Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-vpsfw Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-16T18:45:52.928Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Negative attitudes toward older adults: Subjective time to become older and “stereotype embodiment theory”-based intervention

Subject: Psychology and Psychiatry

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 September 2022

Yuho Shimizu*
Affiliation:
Graduate School of Humanities and Sociology, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan Research Fellow, Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Tokyo, Japan
*
Corresponding author. Email: yuhos1120mizu@gmail.com

Abstract

Reducing negative attitudes toward older adults is an urgent issue. A previous study has conducted “stereotype embodiment theory”-based interventions (SET interventions) that present participants with the contents of SET and related empirical findings. I focus on the subjective time to become older (the perception of how long people feel it will be before they become old) as a mechanism for the effect of SET interventions. I make the SET intervention group and the control group in which the participants are presented with an irrelevant vignette. The data from 641 participants (M = 31.97 years) were analyzed. Consequently, the SET intervention shortened the subjective time to become older and reduced negative attitudes toward older adults. When considering SET interventions, it would be useful to focus not only on the self-interested motives to avoid age discrimination but also on the subjective time to become older.

Type
Research Article
Information
Result type: Supplementary result
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

In various countries including Japan, negative attitudes held by young people toward older adults have been examined. For example, older adults are incompetent, outdated, and stubborn (Harada et al., Reference Harada, Sugisawa, Sugihara, Yamada and Shibata2004; Lytle et al., Reference Lytle, Apriceno, Macdonald, Monahan and Levy2020) and such attitudes lead to lower mental health among older adults, such as less well-being and sexual expression (Syme & Cohn, Reference Syme and Cohn2021). Accordingly, reducing negative attitudes toward older adults is an urgent issue.

Meanwhile, the social group of older adults differs significantly from other discriminated groups such as racial/sexual minorities in that most people belong to it someday. Levy’s (Reference Levy2009) stereotype embodiment theory (SET) is useful for considering this point. SET assumes that old-age stereotypes are internalized throughout life and affect the self undesirably, such as lower cognitive/physical functions (Shimizu et al., Reference Shimizu, Hashimoto and Karasawa2021). Based on this, Shimizu et al. (Reference Shimizu, Hashimoto and Karasawa2022b) conducted the “SET intervention,” in which the contents of SET and related empirical findings are presented to participants, and reduced anti-old attitudes. As a mechanism for the intervention’s effects, the authors focus on the motives to avoid age discrimination. Specifically, they show that not only the altruistic motives for the sake of older adults but also the self-interested motives to avoid undesirable effects on their own future were activated (Shimizu et al., Reference Shimizu, Hashimoto and Karasawa2022b).

Objective

This study aims is to investigate other possible mechanisms of SET interventions. Specifically, I focus on subjective time to become older (the perception of how long people feel it will be before they become old; Shimizu, Reference Shimizu2021). The SET intervention may activate the perception that becoming an older person is not far in the future. This will lead the participants to perceive older adults as being closer to their ingroup rather than an outgroup (North & Fiske, Reference North and Fiske2012; Van Wicklin, Reference Van Wicklin2020).

In this study, I make the SET intervention group and the control group in which participants are presented with an irrelevant vignette about the Internet. The hypothesis is that the SET intervention shortens subjective time to become older and reduces negative attitudes toward older adults.

Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited using CrowdWorks, a major crowdsourcing service in Japan. Participants were 720 Japanese, and those who answered incorrectly to the vignette reading check (see below; N = 74) and who answered incorrectly to the item “Please choose the second from the right” (N = 6) were excluded from the analysis (including duplicates). Consequently, the data used in the analysis consisted of N = 641 (aged 18–39; 232 males and 409 females) with a mean age of 31.97 years (SD = 5.35). Similar results to the main manuscript were obtained when analyzing data from all participants without screening (see Open Science Framework [OSF] repository, https://osf.io/frhxq/). This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Tokyo and was conducted in May 2022. Informed consent was obtained from all the participants.

