Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-5lx2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-29T23:14:09.541Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

‘Do what I mean, not what I say!’. Changes in mothers' action-directives to young children*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Maita H. Schneiderman
Affiliation:
University of Pennsylvania

Abstract

This study investigated mothers' notions of appropriate speech to 40 children aged 1; 6 to 3; 6. Spontaneous maternal action-directives were analysed in terms of the explicitness with which they expressed syntactic and lexical clues to action-directive intentions. Cross-sectional and longitudinal changes, and progressions within maternal repetition sequences, indicated that the children perceived as most limited in inferential skills elicited the most restricted input. Most of the action-directives they heard could be interpreted non-inferentially, because syntactic and lexical clues to intentions were explicitly expressed. External supports were removed gradually in response to the mother's perception of her child's increasing ability to draw extra-linguistic inferences. Several theoretical models of non-literal speech-act comprehension are supported. It is argued that the differences in language use reflect the mother's notions of the speech a listener is likely to understand.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1983

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This paper is based on a doctoral dissertation submitted to the University of Pennsylvania, 1980. It was prepared during an appointment at Syracuse University; portions were presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, Mass., 1981. I thank Lila R. Gleitman (thesis advisor), Henry Gleitman and Joseph A. Glick for advice and encouragement throughout the project; Arthur S. Reber and Marilyn Shatz for many helpful suggestions; Lenore Sherman-Schneiderman for running subjects and analysing data; Adele A. Abrahamson, Barbara Landau, Jacqueline Sachs and Norman Stein for comments on earlier drafts of this paper. Address for correspondence: Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06268.

References

REFERENCES

Bates, E. (1976). Language and context: the acquisition of pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bellinger, D. (1979). Changes in the explicitness of mothers' directives as children age. JChLang 6. 443–58.Google ScholarPubMed
Clark, H. H. (1979). Responding to indirect speech acts. CogPsych 11. 430–47.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. & Lucy, P. (1975). Understanding what is meant from what is said: a study in conversationally conveyed requests. JVLVB 14. 5672.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1977). Wait for me, rollerskate! In Mitchell-Kernan, C. & Ervin-Tripp, S. (eds), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Garvey, C. (1975). Requests and responses in children's speech. JChLang 2. 4163.Google Scholar
James, S. L. (1978). Effect of listener age and situation on the politeness of children's directives. JPsycholingRes 7. 307–17.Google Scholar
Lakoff, R. (1973). The logic of politeness: or mind your P's and Q's. Papers from the Ninth Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Newcombe, N. & Zaslow, M. (1980). Do 2½-year-olds hint? A study of directive forms in the speech of 2½-year-old children to adults. Paper presented at the Southeastern Conference on Human Development,Alexandria, Va.Google Scholar
Newport, E. L. & Gleitman, H. (1977). Maternal self-repetition and the child's acquisition of language. PRCLD 13. 111.Google Scholar
Newport, E. L., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L. R. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Read, B. K. & Cherry, L. J. (1978). Pre-school children's production of directive forms. Discourse Processes 1. 233–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sachs, J. (1980). The role of adult–child play in language development. In Rubin, K. (ed.), Children's play. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Sachs, J. & Truswell, L. (1978). Comprehension of two-word instructions by children in the one-word stage. JChLang 5. 1724.Google Scholar
Schaffer, H. R. & Crook, C. K. (1980). Child compliance and maternal control techniques. DevPsych 16. 5461.Google Scholar
Schneiderman, M. H. (1980). ‘Do what I mean, not what I say!’. Mothers' action-directives to their young children. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Schneiderman, M. H. (in prep.) Children's responses to maternal action-directives.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1975). Indirect speech acts. In Cole, P. & Morgan, J. L. (eds), Syntax and semantics. Vol. 3 Speech acts. New York: Seminar Press.Google Scholar
Shatz, M. (1978 a). Children's comprehension of their mother's question-directives. JChLang 5. 3946.Google Scholar
Shatz, M. (1978 b). On the development of communicative understanding: an early strategy for interpreting and responding to messages. CogPsych 10. 271301.Google Scholar
Shatz, M. (1979). How to do things by asking: form–function pairings in mothers' questions and their relation to children's responses. ChDev 50. 1093–9.Google Scholar
Shipley, E. F., Smith, C. S. & Gleitman, L. R. (1969). A study in the acquisition of language: free responses to commands. Lg 45. 322–42.Google Scholar
Valian, V. & Caplan, J. S. (1979). What children say when asked ‘What?’: a study of the use of syntactic knowledge. JExpChPsych 28. 424–44.Google ScholarPubMed
Valian, V. & Wales, R. J. (1976). What's what: talkers help listeners hear and understand by clarifying sentential relations. Cognition 4. 155–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar