Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T10:47:31.803Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive decline in older adults: applying multiple perspectives to develop novel prevention strategies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 August 2019

Emily B. H. Treichler
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA Sam and Rose Stein Institute for Research on Aging, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center (MIRECC), VA San Diego, San Diego, CA, USA
Dilip V. Jeste*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA Sam and Rose Stein Institute for Research on Aging, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA Department of Neurosciences, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA Email: djeste@ucsd.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Editorial
Copyright
© International Psychogeriatric Association 2019 

Cognitive decline among older adults encompasses the range of subclinical decline, mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and Alzheimer-type dementia. In 2015, 47 million people worldwide were estimated to have dementia, and the prevalence is expected to triple by 2050 (Baumgart et al., Reference Baumgart, Snyder, Carrillo, Fazio, Kim and Johns2015). Alzheimer’s disease is the fifth most-common cause of death among adults 65 years and older in the United States, although this may be an underestimate, as dementia often leads to health conditions that are considered direct causes of death. Examples of these associated conditions include swallowing disorders and immobility, which increase the risk for potentially fatal pneumonia in older adults (Alzheimer’s Association, 2019). Given the gravity of these outcomes, combined with the finding that only 5% to 15% of people with MCI progress to Alzheimer’s disease in a given year (Mitchell and Shiri-Feshki, Reference Mitchell and Shiri-Feshki2008; Petersen, Reference Petersen2011), modifying potential risk factors through prevention strategies is of high importance. Consequently, there has been a growing body of research on identifying at-risk older adults and developing effective prevention strategies for those at risk of dementia.

Previous research has identified a number of potentially modifiable risk factors, including midlife obesity and hypertension, current smoking status, and diabetes, along with factors that decrease risk, such as years of formal education, diet, and participation in cognitive training activities (Baumgart et al., Reference Baumgart, Snyder, Carrillo, Fazio, Kim and Johns2015). Two recent systematic reviews reported insufficient evidence in the current literature for two other prevention strategies: physical activity (Brasure et al., Reference Brasure, Desai, Davila, Nelson, Calvert, Jutkowitz and Kane2018) and pharmacological interventions (Fink et al., Reference Fink, Jutkowitz, McCarten, Hemmy, Butler, Davila and Kane2018). Additionally, although a number of risk factors have been identified, applying those findings to specific individuals has proven rather complicated (Mukadam, Reference Mukadam2018), necessitating continued work to develop risk identification procedures and prevention strategies with clinical utility.

Harnessing multiple theoretical perspectives and methodological approaches will increase our ability to better characterize risk factors of dementia development and identify effective means of prevention. This issue of International Psychogeriatrics includes several articles that focus on cognitive decline, particularly Alzheimer’s and other dementias, from three different but potentially impactful perspectives: examination of internet use as a prevention strategy (Berner et al., Reference Berner, Comijs, Elmståhl, Welmer, Berglund, Anderberg and Deeg2019); artificial intelligence as a mechanism to identify individual-level risk of Alzheimer’s disease conversion (Grassi et al., Reference Grassi, Loewenstein, Caldirola, Schruers, Duara and Perna2018a); and lastly, based on a mouse model, re-invigoration of IGF-2 as a potential preventative agent (Xia et al., Reference Xia, Zhu, Zhao, Yang, Zuo, Xie, Chen and Han2019). Each of these papers is accompanied by thoughtful commentaries by Moxley and Czaja (Reference Moxley and Czaja2019), Graham and Depp (Reference Graham and Depp2019), and O’Hara and Hallmayer (Reference O’Hara and Hallmayer2019), respectively.