Procedure

Participants answered items on negative attitudes toward older adults (time 1). They were then randomly assigned to either the SET intervention group (N = 330; 114 males and 216 females) or the control group (N = 311; 118 males and 193 females). Participants carefully read the vignettes (see below) for each group. No time limit was set for reading the vignettes. After reading one of the vignettes, participants answered the following items: subjective time to become older, motives to avoid age discrimination (self-interested/altruistic), youth identity, and contact experience with older adults (quantitative/qualitative aspects). Then, participants responded to the items on negative attitudes toward older adults (time 2), the vignette reading check, and demographics.

Vignette and reading check

For the SET intervention group, I used the vignette of Study 2 of Shimizu et al. (Reference Shimizu, Hashimoto and Karasawa2022b). The vignette consisted of the contents of SET and related empirical findings (Levy et al., Reference Levy, Ashman and Dror2000; Reference Levy, Slade, May and Caracciolo2006; Westerhof et al., Reference Westerhof, Miche, Brothers, Barrett, Diehl, Montepare, Wahl and Wurm2014). For the control group, I used the vignette of Shimizu et al. (Reference Shimizu, Hashimoto and Karasawa2022b), which was based on Ikeda (Reference Ikeda, Ikeda, Karasawa, Kudo and Muramoto2010). The full text of each vignette was posted on OSF.

For the vignette reading check, the SET intervention group answered the single item “We internalize discriminatory attitudes toward older adults throughout our lives and turn them against ourselves,” and the control group answered the single item “The media as a place where people can be active is called “participatory” media.” Each participant responded using “yes” or “no.” Only those participants who answered this item correctly (“yes” for both) were included in the analysis.

Measurements

Negative attitudes toward older adults were measured by the Japanese version of the Fraboni Scale of Ageism (Harada et al., Reference Harada, Sugisawa, Sugihara, Yamada and Shibata2004), which consists of 14 items (5-point Likert scale), including “Many older people are stingy and hoard their money and possessions.” The mean score was calculated (α = .89 for both times 1 and 2), with the higher score indicating the more negative attitudes.

Subjective time to become older was measured by two items (all of the following were 7-point Likert scale), including “Do you feel that you are far from being an older person?” The mean score was calculated (r = .88, 95%CI = [.86, .90], p < .001), with the higher score indicating the longer subjective time (Shimizu et al., Reference Shimizu, Hashimoto and Karasawa2022a).

Self-interested motives to avoid age discrimination were measured by the single item “Age discrimination should be avoided because it can negatively affect my future.” Altruistic motives to avoid age discrimination were measured by the single item “Age discrimination should be avoided because it can negatively affect older adults” (Shimizu et al., Reference Shimizu, Hashimoto and Karasawa2022b).

Youth identity was measured by the Japanese version of the Group Identification Scale (Uemura, Reference Uemura2001), which consists of seven items, including “Would you think it is accurate if you were described as a typical person of the youth?” The mean score was calculated (α = .86), with the higher score indicating the stronger youth identity.

Contact experience with older adults (quantitative aspect) was measured by the single item “Do you have much contact with older adults on a daily basis?” Contact experience (qualitative aspect) was measured by two items, including “When you have daily contact with older adults, do you feel that it is a friendly relationship?” The mean score was calculated (r = .81, 95%CI = [.78, .83], p < .001), with the higher score indicating the higher quality of contact experience (Shimizu et al., Reference Shimizu, Hashimoto and Karasawa2022c).

As demographic variables, participants were asked about their subjective wealth, age, and gender. Scale items, the data used in the analysis, the R scripts, and the histograms for each variable are posted on OSF.

