Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-lvtdw Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-14T09:19:58.806Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Interpreting and responding to spoken language: children's recognition and use of a speaker's goal*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Leonard Abbeduto*
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin-Madison
Jill Bibler Nuccio
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin-Madison
Radhi Al-Mabuk
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin-Madison
Pamela Rotto
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin-Madison
Fay Maas
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin-Madison
*
Department of Educational Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 1025 W., Johnson, Madison, WI 53706, USA.

Abstract

This study had two purposes. The first was to examine age differences in the extent to which children infer and use a speaker's interpersonal goal to understand speech acts. To this end, the subjects responded to Do you have…? requests in a role-playing task. The goal behind such a request is implicit in the speaker's choice of noun phrase. We sought to determine whether children, like adults, use the noun phrase to infer the speaker's goal and thereby decide whether the request is a yes-no question or a directive. The second purpose was to examine age differences in the extent to which children select responses that carry implications appropriate to the speaker's interpersonal goal. To do this, Do you have… ? requests containing general category labels were addressed to the children when they possessed all the category exemplars and when they possessed only a few exemplars. A simple yes implies that the listener has nearly all exemplars and, therefore, is inappropriate for the speaker's goal in the latter situation. The subjects were six-, seven-, nine-, and eleven-year-olds and adults, with 12 subjects per age. Only the eleven-year-olds and the adults used the speaker's noun phrase and considered the implications of yes.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1992

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was supported by NICHD grant HD24356 to L. Abbeduto and by NICHD grant HD03352 to the Waisman Center on Mental Retardation and Human Development. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development in Kansas City in 1989. We are grateful to the Madison Metropolitan School District, especially Dr Lee Gruenwald and Jack Jorgensen. Thanks also to Terry McMenamin who helped prepare the experimental materials and to Katherine Short and Glenis Benson who assisted with the data collection. Finally, we are indebted to the students, staff and parents of the following schools: Horicon Elementary, Marquette Elementary, Schenk Elementary, Schenk Middle, St Dennis Elementary, St Maria Goretti Elementary and the Waisman Center Kindergarten.

References

REFERENCES

Abbeduto, L., Davies, B. & Furman, L. (1988). The development of speech act comprehension in mentally retarded individuals and nonretarded children. Child Development 59, 6, 1460–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, J. & Perrault, C. (1980). Analyzing intention in utterances. Artificial Intelligence 15, 143–78.Google Scholar
Axia, G. & Baroni, M. R. (1985). Linguistic politeness at different age levels. Child Development 56, 918–27.Google Scholar
Baroni, M. R. & Axia, G. (1989). Children's meta-pragmatic abilities and the identification of polite and impolite requests. First Language 9, 285–97.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1982). Reorganization processes in lexical and syntactic development. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (eds), Language acquisition: the state of the art. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Carrell, P. L. (1981). Children's understanding of indirect requests: comparing child and adult comprehension. Journal of Child Language 8, 329–45.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1979). Responding to indirect speech acts. Cognitive Psychology 11, 430–77.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. & Schunk, D. H. (1980). Polite responses to polite requests. Cognition 8, 111–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, P. & Levesque, H. J. (1990). Rational interaction as the basis for communication. In Cohen, P. R., Morgan, J. & Pollack, M. E. (eds), Intentions in communication. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, P. R., Morgan, J. & Pollack, M. E. (1990). Introduction. In Cohen, P. R., Morgan, J. & Pollack, M. E. (eds), Intentions in communication. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. & Gordon, D. (1986). The development of requests. In Schiefelbusch, R. L. (ed.), Communicative competence: assessment and intervention. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Eson, M. E. & Shapiro, A. S. (1982). When ‘don't’ means ‘do’: pragmatic and cognitive development in understanding an indirect imperative. First Language 3, 8391.Google Scholar
Garvey, C. (1975). Requests and responses in children's speech. Journal of Child Language 2, 4160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In Cole, P. & Morgan, J. L. (eds), Syntax and semantics: Vol. 3. Speech acts. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1986). Some fundamental aspects of language development after age 5. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (eds), Language acquisition. (2nd edn) Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Kautz, H. (1990). A circumscriptive theory of plan recognition. In Cohen, P. R., Morgan, J. & Pollack, M. E. (eds), Intentions in communication. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Ledbetter, P. J. & Dent, C. H. (1988). Young children's sensitivity to direct and indirect request structure. First Language 8, 227–46.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. B., Wilcox, M. J., Fulmer, K. C. & Davis, G. A. (1978). An investigation of children's comprehension of pragmatic meanings. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 21, 528–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levinson, S. C. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Moore, C. F., Dixon, J. A. & Haines, B. A. (1991). Components of understanding in proportional reasoning: a fuzzy set representation of developmental progressions. Child Development 62, 441–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reeder, K. (1980). The emergence of illocutionary skills. Journal of Child Language 7, 1328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reeder, K. & Wakefield, J. (1987). The development of young children's speech act comprehension: how much language is necessary? Applied Psycholinguistics 8, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schank, R. & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Shatz, M. (1978). On the development of communicative understandings: an early strategy for interpreting and responding to messages. Cognitive Psychology 10, 271301.Google Scholar
Shatz, M. & McCloskey, L. (1984). Answering appropriately: a developmental perspective on conversational knowledge. In Kuczaj, S. II (ed.), Discourse development: progress in cognitive development research. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar