin and Grove1985). Boys spend more time hunting as they get older, usually in groups of similar-aged adolescents. Spearfishing is also predominantly, although not exclusively, done by men, especially in open-water or in rivers that are distant from camp. Subadults of both sexes may spearfish, but there is then a decline among females after they have dependent offspring. Agta of all ages and both sexes engage in inter-tidal foraging for a variety of resources including octopus and shellfish; these trips often occur in family groups led by the mother. While gathering wild resources is predominantly a female activity, it also often occurs in family groups which sometimes include adult men. In summary, there are several economic activities that are at least sometimes carried out by both sexes and across age-groups. Nevertheless, the division of labour is still structured in a manner that affects opportunities for childcare. Infants and toddlers are often present when women or subadults are gathering or fishing. Although men are sometimes present on these expeditions, they also frequently go spearfishing, and occasionally hunt, without women or children.

As infants begin to rely less on their mothers’ milk they spend an increasing amount of time in mixed-age and mixed-sex playgroups, and by 2 years of age this constitutes a significant proportion of their day. These playgroups are child-only and represent a key context in which subadults are responsible for supervision and providing other forms of care. Children of both sexes engage in childcare, but girls are usually more involved. In an experimental game assessing Agta children's cooperative behaviour, we found that older children willingly shared food resources with younger children (Major-Smith et al., Reference Major-Smith, Chaudhary, Dyble, Major-Smith, Page, Salali and Migliano2023).

The BaYaka

We worked with BaYaka hunter–gatherers who live in the Ndoki rainforest in the Republic of Congo. Camp size varies from 20 to 80 individuals, average genetic relatedness within camps is low, and there is high between-camp mobility (Dyble et al., Reference Dyble, Salali, Chaudhary, Page, Smith, Thompson and Migliano2015). As with the Agta, BaYaka residence is generally bilocal but families with young children appear to be more likely to live with the mother's relatives. Eleven of the children were living in the same camp as their maternal grandmother, but only three were living with their paternal grandmother. Relationships are predominantly serially monogamous; there are cases of polygyny but the point-prevalence is below 10% (Chaudhary et al., Reference Chaudhary, Salali, Thompson, Dyble, Page, Smith and Migliano2015). In the cases where a man has more than one wife, the wives reside in different camps and the man splits his time between camps.

Unlike some Central African foragers, only men engage in hunting and trapping in the communities we worked with. After reaching adolescence, boys may begin to accompany their fathers or other older men on these expeditions. It is also only men who climb trees to collect honey, although the whole family may go on these trips. Females contribute to subsistence via fishing as well as gathering yams and wild plants, and their foraging expeditions frequently occur in groups including both adult women and girls. Finally, children of both sexes often collect fruits and mushrooms when in playgroups. Infants and toddlers may be taken along for all foraging activities except for hunting and trapping; therefore foraging and childcare are often mutually exclusive activities for adult men but not for women or subadults. The BaYaka also engage in some trade with members of neighbouring Bantu communities, exchanging wild foods for alcohol, cigarettes or manioc (Chaudhary et al., Reference Chaudhary, Salali, Thompson, Rey, Gerbault, Stevenson and Migliano2016; Knight et al., Reference Knight ID, Deniz Salali, Sikka, Derkx, Keestra ID and Chaudhary2021).

Both girls and boys become involved in childcare from around the age of 4, although the involvement of young girls rises rapidly relative to boys and their contribution is larger throughout the rest of the subadult period (Salali et al., Reference Salali, Chaudhary, Bouer, Thompson, Vinicius and Migliano2019). It is not uncommon to see very young children of both sexes feeding, soothing and carrying infants, and a video analysis of a 4-year-old boy suggested that children are capable of sensitive caregiving from a very young age (Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, Minter, Angnged, Cissé, Salali and Migliano2018). Another study found that girls between 4 and 7 years of age increase the foraging efficiency of nursing women during group expeditions by looking after their infants (Jang et al., Reference Jang, Janmaat, Kandza and Boyette2022). As with the Agta, older infants and toddlers spend an increasing proportion of time in mixed-age child-only playgroups from around the age of 2, in which older children are responsible for their supervision and care.

It is noteworthy that this research was conducted with an ethnolinguistic subgroup of the BaYaka who speak Mbendjele. They are distinct from the Aka, who are a different BaYaka subgroup living in the Central African Republic. To avoid confusion, we explicitly refer to the latter as the Aka when making references to research that has been conducted with them.

Data collection

There were no exclusion criteria for this study since we wanted to provide a characterisation of children's close-proximity networks that was as complete and representative as possible. All camp residents were free to participate, and they were all willing to do so. In both populations close-proximity networks were constructed for all children under four years of age. The study sample was comprised of six Agta and three BaYaka camps, and we constructed close-proximity networks for 49 children between 0.08 and 3.99 years of age. It included a total of 30 Agta children, including one set of twins, and 890 allomother–child dyads; and 19 BaYaka children, none of whom were twins, and 790 allomother–child dyads. In the case of two BaYaka children and one Agta child, the exact relationship with all campmates could not be discerned owing to missing information, and as such some analyses have slightly smaller sample sizes. Therefore, we specify samples sizes throughout the Results section.

Measuring close proximity

To quantify the relative amount of time each potential allomother spent in close proximity to each infant/toddler under 4 years of age we used motes, which are portable remote sensing devices; more specifically, we used the UCMote Mini with a TinyOS operating system. They are programmed to emit an imperceptible radio signal known as a ‘beacon’ at a specified time interval, which is then received and stored in the internal memory of any other mote within a specified distance of the sender mote. Upon receipt of a beacon, the recipient mote stores an identification number corresponding to the sender mote.

All camp members wore a mote with a unique identification number in adjustable belt pouches/wristbands for between 5 and 9 days depending on the camp. We programmed all motes to emit beacons every 2 minutes during this period and to record any beacons registered within a 3 meter radius. We chose 3 metres since an allomother can pay clear attention to the child within this distance and quickly intervene if required; this threshold has been used as a measure of proximity in observational studies of hunter–gatherer childcare (e.g. Marlowe, Reference Marlowe, Hewlett and Lamb2005b; Page et al., Reference Page, Emmott, Dyble, Smith, Chaudhary, Viguier and Migliano2021).

By downloading the data from the internal memory of all the motes it was possible to construct children's close-proximity networks. These quantify the relative proportion of time each child spends in close proximity to each potential allomother. The networks are complete since all individuals a child could interact with were wearing motes. Only beacons recorded between 05:00 and 20:00 were included to prevent data reflecting which individuals slept in the same shelter. For the Agta, we also have data derived from setting the motes threshold to 1.5 metres in five of the six camps. We characterised children's close-proximity networks at this threshold as a sensitivity analysis and the key trends remained the same; these results are available in the supplementary material (Figures S1 and S2).

The transition from being cared for to becoming a source of care is extremely fast and seamless; therefore, we considered all individuals older than 4 years of age as potential allomothers. Some children were observed to have transitioned from receiving care to providing it on research trips that were less than a year apart; this is usually catalysed by the birth of a younger sibling whom they begin to care for. The average inter-birth interval among hunter–gatherers is ~3.5 years (Marlowe, Reference Marlowe2005a; Pennington, Reference Pennington, Panter-Brick, Layton and Rowley-Conwy2001). In our entire sample of potential allomothers (n = 283), there were only three individuals under the age of 7.5 – an age at which children unequivocally contribute to childcare – who did not have a younger sibling in camp. Had we chosen a higher age threshold for classifying an individual as a potential allomother, we would have missed the very important contribution of young children in caring for their even younger siblings. Furthermore, as described above, a detailed video analysis of caregiving by a 4-year old BaYaka boy suggested that children can make valuable contributions to childcare from this age (Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, Minter, Angnged, Cissé, Salali and Migliano2018).

Advantages and limitations of the motes

In the supplementary material we present a verification of the motes data using data from a smaller sample of observational focal follows. The verification indicates that the close proximity being measured by the motes does indeed reflect the amount of time potential allomothers are in position to watch over a child and intervene if required. Some of this time may involve actual intervention and high investment care, but it is not possible to determine how often, if at all, this occurs. Thus, we can only conclude that at a minimum a potential allomother is in a position where they can watch over a child and intervene if required, but more costly care may also be provided. Given these limitations, our results do not offer a precise valuation of the allomaternal involvement of different helpers, and cannot be considered to be a complete description of children's allomothering networks.

Furthermore, food provisioning is a fundamental component of allomothering among hunter–gatherers and many other species. The motes do not offer insight into how much different allomothers contribute to provisioning. This limits the possibility of using the them in isolation to draw any firm conclusions regarding the extent to which hunter–gatherer allomothering resembles cooperative or communal breeding systems.