Results

Means, standard deviations, and correlation coefficients for each indicator are shown in Table 1. Participants’ age of the SET intervention group (M = 31.77, SD = 5.48) and control group (M = 32.17, SD = 5.20) did not differ significantly (t(639) = 0.96, p = .34, d = 0.08). Meanwhile, a Welch’s t-test showed that the SET intervention group (M = 5.88, SD = 1.27) had higher self-interested motives than the control (M = 4.86, SD = 1.39; t(626) = 9.72, p < .001, d = 0.77). This result was similar to the previous study (Shimizu et al., Reference Shimizu, Hashimoto and Karasawa2022b). Moreover, a Welch’s t-test showed that the SET intervention group (M = 3.48, SD = 1.59) had a shorter subjective time than the control (M = 4.00, SD = 1.50; t(639) = 4.27, p < .001, d = 0.34). In sum, it suggested that SET interventions shortened participants’ subjective time to become older.

Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and correlation coefficients in each indicator

* p < .05;

** p < .01.

To examine the intervention’s effect, an analysis of the generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) was conducted; negative attitudes toward older adults were the dependent variables. The fixed factors of GLMM were the groups (SET intervention/control), the timings of the measurement (time 1/time 2), and interactions between these factors. The random effects were participants and participants’ subjective time to become older, youth identity, contact experience (quantitative/qualitative aspects), subjective wealth, age, and gender. Results showed a significant interaction between the groups and the timings (t(639) = 8.76, p < .001). Multiple comparisons revealed that negative attitudes were weaker in the SET intervention group at time 2 than at time 1 (t(639) = 13.97, p < .001). At time 2, negative attitudes were weaker in the SET intervention group than in the control (t(702) = 2.36, p = .02). Consequently, I believe that the SET intervention is effective in reducing anti-old attitudes.

A structural equation modeling was conducted with groups as the independent variable, negative attitudes toward older adults (time 2) as the dependent variable, and subjective time to become older as the mediating variable (Figure 1). I controlled for negative attitudes (time 1), youth identity, contact experience (quantitative/qualitative aspects), subjective wealth, age, and gender. The model showed a satisfactory fit of index (RMSEA < .001, AGFI > .999), and the SET intervention group had a shorter subjective time to become older than the control (β = −.16, p < .001). The longer the subjective time, the stronger the negative attitudes (β = .05, p = .007). These findings suggested that the SET intervention shortened participants’ subjective time to become older and reduced negative attitudes toward older adults: the hypothesis was supported.

Figure 1. Results of a structural equation modeling (**p < .01).

Discussion

In this study, I focused on subjective time to become older as a mechanism for the effect of SET interventions. The results showed that the subjective time to become older was shorter in the SET intervention group than the control. Moreover, the results of structural equation modeling suggested that the SET intervention shortened the subjective time to become older and reduced negative attitudes toward older adults. The perception that becoming older was in the not-too-distant future might lead to less anti-old attitudes (North & Fiske, Reference North and Fiske2012; Van Wicklin, Reference Van Wicklin2020).

Despite the above findings, two major limitations exist. First, subjective time to become older was not measured prior to reading vignettes. Although this study showed that the subjective time was shorter in the SET intervention group than the control, I cannot determine whether this result was due to reading vignettes or sample bias. Second, I did not examine the effect of the subjective time on the duration of the intervention effect. Although the SET intervention is shown to be effective for about 1 week (Shimizu et al., Reference Shimizu, Hashimoto and Karasawa2022b), I cannot determine whether this is due to a shortened subjective time to become older. Therefore, future experiments should measure anti-old attitudes and the subjective time well after reading vignettes.

Conclusions

In this study, I focus on subjective time to become older as a mechanism for the effects of SET interventions. SET interventions appeal to self-interested motives to avoid age discrimination (Shimizu et al., Reference Shimizu, Hashimoto and Karasawa2022b), but fail to sufficiently enhance the altruistic motives. Therefore, future research should improve the intervention to increase both self-interested/altruistic motives to avoid age discrimination. Meanwhile, participants’ age in the previous study were diverse (Shimizu et al., Reference Shimizu, Hashimoto and Karasawa2022b), but the present study targeted younger adults aged 18–39. Since younger people have more anti-old attitudes than other generations (Foley et al., Reference Foley, Myrick and Yonge2013), it is expected that the SET intervention will be used practically in educational settings.