The notion of “prescribing” social media to improve cognitive outcomes may seem counterintuitive, given that studies of internet use in younger populations commonly report on its potential downsides such as greater psychopathology, problematic alcohol use, self-injurious behavior, and impaired sleep (Elhai et al., Reference Elhai, Dvorak, Levine and Hall2017; Hale and Guan, Reference Hale and Guan2015; Ko et al., Reference Ko, Yen, Yen, Chen, Weng and Chen2008, Reference Ko, Yen, Chen, Yeh and Yen2009; Lam et al., Reference Lam, Peng, Mai and Jing2009). For older adults, however, internet engagement could be a beneficial preventative agent slowing cognitive decline in some people. Berner et al.’s (Reference Berner, Comijs, Elmståhl, Welmer, Berglund, Anderberg and Deeg2019) well-powered study of older adults in Sweden and the Netherlands found that self-reported internet use at baseline was associated with less cognitive decline, controlling for baseline cognitive functioning. This paper offers a hopeful implication that internet use could be a potentially helpful preventive strategy for older adults. This possibility is particularly exciting given that internet use is an easy and appealing intervention for many older adults who may struggle to access other prevention options, including those with significant mobility limitations or those who live in areas with low healthcare access. Internet use has exploded among older adults over the past few years, with 53% of adults 65 and older using the internet compared to 22% in 2004 (Chang et al., Reference Chang, McAllister and McCaslin2015). If internet use is demonstrated in future research to slow cognitive decline, we may see population-wide impact without resorting to highly structured dissemination tactics.

Berner and colleagues (Reference Berner, Comijs, Elmståhl, Welmer, Berglund, Anderberg and Deeg2019) hypothesized that internet use would provide a challenging and engaging cognitive context that may facilitate learning and active use of other cognitive facilities. The brain remains plastic in later life, and targeted cognitive engagement can help improve cognitive function and associated outcomes even among older adults with significant mental health issues (e.g., Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Bismark, Joshi, Tarasenko, Treichler, Hochberger and Light2018). In Moxley and Czaja’s (Reference Moxley and Czaja2019) commentary, the authors principally argue for social connectivity as the primary mediator of change, specifically mentioning that social connectivity via the internet may decrease loneliness, increase sense of purpose, and provide more accessible methods of connecting to others. Social isolation and loneliness are common among older adults, and are associated with worse cognitive functioning (Boss et al., Reference Boss, Kang and Branson2015). If social connectivity is the primary mediator for the internet’s effect on cognition in older adults, this may also provide support for continued development of social engagement-based interventions for older adults.

In studies comparing younger and older adults, one must consider the possibility that many observed differences between the two age groups are at least partly a result of cohort effects rather than aging effects. Could the process of learning new technology and discovering the internet’s avenues to connect with others be a critical contributor to slowing cognitive decline? If so, it is possible that future generations in which most people have been using the internet and other technology for much of their adult lives may not see the same benefit. However, given the fast pace of technological advances, new technology will always offer opportunities for cognitive challenges and new social connections. As Moxley and Czaja (Reference Moxley and Czaja2019) suggest, future studies examining differences in cognitive decline based on how older adults use the internet will help delineate how personalized internet use can contribute to better cognition with aging.

Xia and colleagues (Reference Xia, Zhu, Zhao, Yang, Zuo, Xie, Chen and Han2019) revitalized a tried-and-true methodological approach, by studying IGF-2 in mice to better understand the impact of inflammation on cognition, and particularly, the potential to use anti-inflammatory agents to prevent neuronal degeneration. For decades, inflammation has been a major target of research and interventions related to cognitive decline, and, as O’Hara and Hallmayer (Reference O’Hara and Hallmayer2019) emphasize, returning to well-known theories and methodologies is an underrated but useful research agenda. Continued use of well-developed, useful theories and models can significantly contribute to the literature, as illustrated by Xia et al.’s work.

Developing effective prevention strategies is a critical component of slowing cognitive decline among older adults. At the same time, identifying older adults most at risk of decline is necessary for impactful dissemination of preventive interventions. A number of potential predictors of cognitive decline have been identified, but a clinically useful method of identifying at-risk individuals is still in process. Grassi et al.’s work (Reference Grassi, Loewenstein, Caldirola, Schruers, Duara and Perna2018a) advances that progression by describing the validation process of their previously developed machine learning approach (Grassi et al., Reference Grassi, Perna, Caldirola, Schruers, Duara and Loewenstein2018b), which aims to identify individuals with MCI who are more likely to convert to Alzheimer’s disease within the next three years. Their machine learning technique showed a high level of predictive power.