Despite these limitations on what can be inferred from the motes relative to traditional observational methods, they also offer some advantages. As mentioned, many observational studies of childcare also measure proximity, and sometimes categorise it as a form of low-cost care in and of itself. Observational studies often include ~15 focal children who are directly observed over the course of 12 daylight hours each (Meehan, Reference Meehan2005). These studies do produce substantial datasets; however, using the motes, it is possible to gather data from larger samples spanning longer periods of time since the researcher does not constantly need to be present after set-up. Here we present motes data for almost 50 children spanning between 5 and 9 days each. Additionally, in observational studies, data are only collected when there is daylight since the researcher needs to be able to see clearly (Ivey, Reference Ivey2000; Marlowe, Reference Marlowe, Hewlett and Lamb2005b; Meehan, Reference Meehan2005; Scelza, Reference Scelza2009). However, in our experience, hunter–gatherer communities often wake up before the sun has fully risen and do not go to sleep as soon as it gets dark. The motes do not require light and therefore can record data during these times. Thus, mote data are less likely to be subject to biases resulting from small sample sizes, short observation periods and a lack of data on care that occurs at certain times of day. In this sense, the motes are complementary to in-depth observational studies which offer high-resolution data on the specific caring behaviours provided by allomothers.

Determining relationship type and relatedness between children and potential allomothers

We conducted genealogical and reproductive history interviews with all adults in each camp. Participants were asked to provide the name and sex of their grandparents, parents, parents’ siblings, siblings, offspring and spouse(s), including those that were deceased. Once this data was collected it was possible to determine the relationship type, e.g. grandparent–grandchild/cousin–cousin/unrelated etc., and the coefficient of relatedness between each child and potential allomother. Given that we are interested in examining the effects of relatedness, individuals are only categorised as aunts or uncles if they are biologically related to the child. ‘Unrelated’ refers to dyads with a coefficient of relatedness of r < 0.125.

Assigning age

Age estimates were calculated for all members of the study population by creating relative age lists with the communities, as well as potential age ranges for all participants. The age ranges were based on dental development, whether their birth happened before or after a memorable event with a known date and sibling birth orders. This information was then integrated using a Gibbs sampling framework producing probability distributions for the age of each participant; these were then collapsed into point estimates based on the mean. For full information and a validation of this method see Diekmann et al. (Reference Diekmann, Smith, Gerbault, Dyble, Page, Chaudhary and Thomas2017).

Characterising children's close-proximity networks

For each child we constructed a close-proximity network which is constituted of the time spent in close proximity with each potential allomother. We quantify the involvement of each potential allomother in each child's close-proximity network. We do this by calculating the amount of time that a given child spends in close proximity to a given potential allomother as a proportion of the time that child spends in close proximity to all potential allomothers. This is simply equivalent to the number of beacons received by a child's mote from a potential allomother's mote as a proportion of the total number of beacons received by that child's mote from the motes of all potential allomothers.

To compare the involvement of different categories of potential allomothers, e.g. fathers, unrelated individuals etc., we calculated the aggregate and adjusted involvement of each category of allomother. For each child, the aggregate involvement of a given category of potential allomother is calculated by summing the involvement of all members of that category in the child's network. We sum them because a complete characterisation of an allomothering system must incorporate the fact that some categories of helper are more available, i.e. have more members in a camp than others.

We reiterate the importance of this measure using an example given in the introduction. If an average co-resident sibling helps much more than an average unrelated campmate, we may infer that the allomothering system resembles a sibling helpers at the nest system and is underpinned by kin selection. However, it may be the case that children often do not have any siblings but usually have many unrelated campmates, and consequently the aggregate involvement of siblings is much lower than that of non-kin. In this case the inference about kin selection and sibling helpers at the nest would have been misleading.

The purpose of the adjusted involvement measure is to facilitate comparison of the involvement of an average individual of each category when they are available, i.e. alive and co-residing with a child. Therefore, only cases in which at least one individual of a given category is available to a child are incorporated into the calculation. For instance, if a child's father is deceased or lives in another camp, the lack of involvement of that father is irrelevant to understanding what fathers do when they are available; therefore, it is ignored in the calculation. For a given child, the adjusted involvement of a category is the mean involvement of members of that category.

In combination with aggregate involvement, adjusted involvement offers insight into the dynamics of the allomothering system at a lower-level and how it is related to availability. For example, fathers may be highly involved when they are available, but the majority of children may not have access to their fathers owing to high male mortality. In this case, the aggregate involvement of fathers would be low, but the adjusted involvement would be high since the latter only considers how involved fathers are in cases when they are present. Providing both the aggregate and adjusted involvement of different categories allows for a more complete description and nuanced understanding of children's close-proximity networks. Table 1 provides definitions of these measures for reference, and a worked example is available in Table S1.

Table 1. Glossary of key measures

Categories of potential allomother

  1. (1) We calculated the aggregate and adjusted involvement of potential allomothers for the following relationship categories: fathers, siblings, grandmothers, grandfathers, aunts/uncles, cousins and unrelated campmates.

  2. (2) We also calculated the adjusted involvement of potential allomothers according to their level of relatedness to a child, which can take the values of: r = 0.5, r = 0.25, r = 0.125 and r =  0. Children often have more than 20 campmates with whom they are unrelated, which is far more than the number of potential allomothers they have at any other level of relatedness. Involvement may be highly skewed in such a large category. For instance, it may be the case that the majority of unrelated campmates are not particularly involved in a child's proximity network, but a few are heavily involved. Therefore, we separately calculated the involvement of each of the six most involved unrelated campmates to gain an insight into the level of skew. We also present data on the most involved potential allomother at each of the other levels of relatedness.

  3. (3) We calculated the aggregate and adjusted involvement of potential allomothers from each life-stage – subadults, adults and post-reproductive individuals. Again, we also calculate the involvement of the most involved potential allomother in each life-stage category.

We define subadults as individuals under 17 years old, which is the mean age of menarche among Agta females (Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Griffin, Estioko-Griffin and Grove1985); we apply this threshold in both populations because there is not an equivalent estimate for the BaYaka. There are no individuals in either sample under the age of 17 who have become parents. In both populations it is also rare for individuals to form a household with a spouse prior to reaching the age of 17. Post-reproductive stage individuals are females older than 50 years old, and males over 50 years old except those with a wife under 50. All other individuals are categorised as being in the adult, i.e. reproductive-age, life-stage. Categorising potential allomothers into these life-stages allows us to isolate the involvement of those individuals who are capable of personal reproduction; in other species, this is typically a prerequisite for allomothering to be classified as cooperative breeding.

Additionally, individuals are further classified as reproductively active or inactive. All subadults and post-reproductive individuals are categorised as reproductively inactive. Adults are considered reproductively active unless one of the following conditions is met, in which case they are considered reproductively inactive: (1) they have not had a live birth in the previous seven years of adulthood, which is approximately twice the average inter-birth interval among extant hunter–gatherers (Marlowe, Reference Marlowe2005a; Pennington, Reference Pennington, Panter-Brick, Layton and Rowley-Conwy2001); and (2) they are living in their natal household and have had no live births, i.e. they have delayed ‘dispersal' and reproduction. In communal breeding systems allomothers are reproductively active, whereas in cooperative breeding systems they are reproductively inactive.

Children's age-groups

We provide a comparison of how our results vary for children under 2 years of age vs. those 2 years old and above. From approximately 2 years old, children spend a substantial amount of time in child-only playgroups during which they are in proximity to other children rather than adults, this represents a key transition in the composition of their proximity networks. This change in children's social life has been reported in many hunter–gatherer populations including the Agta and BaYaka (Chaudhary & Swanepoel, Reference Chaudhary and Swanepoel2023; Lew-Levy et al., Reference Lew-Levy, Reckin, Lavi, Cristóbal-Azkarate and Ellis-Davies2017; Page et al., Reference Page, Emmott, Dyble, Smith, Chaudhary, Viguier and Migliano2021).

Statistical analysis

The following analyses were conducted in R version 4.2.1 and figures were created using the ggplot2 package.

First, we wished to establish the shared features of the composition of children's proximity networks across the two populations as we were interested in identifying which elements of allomothering systems are the most consistent/variable across hunter–gatherer societies. Additionally, we wished to investigate how the composition of children's proximity networks may change with age. Many studies of direct care focus on early infancy. However, opportunities for allomothering probably increase as a child relies less on their mother's milk. Correspondingly, some studies highlight that around the age of 2 children begin spending more time in playgroups away from their mother (Lew-Levy et al., Reference Lew-Levy, Reckin, Lavi, Cristóbal-Azkarate and Ellis-Davies2017; Page et al., Reference Page, Emmott, Dyble, Smith, Chaudhary, Viguier and Migliano2021).

We used Dirichlet regressions to test for differences in the composition of children's proximity networks between the two populations and also between the two age-groups of children. Dirichlet regressions are appropriate when the dependent variable is compositional in nature, i.e. comprising multiple proportions that sum to 1.