Data Availability Statement

The data used in the analysis is posted on the Open Science Framework (OSF) repository (https://osf.io/frhxq/).

Authorship Contributions

Y.S. designed the study, conducted data gathering, performed statistical analyses, and wrote the article.

Funding Statement

This research was supported by JSPS KAKENHI (22J20303).

Conflict of Interest

The author declares none.

References

Foley, V., Myrick, F., & Yonge, O. (2013). Intergenerational conflict in nursing preceptorship. Nurse Education Today, 33, 10031007. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nedt.2012.07.019Google ScholarPubMed
Harada, K., Sugisawa, H., Sugihara, Y., Yamada, Y., & Shibata, H. (2004). Development of a Japanese short version of the Fraboni scale of ageism (FSA): Measuring ageism among Japanese young men living in urban area. Japanese Journal of Gerontology, 26, 308319.Google Scholar
Ikeda, K. (2010). Mass media and the Internet. In Ikeda, K., Karasawa, M., Kudo, E., & Muramoto, Y. (Eds.), Social psychology: Active social animals in multi-layered constraints (pp. 267290). Yuhikaku.Google Scholar
Levy, B. R. (2009). Stereotype embodiment: A psychosocial approach to aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 332336. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01662.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, B. R., Ashman, O., & Dror, I. (2000). To be or not to be: The effects of aging stereotypes on the will to live. OMEGA-Journal of Death and Dying, 40, 409420. https://doi.org/10.2190/Y2GE-BVYQ-NF0E-83VRCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, B. R., Slade, M. D., May, J., & Caracciolo, E. A. (2006). Physical recovery after acute myocardial infarction: Positive age self-stereotypes as a resource. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 62, 285301. https://doi.org/10.2190/EJK1-1Q0D-LHGE-7A35CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lytle, A., Apriceno, M., Macdonald, J., Monahan, C., & Levy, S. R. (2020). Pre-pandemic ageism toward older adults predicts behavioral intentions during the COVID-19 pandemic. Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 77, 1115. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbaa210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
North, M. S., & Fiske, S. T. (2012). An inconvenienced youth? Ageism and its potential intergenerational roots. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 982997. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027843Google ScholarPubMed
Shimizu, Y. (2021). An overlooked perspective in psychological interventions to reduce anti-elderly discriminatory attitudes. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 765394. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.765394Google ScholarPubMed
Shimizu, Y., Hashimoto, T., & Karasawa, K. (2021). The complementation of the stereotype embodiment theory: Focusing on the social identity theory. Journal of Human Environmental Studies, 19, 914. https://doi.org/10.4189/shes.19.9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shimizu, Y., Hashimoto, T., & Karasawa, K. (2022a). Ageist attitudes: Youth identity, subjective time to become older, and impressions of the general older population’s physical and mental health. Advances in Gerontology, 12, 155159. https://doi.org/10.1134/S2079057022020163CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shimizu, Y., Hashimoto, T., & Karasawa, K. (2022b). Decreasing anti-elderly discriminatory attitudes: Conducting a ‘stereotype embodiment theory’-based intervention. European Journal of Social Psychology, 52, 174190. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2823Google Scholar
Shimizu, Y., Hashimoto, T., & Karasawa, K. (2022c). The relationship between disease avoidance and attitudes toward older people. Advances in Gerontology, 12, 242246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Syme, M. L., & Cohn, T. J. (2021). Aging sexual stereotypes and sexual expression in mid-and later life: Examining the stereotype matching effect. Aging and Mental Health, 25, 15071514. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2020.1758909Google ScholarPubMed
Uemura, Z. (2001). Ambiguity tolerance, group identification, and attitudes toward newcomer acceptance. Japanese Journal of Personality, 10, 2734. https://doi.org/10.2132/jjpjspp.10.1_27Google Scholar
Van Wicklin, S. A. (2020). Ageism in nursing. Plastic Surgical Nursing, 40, 2024. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSN.0000000000000290Google Scholar
Westerhof, G. J., Miche, M., Brothers, A. F., Barrett, A. E., Diehl, M., Montepare, J. M., Wahl, H.-W., & Wurm, S. (2014). The influence of subjective aging on health and longevity: A meta-analysis of longitudinal data. Psychology and Aging, 29, 793802. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and correlation coefficients in each indicator

Figure 1

Figure 1. Results of a structural equation modeling (**p < .01).