Graham and Depp’s (Reference Graham and Depp2019) thoughtful commentary highlights the strengths of this approach, including use of clinical predictors in the model and their strong prediction results, while also cautioning about the need to replicate these results in other samples. Machine learning’s capability for complexity can at times be a double-edged sword, as predictors that fit one population perfectly may increase error in another population. In this respect, a strength of Berner et al.’s paper (Reference Berner, Comijs, Elmståhl, Welmer, Berglund, Anderberg and Deeg2019) is mirrored in a limitation of Grassi et al.’s (Reference Grassi, Loewenstein, Caldirola, Schruers, Duara and Perna2018a): specifically, that Berner et al. collected data from two countries, increasing the generalizability of their findings, while Grassi et al.’s work was limited to one American city. International data collection is a way forward for public health research, and increased international collaborations via large consortia and open science ‘accelerators’ provides a path for researchers to increase their impact.

Like Graham and Depp (Reference Graham and Depp2019), we also view Grassi et al.’s paper (Reference Grassi, Loewenstein, Caldirola, Schruers, Duara and Perna2018a) with a combination of excitement and caution. Machine learning provides an avenue to increase complexity of models, allowing for a way to integrate many predictors into a single calculation of risk. However, in order for these approaches to be practical for clinical use, these models must incorporate only the data that can be feasibly collected in usual care settings. Grassi et al. (Reference Grassi, Loewenstein, Caldirola, Schruers, Duara and Perna2018a) include some sources of data that are likely to be feasible in many clinics, but accessibility for different data sources will vary by clinic. Using as many easy pieces of data (e.g., demographic data, quick and easy to collect clinical data) will increase ultimate uptake and utility. Graham and Depp (Reference Graham and Depp2019) also expressed the need for these approaches to improve interpretability, given the so-called ‘black box’ nature of machine learning – i.e. it doesn’t tell us how it arrived at a specific conclusion. Ultimately, machine learning approaches like Grassi et al.’s (Reference Grassi, Loewenstein, Caldirola, Schruers, Duara and Perna2018a) are promising and innovative, but their clinical impact is yet to be determined.

Overall, identification of high-risk individuals and prevention strategies should be major goals for research on cognitive decline in older adults. This issue adds to a growing body of literature that informs machine learning-driven identification methods and accessible prevention strategies. Future work will benefit from consideration of diverse methods, particularly by integrating classical methods with forward-thinking innovations. This will allow for the development of a clinically useful toolkit that assists healthcare providers in identifying older adults at high risk of cognitive decline and facilitates implementation of preventive strategies in healthcare and at home. The studies presented here indicate that this type of toolkit is in development, and benefits to older adults will only continue to increase in the near future.

Funding

This work was supported, in part, by the National Institute of Mental Health T32 Geriatric Mental Health Program (grant MH019934 to DVJ [PI]), and an R01 grant (R01MH094151-01 to DVJ [PI]), and the Sam and Rose Stein Institute for Research on Aging, University of California San Diego.