We conducted one Dirichlet regression in which the dependent variable was the composition of a child's network in terms of the aggregate involvement of each relationship category of potential allomother, e.g. fathers, siblings, etc. We conducted another Dirichlet regression in which the dependent variable was the composition of a child's network in terms of the aggregate involvement of each life-stage category of potential allomother – subadults, adults and post-reproductive campmates. Since the dependent variable reflects the composition of a network, these analyses are conducted at the child-level, i.e. one network per child.

In both Dirichlet regressions, study population and child age-group were the predictors. These were conducted using the DirichletReg package.

Second, establishing differences in the aggregate involvement of the different categories of potential allomother is required to understand allomothering from a systems perspective. This is because it is necessary to incorporate the fact that some categories of helper are more available than others, as discussed above. To test for differences in the aggregate involvement of different categories of potential allomother we first applied a centred log-ratio (clr) transformation to the aggregate network composition data using the compositions package. Compositional data is constrained because it consists of multiple proportions which are between 0 and 1, add up to 1 and are non-independent. Clr transformations are frequently used in compositional data analysis to transform the data into a format which is appropriate for the application of standard statistical techniques.

We then conducted pairwise Wilcoxon signed-ranked permutation tests to test for significant difference in the aggregate involvement of (1) different relationship categories; and (2) different life-stage categories of potential allomothers. Permutation tests were chosen since they are better able to handle tied data, they were conducted using the coin package. We applied Bonferroni corrections for multiple testing.

We also conducted a Wilcoxon signed-rank permutation test to examine whether the aggregate involvement of all categories of relative combined was significantly greater than that of unrelated campmates. We conducted another Wilcoxon signed-rank permutation test to examine whether the aggregate involvement of reproductively inactive campmates was significantly greater than that of reproductively active campmates. These Wilcoxon signed-rank permutation tests were conducted separately for each population.

Third, we also wished to compare the involvement of an average individual of each category when they are available, i.e. alive and co-residing with a child. To examine the factors that predict the involvement of an individual available potential allomother we conducted mixed-effects models on the clr transformed data.

These mixed-effects models were conducted at the dyad level and the dependent variable was the involvement of a given potential allomother in a given child's network. All possible dyads were included, thus each child and each potential allomother appear multiple times in the analysis. Therefore, allomother ID and child ID were included as crossed-random effects. We conducted several of these mixed-effects regressions on the clr transformed data, one for each of the following predictors:

  1. (1) Relationship type of the potential allomother-child dyad. This variable could take the following values – fathers; sibling; grandmother; grandfather; aunt/uncle; cousin; unrelated campmate.

  2. (2) Coefficient of relatedness of the potential allomother-child dyad. This variable could take the values – most involved unrelated allomother; r = 0; r = 0.125; r = 0.25; r = 0.5. In order to compare like for like, we also followed this up with a model adding ‘most involved relative outside of the nuclear family’ as an extra value.

  3. (3) Life-stage of the potential allomother. This variable could take the following values – subadult; adult; post-reproductive campmate; most involved subadult; most involved adult; most involved post-reproductive campmate.

The reason for including ‘most involved’ individuals from certain categories in (2) and (3) is because in categories where many individuals are available, their relative involvement is likely to be highly skewed. For instance, the average involvement of an unrelated potential allomother may be low, but the involvement of the most involved unrelated allomother may be very high.

These mixed-effects models were conducted using the package lmerTest, and were run separately for the Agta and BaYaka.

Fourth, the initial findings highlighted notable differences in grandmaternal involvement between the two populations. Therefore, we examined whether this may be a product of differences in the availability of grandmothers, as well as differences in the extent to which they were busy with mothering duties. We used chi-square tests to test for differences between the Agta and BaYaka in the proportion of children who had (1) a co-resident grandmother and (2) a co-resident grandmother with no dependent offspring of her own.

Results

Agta and BaYaka children have strikingly similar close-proximity networks and rely on the availability of several categories of related and unrelated campmates

Table 2 provides demographic information for the children in each population as well as information on the availability of different categories of potential allomother. The proximity data suggest that allocare may occur extensively in both populations as mothers only account for 22 and 21% of children's close-proximity interactions in the Agta and BaYaka, respectively. The results show that the constitutions of children's close-proximity networks in both populations are very similar from a systems perspective. The aggregate involvement of each category of potential allomother is virtually identical except with respect to grandmothers and fathers, the former having greater aggregate involvement among the BaYaka and the latter among the Agta (Figure 1a and b). Using Dirichlet regression we found that the association between study population and the aggregate involvement of grandmothers approached statistical significance (p = 0.051; n = 46 (29 Agta)); this was not the case for any other relationship category (see Table S2 for full results). The aggregate involvement of grandmothers in the networks of Agta and BaYaka children was 2 and 8% respectively.

Table 2. Focal children demographic characteristics and potential allomother availability

a A potential allomother was considered part of a child's close-proximity network if their involvement was greater than 1%.

b Availability refers to the number of potential allomothers in that category that live in the same camp from the perspective of an average child.

Figure 1. Aggregate and adjusted involvement of different categories of potential allomother. Error bars represent standard errors. The following are calculated by averaging the data from the close-proximity networks of all children in a given population. (a and b) The aggregate involvement of each category of potential allomother (n = 46 children (29 Agta)). GMs refers to grandmothers and GFs refers to grandfathers. (c and d) The adjusted involvement of each category of potential allomother (n = 46 children [29 Agta]). (e and f) The adjusted involvement of an average relative (av), and the most involved relative (first), with coefficients of relatedness 0.125/0.25/0.5 respectively, and the involvement of the six most involved unrelated campmates (n = 47 children (28 Agta)). For example, child x has two relatives with whom they share a coefficient of relatedness of r = 0.25, who were responsible for 20 and 10% of their close proximity interactions respectively. For this child, the adjusted involvement of r = 0.25 (av) would be 15%, and of r = 0.25 (first) would be 20%.

In both populations unrelated campmates and siblings stand out, each were responsible for ~30% of the time children spend in close proximity to others. Their combined aggregate involvement in the networks is more than three timese the combined aggregate involvement of fathers and grandmothers. Excluding siblings, unrelated campmates had significantly higher aggregate involvement than each category of relative (n = 46 (29 Agta); Figure 1a and b). All of these differences remained significant at the p < 0.001 level after applying a Bonferonni correction for conducting multiple pairwise tests. That is not to say that genetic relatedness is unimportant. The aggregate involvement of all categories of relative combined comprised approximately 70% of children's close-proximity time in both populations and was significantly higher than unrelated campmates among both the Agta (p < 0.001; n = 29) and the BaYaka (p = 0.007; n = 17).

Fathers and grandmothers are the only categories in which the adjusted involvement is greater than aggregate involvement (Figure 1ad), highlighting that these potential allomothers are not always available to children. For instance, in cases where an Agta grandmother co-resides with a grandchild, she is typically responsible for 5% of her grandchild's time in close proximity to potential allomothers (Figure 1c). However, the aggregate involvement of Agta grandmothers is only 2% because a majority of children do not live with a grandmother (Figure 1a; Table 2).

The adjusted involvement of unrelated campmates is 1.8 and 1.4% in the Agta and BaYaka respectively; in both populations this is the lowest of all relationship categories (Figure 1c and d). Accordingly, in the corresponding mixed-effects model, being an unrelated campmate was associated with significantly lower involvement than being a member of any other relationship category, except in the case of Agta grandfathers where the association approached significance (p = 0.054) (Agta: n = 866 dyads; BaYaka: n = 736 dyads; see Table S3 for full results). Although most unrelated campmates have little involvement, there are typically two or three who are heavily involved in a child's close-proximity network (Figure 1e and f). In both populations, a child's three most involved unrelated campmates are together responsible for ~20% of that child's total time in close proximity to potential allomothers. Furthermore, the most involved unrelated campmate spends more time in close proximity to a child than an average relative sharing a coefficient of relatedness of 0.25 (Figure 1e and f). The corresponding result in the mixed-effect models was significant for the Agta (p = 0.007) and approached significance for the BaYaka (p = 0.095) (Agta: n = 866 dyads; BaYaka: n = 736 dyads; see Table S4 for full results). That is not to imply that relatives outside of the nuclear family are generally less important. When we compared like for like, the most involved relative outside the nuclear family was more involved than the most involved unrelated campmate (Figure 1e and f), although these differences were not statistically significant in either population (Table S5).

We divided the sample into children under 2 years old and children 2 years old and above. At the aggregate level, unrelated campmates and siblings remain the two most involved categories by quite some margin for both age-groups in both populations (Table 3). This margin is even more pronounced in children 2 years old and above. In a Dirichlet regression, child age-group was positively associated with the aggregate involvement of siblings (p < 0.001) and unrelated campmates (p = 0.059; n = 46 (29 Agta)). There were no associations between age-group and any other relationship category which either were significant or approached statistical significance (Table S2).