Reviewing editor:  Massimo Grassi Universita degli Studi di Padova Scuola di Psicologia, Department of General Psychology, via Venezia 8, Padova, Italy, 35131
Minor revisions requested.

Review 1: Negative Attitudes Toward Older Adults: Subjective Time to Become Older and ‘Stereotype Embodiment Theory’-based Intervention

Conflict of interest statement

None

Comments

Comments to the Author: In general, I find the idea motivating the study interesting. I have some comments on the paper that could improve the impact of the study.

In the abstract, information about subjects (n, age) should be added.

Overall, in both the introduction and methods, it is appreciated if the author adds more detail about some issues that in the present format of the manuscript are difficult to understand. For example, in the Introduction, the author should better specify the aim of the study. Moreover, I suggest better explaining the sentence on lines 35-36, as well as the SET (lines 41-42). Please, the author should also specify the context of the irrelevant vignettes (line 60).

In the method, I suggest putting more explanation about the intervention group and the control group (duration, trainer, etc).

Regarding the Results section, did the author check the equivalence of age and years of education between the intervention group and the control group?

In the discussion, more discussion about the results is needed, particularly regarding results of the structural equation modeling. Moreover, the findings of the study should be discussed in light of the existing literature.

Presentation

Overall score 2.6 out of 5
Is the article written in clear and proper English? (30%)
3 out of 5
Is the data presented in the most useful manner? (40%)
2 out of 5
Does the paper cite relevant and related articles appropriately? (30%)
3 out of 5

Context

Overall score 3.2 out of 5
Does the title suitably represent the article? (25%)
5 out of 5
Does the abstract correctly embody the content of the article? (25%)
4 out of 5
Does the introduction give appropriate context? (25%)
2 out of 5
Is the objective of the experiment clearly defined? (25%)
2 out of 5

Analysis

Overall score 2.8 out of 5
Does the discussion adequately interpret the results presented? (40%)
2 out of 5
Is the conclusion consistent with the results and discussion? (40%)
3 out of 5
Are the limitations of the experiment as well as the contributions of the experiment clearly outlined? (20%)
4 out of 5

Review 2: Negative Attitudes Toward Older Adults: Subjective Time to Become Older and ‘Stereotype Embodiment Theory’-based Intervention

Conflict of interest statement

no

Comments

Comments to the Author: The authors addressed a very interesting topic concerning the negative attitudes held by young people toward older adults and the effect on older’ mental health.

The study is well designed and appropriate to address the research question.

Can the author report how many males and females are included in the SET and control groups?

Presentation

Overall score 3.6 out of 5
Is the article written in clear and proper English? (30%)
4 out of 5
Is the data presented in the most useful manner? (40%)
3 out of 5
Does the paper cite relevant and related articles appropriately? (30%)
4 out of 5

Context

Overall score 4.2 out of 5
Does the title suitably represent the article? (25%)
4 out of 5
Does the abstract correctly embody the content of the article? (25%)
4 out of 5
Does the introduction give appropriate context? (25%)
4 out of 5
Is the objective of the experiment clearly defined? (25%)
5 out of 5

Analysis

Overall score 5 out of 5
Does the discussion adequately interpret the results presented? (40%)
5 out of 5
Is the conclusion consistent with the results and discussion? (40%)
5 out of 5
Are the limitations of the experiment as well as the contributions of the experiment clearly outlined? (20%)
5 out of 5