References

Alzheimer’s Association. (2019). Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 15, 321387.Google Scholar
Baumgart, M., Snyder, H. M., Carrillo, M. C., Fazio, S., Kim, H. and Johns, H. (2015). Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: a population-based perspective. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 11, 718726. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.05.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berner, J., Comijs, H., Elmståhl, S., Welmer, A.-K., Berglund, J. S., Anderberg, P. and Deeg, D. (2019). Maintaining cognitive function with internet use: a two-country, six-year longitudinal study. International Psychogeriatrics, 31, 929936.Google Scholar
Boss, L., Kang, D.-H. and Branson, S. (2015). Loneliness and cognitive function in the older adult: a systematic review. International Psychogeriatrics, 27, 541553. doi: 10.1017/S1041610214002749.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brasure, M., Desai, P., Davila, H., Nelson, V. A., Calvert, C., Jutkowitz, E., … Kane, R. L. (2018). Physical activity interventions in preventing cognitive decline and Alzheimer- type dementia: a systematic review. Annals of Internal Medicine, 168, 30. doi: 10.7326/M17-1528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, J., McAllister, C. and McCaslin, R. (2015). Correlates of, and barriers to, internet use among older adults. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 58, 6685. doi: 10.1080/01634372.2014.913754.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elhai, J. D., Dvorak, R. D., Levine, J. C. and Hall, B. J. (2017). Problematic smartphone use: A conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with anxiety and depression psychopathology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 207, 251259. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.08.030.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fink, H. A., Jutkowitz, E., McCarten, J. R., Hemmy, L. S., Butler, M., Davila, H., … Kane, R. L. (2018). Pharmacologic interventions to prevent cognitive decline, mild cognitive impairment, and clinical Alzheimer-type dementia: a systematic review. Annals of Internal Medicine, 168, 39. doi: 10.7326/M17-1529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, S. A. and Depp, C. A. (2019). Artificial intelligence and risk prediction in geriatric mental health: what happens next? International Psychogeriatrics, 31, 921923.Google Scholar
Grassi, M., Loewenstein, D. A., Caldirola, D., Schruers, K., Duara, R. and Perna, G. (2018a). A clinically-translatable machine learning algorithm for the prediction of Alzheimer’s disease conversion: further evidence of its accuracy via a transfer learning approach. International Psychogeriatrics, 31, 937945.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grassi, M., Perna, G., Caldirola, D., Schruers, K., Duara, R. and Loewenstein, D. A. (2018b). A clinically-translatable machine learning algorithm for the prediction of Alzheimer’s disease conversion in individuals with mild and premild cognitive impairment. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 61, 15551573. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hale, L. and Guan, S. (2015). Screen time and sleep among school-aged children and adolescents: A systematic literature review. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 21, 5058. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2014.07.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ko, C. H., Yen, J. Y., Yen, C. F., Chen, C. S., Weng, C. C. and Chen, C. C. (2008). The association between Internet addiction and problematic alcohol use in adolescents: the problem behavior model. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 11, 571576.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ko, C. H., Yen, J. Y., Chen, C. S., Yeh, Y. C. and Yen, C. F. (2009). Predictive values of psychiatric symptoms for Internet addiction in adolescents: a 2-year prospective study. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine , 163, 937943.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lam, L. T., Peng, Z., Mai, J. and Jing, J. (2009). The association between Internet addiction and self-injurious behaviour among adolescents. Injury Prevention, 15, 403408.10.1136/ip.2009.021949CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, A. J. and Shiri-Feshki, M. (2008). Temporal trends in the long term risk of progression of mild cognitive impairment: a pooled analysis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 79, 13861391. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.142679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moxley, J. H. and Czaja, S. J. (2019). Potential mediators of the relationship between Internet usage and cognition. International Psychogeriatrics, 31, 917919.Google Scholar
Mukadam, N. (2018). Improving the diagnosis and prediction of progression in mild cognitive impairment. International Psychogeriatrics, 30, 14191421. doi: 10.1017/S1041610218001692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Hara, R. and Hallmayer, J. (2019). Rediscovering an old friend: a role for insulin growth factor-2 in reducing neurodegeneration, decreasing amyloid beta, and improving memory. International Psychogeriatrics, 31, 925927.Google Scholar
Petersen, R. (2011). Mild Cognitive Impairment. The New England Journal of Medicine, 364, 22272234. doi: 10.1056/NEJMcp0910237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, M. L., Bismark, A. W., Joshi, Y. B., Tarasenko, M., Treichler, E. B. H., Hochberger, W. C., … Light, G. A. (2018). Targeted cognitive training improves auditory and verbal outcomes among treatment refractory schizophrenia patients mandated to residential care. Schizophrenia Research, 202, 378384. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2018.07.025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xia, L., Zhu, X., Zhao, Y., Yang, G., Zuo, X., Xie, P., Chen, C. and Han, Q. (2019). Genome-wide RNA sequencing analysis reveals that IGF-2 attenuates memory decline, oxidative stress and amyloid plaques in an Alzheimer’s disease mouse model (AD) by activating the PI3K/AKT/CREB signaling pathway. International Psychogeriatrics, 31, 947959.CrossRefGoogle Scholar