Table 3. Aggregate involvement (%) of potential allomothers by relationship type and child age-group

Variation between the Agta and BaYaka in grandmaternal involvement

As noted above, the study population had a predictive effect on the aggregate involvement of grandmothers which approached statistical significance (Figure 1a and b). This was not the case for any other category of potential allomother; therefore we investigated this difference further. The difference is in part related to availability. When we examined a larger dataset, beyond just those camps where the motes study took place, we found that BaYaka children under the age of 4 were much more likely to have a co-resident grandmother than their Agta counterparts (65.9 vs. 44.7%; χ 2 = 4.6, p = 0.033, n = 155 (114 Agta)). Furthermore, even when an Agta grandmother is co-residing with a grandchild, her involvement is approximately half that of a BaYaka grandmother (Figure 1c and d). These two trends may be tied to women's fertility schedules. The higher total fertility rate among the Agta (8.07) compared with BaYaka (6.07; Table S6) increases the likelihood that a given Agta child's grandmother resides in a different camp since she is more likely to have other grandchildren living elsewhere. Moreover, there is a later cessation of reproduction among Agta women (Figure S3). Consequently, the likelihood that a co-resident Agta grandmother does not have any dependent offspring of her own, i.e. is not busy being a mother, is much lower than that of a BaYaka grandmother (37.3% vs. 73.1%; χ 2 = 7.5, p = 0.006, n = 77 (51 Agta)).

Both reproductively active and inactive helpers are involved in children's close-proximity networks in both populations

Our results also revealed a very strong similarity between the Agta and BaYaka in the involvement of campmates of different life-stages in children's close-proximity networks (Figure 2). Accordingly, in the Dirichlet regression, there was no significant association between study population and the aggregate involvement of subadults, adults or post-reproductive potential allomothers (n = 49 (30 Agta); Table S7). In both populations the aggregate involvement of subadults was the highest at 54% and the aggregate involvement of post-reproductive campmates the smallest at 9% and 11% in the Agta and BaYaka, respectively. We conducted Wilcoxon signed-rank permutation tests and found that the aggregate involvement of subadults was significantly higher than adults (p < 0.001), and the aggregate involvement of adults was significantly higher than post-reproductive campmates (p < 0.001) (n = 49 (30 Agta)). The involvement of potential allomothers in all life-stages included both related and unrelated helpers (Table 4).

Figure 2. Both reproductively active and inactive camp members are involved in children's close-proximity networks. Results for the Agta in red and the BaYaka in blue. Error bars represent standard errors. See methods for definitions of life-stages and reproductive status. (a and b) The inner-ring is the aggregate involvement of reproductively active and inactive campmates respectively; the outer ring dissects this involvement by life-stage. (n = 49 children (30 Agta)). (c and d) The adjusted involvement of children's most involved (first) and average (av) potential allomother of each life-stage (n = 49 children (30 Agta)).

Table 4. Aggregate involvement split by life-stage and relatedness status

The low aggregate involvement of post-reproductive individuals is not solely a product of low availability since this trend is also reflected in our comparison of the most involved allomother from each life-stage (Figure 2c and d). In the mixed-effects models (Agta: n = 890 dyads; BaYaka: n = 790 dyads), being the most involved post-reproductive individual was associated with significantly lower involvement than being the most involved subadult (Agta: p < 0.001; BaYaka: p < 0.001) and adult (Agta: p < 0.001; BaYaka: p = 0.025; Table S8).

The trends are similar across the networks of children in each age-group (Table 5). In the Dirichlet regression, there was no significant association between child age-group and the aggregate involvement of adults or post-reproductive campmates (n = 49 (30 Agta); Table S7). However, there was a positive association between child age-group and the aggregate involvement of subadults which approached statistical significance (p = 0.060).

Table 5. Aggregate involvement (%) of potential allomothers by life-stage and child age-group

Overall, in both of the populations, children spent 69% of their close-proximity time with reproductively inactive potential allomothers, significantly more than the 31% with reproductively active campmates (Agta: p < 0.001, n = 30; BaYaka: p < 0.001, n = 19; Figure 2a and b). Importantly, we found that 22% of Agta and 16% of BaYaka adults were reproductively inactive. That is to say, they had either (1) not had a live birth in the previous seven adult years or (2) they had not left their natal household nor had a live birth. Their aggregate involvement was 6 and 4% in the Agta and BaYaka respectively. This is noteworthy since it is the contributions of these individuals which are being referred to in traditional definitions of cooperative breeding in non-human animals.

The core involvement of subadults, unrelated campmates and siblings is consistent across hunter–gatherer societies

The Agta and BaYaka close-proximity networks presented here are strikingly similar in terms of core contributions of siblings, unrelated campmates and subadults, whereas the contributions of fathers and grandmothers vary between the populations. We searched for data on hunter–gatherer allomothering in other societies and found very similar trends across ecologies, from savannahs to deserts to tropical forests (Table 6). Siblings (28–32%) and unrelated (30–40%) allomothers are consistently the two highest contributors to childcare. The only exception to this is among the Australian Martu where the author notes a biased sample in which 70% of the sampled children did not have any siblings over the age of 5 (Scelza, Reference Scelza2009). The contribution of subadults is also consistently high across populations where data is available (54–62%).

Table 6. Comparison of allocare trends across hunter–gatherer societies

Entries represent aggregate allomaternal contributions provided by each category of allomother.

a Sources cited either present the figures shown here or provide the data required to calculate the figures shown here. Details of how the calculations were conducted can be found in the supplementary material.

b A subadult can also be a sibling or an unrelated campmate, hence row totals can exceed 100%.

e The Hadza data on the allomaternal contributions of siblings, fathers and grandmothers are from Marlowe (Reference Marlowe, Hewlett and Lamb2005b). However, Marlowe (Reference Marlowe, Hewlett and Lamb2005b) does not present the data required to calculate the contributions of unrelated and subadult allomothers. Therefore, the figures for unrelated and subadult allomothers are derived from a study that specifically examined which allomothers spent time holding infants and toddlers (Crittenden & Marlowe, Reference Crittenden and Marlowe2008).

f Scelza (Reference Scelza2009), as summarised in Kramer (Reference Kramer2010).

g The author highlights that the low sibling contribution may be an underestimate resulting from features of the specific sample in the study – 40% of the focal children were first-borns and another 30% had no siblings over the age of five.

h Kruger and Konner (Reference Kruger and Konner2010). This study did not examine all forms of allocare, rather it specifically focused on which allomothers respond to infant crying.

The contribution of fathers to direct care is more variable across societies but never reaches 20%. Grandmaternal contributions are very variable ranging from 2 to 21%. Overall, it appears that substantial involvement of siblings, unrelated campmates and subadults may be the most consistent features of non-maternal direct investment across hunter–gatherers, whereas the aggregate involvement of fathers and grandmothers is lower and more variable than these categories.

Discussion

Here, we found several striking similarities between the close-proximity networks of Agta and BaYaka children. These are mirrored in available data on allomothering systems of other hunter–gatherer populations living across vastly differing ecologies, from savannahs to deserts to tropical rainforests.

Unrelated campmates, siblings and subadults were the most involved in children's close-proximity networks at the aggregate level. The involvement of fathers and grandmothers was the most variable between the two populations. Importantly, both related and unrelated and reproductively active and inactive campmates spend substantial amounts of time in close-proximity with infants and toddlers. This, in and of itself, suggests that a label of cooperative breeding or communal breeding does not encapsulate hunter–gatherer allomothering at a systems level.

The differences between the involvement of categories at the aggregate vs. adjusted level highlight an association between helper availability and allomothering systems. Below we discuss the involvement of different categories of potential allomother, and where relevant, the forces that shape their availability and involvement. Accordingly, we are able to consider similarities and differences with descriptions of cooperative and communal breeding in other taxa.

Reproductively inactive potential allomothers

Reproductively inactive campmates were responsible for approximately 70% of children's time in close-proximity to potential allomothers. Their involvement satisfies loose definitions of cooperative breeding, and in some cases has parallels with traditional definitions.

Grandmothers and post-reproductive campmates

One of the reasons it may have become common to describe humans as cooperative breeders is due to the heavy emphasis on post-reproductive grandmothers (Hawkes & Coxworth, Reference Hawkes and Coxworth2013). They are reproductively inactive helpers; and the extended post-reproductive lifespan evolved via reallocation of energy away from reproduction towards somatic effort to increase vigour and buffer mortality risks in later life (Hawkes, Reference Hawkes2003). Thus, the emergence of grandmothering involved a reduction in personal reproductive effort, which was favoured because of the inclusive fitness benefits of helping a closely related reproductively active female.

Regardless of these similarities, we found that the involvement of grandmothers and other post-reproductive campmates to be modest, particularly among the Agta. The low aggregate involvement appears to be partially due to low availability, presumably because of mortality and multi-local residence. The low adjusted involvement of an Agta grandmother may be due to women's late cessation of reproduction, resulting in grandmothers being busy providing care to their own offspring. Correspondingly, Ache women stop reproducing even later in life (Migliano et al., Reference Migliano, Vinicius and Lahr2007), and anthropologists working with the Ache emphasise that grandmothers are unimportant (Hill & Hurtado, Reference Hill and Hurtado2009). In contrast, Scelza (Reference Scelza2009) highlights that Martu grandmothers can make vital allomaternal contributions because most of them have finished their personal reproduction and are no longer busy rearing their own children. Indeed, the involvement of grandmothers is one of the most variable features of allomaternal systems across hunter–gatherer societies (Table 6); we suspect that this may be driven by variability in fertility schedules.

We emphasise that Table 6 and the motes data only examine grandmaternal involvement in childcare. We have not assessed the contributions grandmothers make in the domains of provisioning and knowledge sharing, which have both been highlighted by other anthropologists (Hawkes et al., Reference Hawkes, O'Connell and Blurton Jones1997; Scelza & Hinde, Reference Scelza and Hinde2019). Additionally, the data presented is from the perspective of an average individual child. As such, we are not attempting to evaluate the inclusive fitness gains of a post-reproductive lifespan. This would require taking the grandmother's perspective and examining her allomaternal contributions across all domains to all her grandchildren.

Reproductively inactive adults

One of the primary motivations for studying traditional cooperative breeding in non-human animals is to understand why sexually mature helpers would remain non-reproductive and help raise the offspring of another adult (Hatchwell, Reference Hatchwell2009; Lukas & Clutton-Brock, Reference Lukas and Clutton-Brock2012b). As far as we are aware, among hunter–gatherers there has been only one quantification of the allomaternal contribution of reproductively inactive adults who are of reproductive age. Strikingly, 32% of reproductive-aged Efe women were reproductively inactive; however, they only provided 12% of allocare (Ivey, Reference Ivey2000). According to our definition, we found that 22% of Agta, and 16% of BaYaka, reproductive-aged adults were reproductively inactive. Their aggregate involvement was approximately 5% in both populations. Thus, a majority of Agta and BaYaka allomothering does not fit traditional definitions of cooperative breeding.

Siblings

Siblings, alongside unrelated campmates, had the highest involvement in the close-proximity networks of infants and toddlers, and their aggregate contribution is high among other foraging societies (Table 6). The high-relatedness between siblings arises owing to pair-bonding in both humans and cooperatively breeding non-human animals. In many cases, such as white fronted bee eaters, the reason for delayed dispersal of sibling helpers is low resource availability (Emlen, Reference Emlen1982). Hunter–gatherer siblings are not ‘delaying’ dispersal per se; they remain sexually immature when many of their younger siblings are being born. However, it is still difficulties in resource acquisition – arising from a high skill foraging niche rather than low resource availability – which drove the evolution of an extended immature period, and in turn makes siblings available as helpers. That is to say, it is an ecological constraint that favours helping behaviour over commencing personal reproduction. In this sense, the vital contribution of siblings has some similarities to cooperative breeding in non-human animals.

Subadults (related and unrelated)

Subadults had greater aggregate involvement than adults and post-reproductive campmates in the proximity networks described here, and we found this trend to be mirrored in studies of childcare across hunter–gatherer populations (Table 6). They are particularly interesting when considering the evolution of allomothering systems as they are both recipients and providers of allomothering. They remain nutritionally dependent in the natal household for an extended period while they acquire the necessary subsistence skills. As such, mothers rely on allomaternal support to raise multiple dependent offspring simultaneously. Similarly, complex foraging niches and the need for skill acquisition can lead to extended dependence and a reliance on allomothering in other species such as white-winged choughs (Heinsohn, Reference Heinsohn1991), and greater dependency loads are associated with the evolution of allomothering more generally (Lukas & Clutton-Brock, Reference Lukas and Clutton-Brock2012b). While hunter–gatherer subadults create a need for allomaternal provisioning as observed in other species, the results and inter-population comparison presented here highlight that they also make the largest allomaternal contribution to childcare.

Subadult care can play an important role in freeing mothers to engage in other tasks; this can happen even if mothers are present. For instance, BaYaka subadults that attend to infants on foraging expeditions increase the productivity of nursing mothers (Jang et al., Reference Jang, Janmaat, Kandza and Boyette2022). As infants become less dependent on their mother's milk, the potential for subadults to release mothers from caretaking duties increases further. Across foraging societies, older infants begin spending much of the day in mixed-age playgroups where they are supervised by older children (Lew-Levy et al., Reference Lew-Levy, Reckin, Lavi, Cristóbal-Azkarate and Ellis-Davies2017; Page et al., Reference Page, Emmott, Dyble, Smith, Chaudhary, Viguier and Migliano2021; Salali et al., Reference Salali, Chaudhary, Bouer, Thompson, Vinicius and Migliano2019). Correspondingly, we found that subadults and siblings spent more time with those infants/toddlers who were 2 years old and above.

It is important to highlight that the motivation for subadult allomothers to spend time in playgroups is not primarily to provide allocare. Key proximate motivations are probably the enjoyment of play, spending time with friends and being active. Play also has important ultimate functions unrelated to allocare, such as the acquisition and transmission of subsistence skills and social norms (Lew-Levy et al., Reference Lew-Levy, Reckin, Lavi, Cristóbal-Azkarate and Ellis-Davies2017, Reference Lew-Levy, Lavi, Reckin, Cristóbal-Azkarate and Ellis-Davies2018). Although allomothering is not the driving force behind subadult participation in playgroups, they are still the ones supervising and providing care to any infants and toddlers who are present. Correspondingly, we previously found that as Agta infants and toddlers spend more time in playgroups, their mothers devote less time to childcare and are freer to engage in other tasks (Page et al., Reference Page, Emmott, Dyble, Smith, Chaudhary, Viguier and Migliano2021).

This supervision and care that occur in these playgroups blend features of cooperative and communal breeding, including reproductively inactive helpers, some of whom are unrelated. When infants and toddlers are pooled in space, then mothers can engage in other activities while other caregivers supervise the playgroup. This is a similar to the crèches of communally breeding mammals. For example, among free-living house mice and black-and-white ruffed lemurs, crèching decreases the time mothers spend at the nest and increases their foraging time (Auclair et al., Reference Auclair, König, Ferrari, Perony and Lindholm2014; Baden et al., Reference Baden, Wright, Louis and Bradley2013). However, in contrast to communal breeding systems where reproductively active mothers take turns in supervising the crèche, in hunter–gatherer playgroups it is reproductively inactive subadults who supervise. Their availability to do so is a result of the late age of maturity already discussed, which is more similar to a cooperative breeding system. Equally, subadults in the same playgroup come from numerous reproductively active households and are often unrelated. Therefore, the processes that lead to the high availability and involvement of subadults are a combination of those that set the stage of cooperative and communal breeding.

Reproductively active helpers

Fathers

Pair-bonding provides a foundation for cooperative breeding in birds and mammals as it produces high relatedness between a female's offspring (Cornwallis et al., Reference Cornwallis, West, Davis and Griffin2010; Lukas & Clutton-Brock, Reference Lukas and Clutton-Brock2013). In our sample 80% of Agta and BaYaka children had access to a co-resident father. Although fathers are reproductively active, their contribution is not a form of communal breeding since they are providing care to their own offspring. Bi-parental care is normative among cooperatively breeding birds, and common in cooperatively breeding mammals (Griesser et al., Reference Griesser, Drobniak, Nakagawa and Botero2017; Opie et al., Reference Opie, Atkinson, Dunbar and Shultz2013).

We found that the time fathers and grandmothers spend in close proximity to children is relatively similar among the BaYaka. However, among the Agta, the relatively higher involvement of fathers may be a response to the lower involvement and availability of grandmothers. This may be because the payoff to paternal care increases when their offspring are lacking grandmaternal support. Indeed, there is an inverse relationship between the paternal and grandmaternal investment across Aka camps (Meehan, Reference Meehan2005), and among the Martu, the substantial care provided by grandmothers is speculated to underpin the low paternal effort (Scelza, Reference Scelza2009).

Unrelated campmates

We found unrelated campmates to have the largest aggregate involvement alongside siblings in both the Agta and BaYaka; this is the case in other hunter–gatherer societies where their contribution has been calculated (Table 6). About half of this involvement was of reproductively active adults. Their availability is a result of bilocal residence patterns which results in low relatedness between households in a camp (Dyble et al., Reference Dyble, Salali, Chaudhary, Page, Smith, Thompson and Migliano2015). This is distinct from cooperative breeding, which evolves in the context of high relatedness (Lukas and Clutton-Brock, Reference Lukas and Clutton-Brock2012a).

Using the motes, we previously found that Agta allomothering of unrelated children is best predicted by reciprocity (Page et al., Reference Page, Thomas, Smith, Dyble, Viguier, Chaudhary and Migliano2019). Thus, the involvement of unrelated reproductively active adults can be effectively characterised as communal breeding – multiple reproductive pairs sharing caregiving responsibilities with one another. This is arguably the only contribution that fits neatly within the existing classification schemes.

Limitations

The primary limitation of this study is that the motes do not provide information on what is occurring when a potential allomother is in close proximity to a child. Certain categories of allomother may be more likely to be providing high investment care, as observed among Martu Grandmothers (Scelza, Reference Scelza2009). Additionally, some categories of helper may be more skilled at providing care. It has been suggested elsewhere that subadult allomothers may be ‘learning to mother’ (Crittenden & Marlowe, Reference Crittenden and Marlowe2008; Fairbanks, Reference Fairbanks1989); therefore, it may take time before their care will match the quality of care of older caregivers. Equally, some allomothers may be more likely to spend time in close proximity to a child when no other potential allomothers are around. For all these reasons, a potential allomother's involvement in a child's close-proximity network cannot be assumed to be equivalent to the value of their allomaternal contribution. Despite these limitations, we can consider close proximity as the minimum form of, and minimum requirement for, allocare. Therefore, the results here still provide insight into the floor and ceiling of the contributions to childcare provided by each category of allomother. The fact that our results are mirrored in childcare studies measuring other caregiving behaviours provides support for their validity.

The contributions allomothers make via provisioning may follow different trends to those presented here. In particular, provisioning systems do not rely so heavily on subadults and siblings, who have been shown to remain net consumers of calories for an extended period in other hunter–gatherer societies (Kaplan et al., Reference Kaplan, Hill, Lancaster and Hurtado2000), although their contribution to household provisioning should not be undervalued since they may target different foods to adults, thus increasing dietary breadth (Crittenden et al., Reference Crittenden, Conklin-Brittain, Zes, Schoeninger and Marlowe2013; Kramer, Reference Kramer2014). Equally, the importance of fathers and grandmothers in food provisioning is probably higher than their involvement in close-proximity networks. In fact, there may be some inverse relationship since when fathers are hunting they cannot also be in close proximity to their infants. Provisioning contributions are also likely more variable across ecologies given that the sex division of labour and subadult foraging activities are determined by diverse dietary niches (Kramer, Reference Kramer2005; Marlowe, Reference Marlowe2005a).

Finally, accurately valuing the contributions of different allomothers should ideally involve analysing the effect of their help on the fitness of the children receiving the care and their mothers.

Given these limitations, our results do not offer a full valuation of the allomaternal contributions of different helpers and cannot be considered to be a complete description of the allomothering system.

Conclusion

We lived as hunter–gatherers for the majority of our evolutionary history, thus studying contemporary hunter–gatherers is particularly informative for our understanding of the evolution of human allomothering. Humans are frequently described as cooperative breeders. However, our results highlight that allomothering is widely distributed; it involves reproductively active and inactive, and related and unrelated, helpers.

The forces shaping the availability and involvement of the helpers who are most involved have some similarities to those operating in non-human cooperative and communal breeding systems. However, with the exception of help from reproductively active adults outside of the household, which resembles communal breeding, none of the key contributions fit neatly into existing classification schemes. Moreover, traditional definitions of cooperative breeding focus on help provided by sexually mature individuals who are reproductively inactive; among both the Agta and BaYaka, the involvement of these individuals is very limited. If the purpose of inter-specific classification schemes is to identify similarities across taxa and investigate potentially overlapping evolutionary processes, then it is unhelpful to classify hunter–gatherer allomothering as cooperative or communal breeding. It has some similarities with both systems but also has several features found in neither.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2024.1.

Acknowledgements

We thank all the BaYaka and Agta involved for their participation and hospitality and all our field assistants and translators for their hard work. Thanks are due to Jerome Lewis and Rudolph Schlaepfer for their help in organising and managing the fieldwork. We also thank the three anonymous reviewers and the editor, Rebecca Sear, for their valuable feedback and suggestions throughout the review process.

Author contributions

NC conceived of the study. NC, MD, AEP, GDS, DS, JT, SV collected the data. SV designed and developed the motes. NC analysed the data and AEP helped with data preparation. NC wrote the manuscript with the help ABM and LV, and also received and incorporated feedback from all other authors.

Financial support

This work was supported by the Leverhulme Trust (ABM, grant number RP2011-R-045). DM-S was also supported by the John Templeton Foundation (grant ID: 61917).

Conflicts of interest

None of the authors declare any conflicts of interest.

Research transparency and reproducibility

Processed data that support the findings of this study are available here: https://osf.io/n4b9e/

References

Arnold, K. E., & Owens, I. P. F. (1998). Cooperative breeding in birds: A comparative test of the life history hypothesis. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 265(1398). https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1998.0355CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auclair, Y., König, B., Ferrari, M., Perony, N., & Lindholm, A. K. (2014). Nest attendance of lactating females in a wild house mouse population: benefits associated with communal nesting. Animal Behaviour, 92, 143149. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ANBEHAV.2014.03.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baden, A. L., Wright, P. C., Louis, E. E., & Bradley, B. J. (2013). Communal nesting, kinship, and maternal success in a social primate. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 67(12), 19391950. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00265-013-1601-Y/TABLES/2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bebbington, K., Fairfield, E. A., Spurgin, L. G., Kingma, S. A., Dugdale, H., Komdeur, J., & Richardson, D. S. (2018). Joint care can outweigh costs of nonkin competition in communal breeders. Behavioral Ecology, 29(1), 169178. https://doi.org/10.1093/BEHECO/ARX137CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bogin, B., Bragg, J., & Kuzawa, C. (2014). Humans are not cooperative breeders but practice biocultural reproduction. Annals of Human Biology, 41(4). https://doi.org/10.3109/03014460.2014.923938CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burkart, J. M., Hrdy, S. B., & Van Schaik, C. P. (2009). Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 18(5). https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burkart, J. M., Van Schaik, C., & Griesser, M. (2017). Looking for unity in diversity: Human cooperative childcare in comparative perspective. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284(1869). https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.1184Google ScholarPubMed
Chaudhary, N., & Swanepoel, A. (2023). Editorial perspective: What can we learn from hunter–gatherers about children's mental health? An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1111/JCPP.13773CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chaudhary, N., Salali, G. D., Thompson, J., Dyble, M., Page, A., Smith, D., … Migliano, A. B. (2015). Polygyny without wealth: Popularity in gift games predicts polygyny in BaYaka Pygmies. Royal Society Open Science, 2(5). https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.150054CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chaudhary, N., Salali, G. D., Thompson, J., Rey, A., Gerbault, P., Stevenson, E. G. J., … Migliano, A. B. (2016). Competition for Cooperation: Variability, benefits and heritability of relational wealth in hunter–gatherers. Scientific Reports, 6. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep29120CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clutton-Brock, T. (2002). Breeding together: Kin selection and mutualism in cooperative vertebrates. Science, 296(5565), 6972. https://doi.org/10.1126/SCIENCE.296.5565.69CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cockburn, A. (2006). Prevalence of different modes of parental care in birds. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 273(1592), 13751383. https://doi.org/10.1098/RSPB.2005.3458CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornwallis, C. K., West, S. A., & Griffin, A. S. (2009). Routes to indirect fitness in cooperatively breeding vertebrates: Kin discrimination and limited dispersal. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 22(12). https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01853.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornwallis, C. K., West, S. A., Davis, K. E., & Griffin, A. S. (2010). Promiscuity and the evolutionary transition to complex societies. Nature, 466(7309), 969972. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09335CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crittenden, A. N., & Marlowe, F. W. (2008). Allomaternal care among the Hadza of Tanzania. Human Nature, 19(3). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-008-9043-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crittenden, A. N., Conklin-Brittain, N. L., Zes, D. A., Schoeninger, M. J., & Marlowe, F. W. (2013). Juvenile foraging among the Hadza: Implications for human life history. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(4), 299304. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.EVOLHUMBEHAV.2013.04.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diekmann, Y., Smith, D., Gerbault, P., Dyble, M., Page, A. E., Chaudhary, N., … Thomas, M. G. (2017). Accurate age estimation in small-scale societies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(31). https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1619583114Google ScholarPubMed
Dyble, M., Salali, G. D., Chaudhary, N., Page, A., Smith, D., Thompson, J., … Migliano, A. B. (2015). Sex equality can explain the unique social structure of hunter–gatherer bands. Science, 348(6236). https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaa5139CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dyble, M., Thompson, J., Smith, D., Salali, G. D., Chaudhary, N., Page, A. E., … Migliano, A. B. (2016). Networks of food sharing reveal the functional significance of multilevel sociality in two hunter–gatherer groups. Current Biology, 26(15). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.05.064CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dyble, M., Thorley, J., Page, A. E., Smith, D., and Migliano, A. B. (2019) Engagement in agricultural work is associated with reduced leisure time among Agta hunter-gatherers. Nature Human Behaviour. http://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0614-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emlen, S. T. (1982). The evolution of helping. I. An ecological constraints model. The American Naturalist, 119(1). https://doi.org/10.1086/283888CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fairbanks, L. A. (1989). Early experience and cross-generational continuity of mother–infant contact in vervet monkeys. Developmental Psychobiology, 22(7). https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.420220703CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilchrist, J. S., Otali, E., & Mwanguhya, F. (2004). Why breed communally? Factors affecting fecundity in a communal breeding mammal: The banded mongoose (Mungos mungo). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 57(2), 119131. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00265-004-0837-Y/FIGURES/7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, M. J., Griffin, P. B., Estioko-Griffin, A. A., & Grove, J. S. (1985). The compatibility of hunting and mothering among the Agta hunter–gatherers of the Philippines. Sex Roles, 12(11–12), 11991209. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00287829/METRICSCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griesser, M., Drobniak, S. M., Nakagawa, S., & Botero, C. A. (2017). Family living sets the stage for cooperative breeding and ecological resilience in birds. PLOS Biology, 15(6), e2000483. https://doi.org/10.1371/JOURNAL.PBIO.2000483CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurven, M., Hill, K., Kaplan, H., Hurtado, A., & Lyles, R. (2000). Food transfers among Hiwi foragers of Venezuela: Tests of reciprocity. Human Ecology, 28(2). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007067919982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatchwell, B. J. (2009). The evolution of cooperative breeding in birds: Kinship, dispersal and life history. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1533). https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkes, K. (2003). Grandmothers and the evolution of human longevity. American Journal of Human Biology, 15(3). https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.10156CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkes, K., & Coxworth, J. E. (2013). Grandmothers and the evolution of human longevity: A review of findings and future directions. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 22(6), 294302. https://doi.org/10.1002/EVAN.21382Google Scholar
Hawkes, K., O'Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1997). Hadza women's time allocation, offspring provisioning, and the evolution of long postmenopausal life spans. Anthropology, 38(4), 551577. https://doi.org/10.1086/204646Google Scholar
Hawkes, K., O'Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (2001). Hunting and nuclear families: Some lessons from the Hadza about men's work. Current Anthropology, 42(5). https://doi.org/10.1086/322559CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkes, K., O'Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (2014). More Lessons from the Hadza about men's work. Human Nature, 25(4). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-014-9212-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkes, K., O'Connell, J. F., Blurton Jones, N. G., Alvarez, H., & Charnov, E. L. (1998). Grandmothering, menopause, and the evolution of human life histories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95(3). https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.3.1336Google ScholarPubMed
Heinsohn, R. G. (1991). Slow learning of foraging skills and extended parental care in cooperatively breeding white-winged choughs. Source: The American Naturalist, 137(6), 864881. https://www.jstor.org/stable/2462405Google Scholar
Helfrecht, C., Roulette, J. W., Lane, A., Sintayehu, B., & Meehan, C. L. (2020). Life history and socioecology of infancy. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 173(4), 619629. https://doi.org/10.1002/AJPA.24145CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hewlett, B. (1991). Intimate fathers. University of Michigan Press. https://doi.org/10.3998/mpub.13211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. E. (2009). Integrating evolution, culture and developmental psychology: Explaining caregiver-infant proximity and responsiveness in central Africa and the USA. In Keller, H., & Poortinga, Y.H. (Eds.), Between culture and biology: Perspectives on ontogenetic development (pp. 241269). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (1996). Ache life history. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781351329248Google Scholar
Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (2009). Cooperative breeding in South American hunter–gatherers. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1674). https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1061Google ScholarPubMed
Hrdy, S. B. (2007). Evolutionary context of human development: The cooperative breeding model. In Family relationships: An evolutionary perspective. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195320510.003.0003Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (2009). Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Ivey, P. K. (2000). Cooperative reproduction in Ituri forest Hunter–Gatherers: Who cares for Efe infants? Current Anthropology, 41(5). https://doi.org/10.1086/317414CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jang, H., Janmaat, K. R. L., Kandza, V., & Boyette, A. H. (2022). Girls in early childhood increase food returns of nursing women during subsistence activities of the BaYaka in the Republic of Congo. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 289(1987). https://doi.org/10.1098/RSPB.2022.1407Google ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9(4). https://doi.org/10.1002/1520-6505(2000)9:4&lt;156::AID-EVAN5>3.0.CO;2-73.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, R. L. (2013). The lifeways of hunter–gatherers. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9781139176132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleiman, D. G., & Malcolm, J. R. (1981). The evolution of male parental investment in mammals. In Gubernick, D.J., & Klopfer, P.H. (Eds.), Parental care in mammals (pp. 347387). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knight ID, J. K., Deniz Salali, G., Sikka, G., Derkx, I., Keestra ID, S. M., & Chaudhary, N. I. (2021). Quantifying patterns of alcohol consumption and its effects on health and wellbeing among BaYaka hunter–gatherers: A mixed-methods cross-sectional study. PLOS ONE, 16(10), e0258384. https://doi.org/10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0258384CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koenig, W. D., Pitelka, F. A., Carmen, W. J., Mumme, R. L., & Stanback, M. T. (1992). The evolution of delayed dispersal in cooperative breeders. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 67(2), 111150. https://doi.org/10.1086/417552CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kokko, H., Johnstone, R. A., & Wright, J. (2002). The evolution of parental and alloparental effort in cooperatively breeding groups: When should helpers pay to stay? Behavioral Ecology, 13(3), 291300. https://doi.org/10.1093/BEHECO/13.3.291CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Konner, M. (2018). Hunter–gatherer infancy and childhood. In Hunter–gatherer childhoods (pp. 1964). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203789445-3Google Scholar
Kramer, K. L. (2005). Children's help and the pace of reproduction: Cooperative breeding in humans. Evolutionary Anthropology, 14(6). https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20082CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kramer, K. L. (2010). Cooperative breeding and its significance to the demographic success of humans. Annual Review of Anthropology, 39. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.012809.105054CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kramer, K. L. (2014). Why what juveniles do matters in the evolution of cooperative breeding. Human Nature, 25(1). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-013-9189-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kruger, A. C., & Konner, M. (2010). Who responds to crying?: Maternal care and allocare among the!Kung. Human Nature, 21(3), 309329. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-010-9095-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, S. E., & Pusey, A. E. (1997). Factors influencing the occurrence of communal care in plural breeding mammals. In Solomon, N.G., & French, J.A. (Eds.), Cooperative breeding in mammals (pp. 335363). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lew-Levy, S., Lavi, N., Reckin, R., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., & Ellis-Davies, K. (2018). How do hunter–gatherer children learn social and gender norms? A meta-ethnographic review. Cross-Cultural Research, 52(2). https://doi.org/10.1177/1069397117723552CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lew-Levy, S., Reckin, R., Kissler, S. M., Pretelli, I., Boyette, A. H., Crittenden, A. N., …, Davis, H. E. (2022). Socioecology shapes child and adolescent time allocation in twelve hunter–gatherer and mixed-subsistence forager societies. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 116. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-12217-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lew-Levy, S., Reckin, R., Lavi, N., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., & Ellis-Davies, K. (2017). How do hunter–gatherer children learn subsistence skills?: A meta-ethnographic review. Human Nature, 28(4), 367394. https://doi.org/10.1007/S12110-017-9302-2/TABLES/1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovejoy, C. O. (1981). The origin of man. Science, 211(4480). https://doi.org/10.1126/science.211.4480.341CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lukas, D., & Clutton-Brock, T. (2012a). Cooperative breeding and monogamy in mammalian societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1736). https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2011.2468Google ScholarPubMed
Lukas, D., & Clutton-Brock, T. (2012b). Life histories and the evolution of cooperative breeding in mammals. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1744), 40654070. https://doi.org/10.1098/RSPB.2012.1433CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lukas, D., & Clutton-Brock, T. H. (2013). The evolution of social monogamy in mammals. Science, 341(6145), 526530. https://doi.org/10.1126/SCIENCE.1238677/SUPPL_FILE/LUKAS.SM.PDFCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lukas, D., & Clutton-Brock, T. (2016). Climate and the distribution of cooperative breeding in mammals. Royal Society Open Science, 4(1). https://doi.org/10.1098/RSOS.160897Google Scholar
Major-Smith, D., Chaudhary, N., Dyble, M., Major-Smith, K., Page, A. E., Salali, G. D., … Migliano, A. B. (2023). Cooperation and partner choice among Agta hunter–gatherer children: An evolutionary developmental perspective. PLOS ONE, 18(4), e0284360. https://doi.org/10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0284360CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marlowe, F. W. (2003). A critical period for provisioning by Hadza men. Implications for pair bonding. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24(3). https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(03)00014-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marlowe, F. W. (2005a). Hunter–gatherers and human evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 14(2). https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20046CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marlowe, F. W. (2005b). Who tends Hadza children? In Hewlett, B. S. & Lamb, M. E. (Eds.), Hunter–gatherer childhoods (pp. 177190). Aldine Transaction. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203789445-13Google Scholar
Meehan, C. L. (2005). The effects of residential locality on parental and alloparental investment among the Aka foragers of the Central African Republic. Human Nature, 16(1), 5880. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-005-1007-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meehan, C. L., Quinlan, R., & Malcom, C. D. (2013). Cooperative breeding and maternal energy expenditure among Aka foragers. American Journal of Human Biology, 25(1). https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.22336CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meehan, C. L., Lackey, K. A., Hagen, E. H., Williams, J. E., Roulette, J., Helfrecht, C., … McGuire, M. K. (2018). Social networks, cooperative breeding, and the human milk microbiome. American Journal of Human Biology, 30(4), e23131. https://doi.org/10.1002/AJHB.23131CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesman, J., Minter, T., Angnged, A., Cissé, I. A. H., Salali, G. D., & Migliano, A. B. (2018). Universality without uniformity: A culturally inclusive approach to sensitive responsiveness in infant caregiving. Child Development, 89(3). https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12795CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Migliano, A. B., Vinicius, L., & Lahr, M. M. (2007). Life history trade-offs explain the evolution of human pygmies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(51). https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0708024105Google ScholarPubMed
Opie, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Dunbar, R. I. M., & Shultz, S. (2013). Male infanticide leads to social monogamy in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(33), 1332813332. https://doi.org/10.1073/PNAS.1307903110/SUPPL_FILE/SD01.XLSXCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Packer, C., Pusey, A. E., & Eberly, L. E. (2001). Egalitarianism in female African lions. Science, 293(5530), 690693. https://doi.org/10.1126/SCIENCE.1062320/ASSET/76C705D7-8EDE-443B-B50F-78EF00D0D86F/ASSETS/GRAPHIC/SE291960704A.JPEGCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Page, A. E., Chaudhary, N., Viguier, S., Dyble, M., Thompson, J., Smith, D., … Migliano, A. B. (2017). Hunter–gatherer social networks and reproductive success. Scientific Reports, 7(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-01310-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Page, A. E., Emmott, E. H., Dyble, M., Smith, D., Chaudhary, N., Viguier, S., & Migliano, A. B. (2021). Children are important too: Juvenile playgroups and maternal childcare in a foraging population, the Agta. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376(1827), rstb.2020.0026. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0026CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Page, A. E., Thomas, M. G., Smith, D., Dyble, M., Viguier, S., Chaudhary, N., …, Migliano, A. B. (2019). Testing adaptive hypotheses of alloparenting in Agta foragers. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(11). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0679-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennington, R. (2001). Hunter–gatherer demography. In Panter-Brick, C., Layton, R., & Rowley-Conwy, P. (Eds.), Hunter–gatherers: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 170204). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Piper, W. H., Parker, P. G., & Rabenold, K. N. (1995). Facultative dispersal by juvenile males in the cooperative stripe-backed wren. Behavioral Ecology, 6(3), 337342. https://doi.org/10.1093/BEHECO/6.3.337CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pusey, A. E., & Packer, C. (1994). Non-offspring nursing in social carnivores: Minimizing the costs. Behavioral Ecology, 5, 362374. https://academic.oup.com/beheco/article/5/4/362/203711CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rendell, L., Cantor, M., Gero, S., Whitehead, H., & Mann, J. (2019). Causes and consequences of female centrality in cetacean societies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 374(1780). https://doi.org/10.1098/RSTB.2018.0066CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riehl, C. (2013). Evolutionary routes to non-kin cooperative breeding in birds. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280(1772). https://doi.org/10.1098/RSPB.2013.2245Google ScholarPubMed
Rubenstein, D. R., & Lovette, I. J. (2007). Temporal environmental variability drives the evolution of cooperative breeding in birds. Current Biology, 17(16), 14141419. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CUB.2007.07.032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salali, G. D., Chaudhary, N., Bouer, J., Thompson, J., Vinicius, L., & Migliano, A. B. (2019). Development of social learning and play in BaYaka hunter–gatherers of Congo. Scientific Reports, 9(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-47515-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salali, G. D., Chaudhary, N., Thompson, J., Grace, O. M., van der Burgt, X. M., Dyble, M., …, Migliano, A. B. (2016). Knowledge-sharing networks in hunter–gatherers and the evolution of cumulative culture. Current Biology, 26(18). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.07.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salali, G. D., Dyble, M., Chaudhary, N., Sikka, G., Derkx, I., Keestra, S. M., …, Migliano, A. B. (2020). Global WEIRDing: Transitions in wild plant knowledge and treatment preferences in Congo hunter–gatherers. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 2. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2020.26CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scelza, B. A. (2009). The grandmaternal niche: Critical caretaking among Martu Aborigines. American Journal of Human Biology, 21(4). https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.20934CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scelza, B. A., & Hinde, K. (2019). Crucial contributions: A biocultural study of grandmothering during the perinatal period. Human Nature, 30(4), 371397. https://doi.org/10.1007/S12110-019-09356-2/CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sear, R., & Mace, R. (2008). Who keeps children alive? A review of the effects of kin on child survival. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(1). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherley, G. H. (1990). Co-operative breeding in riflemen (Acanthissitta chloris) benefits to parents, offspring and helpers. Behaviour, 112(1–2), 122. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853990X00653CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, D., Dyble, M., Major, K., Page, A. E., Chaudhary, N., Salali, G. D., …, Mace, R. (2019). A friend in need is a friend indeed: Need-based sharing, rather than cooperative assortment, predicts experimental resource transfers among Agta hunter–gatherers. Evolution and Human Behavior, 40(1). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.08.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, D., Schlaepfer, P., Major, K., Dyble, M., Page, A. E., Thompson, J., …, Migliano, A. B. (2017). Cooperation and the evolution of hunter–gatherer storytelling. Nature Communications, 8(1), 19. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-017-02036-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strassmann, B. I., & Kurapati, N. T. (2010). Are humans cooperative breeders?: Most studies of natural fertility populations do not support the grandmother hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(1), 3539. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X09991749CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, B. M., & Marlowe, F. W. (2014). Toward a reality-based understanding of Hadza men's work: A response to Hawkes et al. (2014). Human Nature, 25(4). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-014-9218-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodburn, J. (1982). Egalitarian societies. Man, 17(3), 431451. https://doi.org/10.2307/2801707CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, J. J., He, Q. Q., Deng, L. L., Wang, S. C., Mace, R., Ji, T., & Tao, Y. (2013). Communal breeding promotes a matrilineal social system where husband and wife live apart. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280(1758). https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.0010Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Glossary of key measures

Figure 1

Table 2. Focal children demographic characteristics and potential allomother availability

Figure 2

Figure 1. Aggregate and adjusted involvement of different categories of potential allomother. Error bars represent standard errors. The following are calculated by averaging the data from the close-proximity networks of all children in a given population. (a and b) The aggregate involvement of each category of potential allomother (n = 46 children (29 Agta)). GMs refers to grandmothers and GFs refers to grandfathers. (c and d) The adjusted involvement of each category of potential allomother (n = 46 children [29 Agta]). (e and f) The adjusted involvement of an average relative (av), and the most involved relative (first), with coefficients of relatedness 0.125/0.25/0.5 respectively, and the involvement of the six most involved unrelated campmates (n = 47 children (28 Agta)). For example, child x has two relatives with whom they share a coefficient of relatedness of r = 0.25, who were responsible for 20 and 10% of their close proximity interactions respectively. For this child, the adjusted involvement of r = 0.25 (av) would be 15%, and of r = 0.25 (first) would be 20%.

Figure 3

Table 3. Aggregate involvement (%) of potential allomothers by relationship type and child age-group

Figure 4

Figure 2. Both reproductively active and inactive camp members are involved in children's close-proximity networks. Results for the Agta in red and the BaYaka in blue. Error bars represent standard errors. See methods for definitions of life-stages and reproductive status. (a and b) The inner-ring is the aggregate involvement of reproductively active and inactive campmates respectively; the outer ring dissects this involvement by life-stage. (n = 49 children (30 Agta)). (c and d) The adjusted involvement of children's most involved (first) and average (av) potential allomother of each life-stage (n = 49 children (30 Agta)).

Figure 5

Table 4. Aggregate involvement split by life-stage and relatedness status

Figure 6

Table 5. Aggregate involvement (%) of potential allomothers by life-stage and child age-group

Figure 7

Table 6. Comparison of allocare trends across hunter–gatherer societies

Supplementary material: File

Chaudhary et al. supplementary material

Chaudhary et al. supplementary material
Download Chaudhary et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.9 MB