Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-sjtt6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-06T06:23:21.108Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Language differentiation by the bilingual infant*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Marilyn May Vihman
Affiliation:
Stanford University

Abstract

This paper traces the process involved in the bilingual infant's gradual differentiation of his two languages, beginning with the acquisition of a dual lexicon. Word combination is at first based indiscriminately on this dual language source; function words account for a disproportionately large number of tokens used in mixed-language utterances. Universal principles of child syntax are at first applied; later, rules specific to each of the languages are developed separately. The development of self-awareness and sensitivity to standards in the second year provides the essential cognitive underpinning for the child to begin to avoid mixed-language utterances and to choose his language according to his interlocutor. At a still later point the bilingual older child may begin to make use of code-switching strategies appropriate to his or her bilingual community.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1985

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bloom, L. (1970). Language development: form and function in emerging grammars. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time: the use of single word utterances before syntax. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Branigan, G. (1979). Some reasons why successive single word utterances are not. JChLang 6. 411–21.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1978 a). Strategies for communicating. ChDev 49. 953–9.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1978 b). Awareness of language: some evidence from what children say and do. In Sinclair, E., Jarvella, R. & Levelt, R. (eds), Children's conception of language. Berlin: Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
Fantini, A. E. (1978). Bilingual behavior and social cues: case studies of two bilingual children. In Paradis, M. (ed.), Aspects of bilingualism. Columbia: Hornbeam Press.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. (1985). Auf deutsch duck: language separation in early bilingualism. Osmania papers in linguistics 8. (Nirmala Memorial Volume.)Google Scholar
Franklin, M. B. (1979). Metalinguistic functioning in development. In Smith, N. R. & Franklin, M. B. (eds), Symbolic functioning in childhood. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Garman, M. (1979). Early grammatical development. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (eds), Language acquisition: studies in first language development. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. (1982). Life with two languages: an introduction to bilingualism. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Imedadze, N. V. (1960). K psikhologicheskoy prirode rannego dvuyazychiya. Voprosy Psikkologii 6. 60–9.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (1976). Phonological disability in children. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Kagan, I. (1981). The second year. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klima, E. S. & Bellugi, U. (1966). Syntactic regularities in the speech of children. In Lyons, J. & Wales, R. J. (eds), Psycholinguistics papers: the proceedings of the 1966 Edinburgh conference. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Leopold, V. F. (1939). Speech development of a bilingual child: a linguist's record. Vol. 1, Vocabulary growth in the first two years. Evanston, Ill.: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Lindholm, K. J. & Padilla, A. M. (1978). Language mixing in bilingual children. JChLang 5. 327–35.Google Scholar
McCune-Nicolich, L. (1980). The cognitive bases of relational words in the single word period. JChLang 8. 1534.Google Scholar
Murrell, M. (1966). Language acquisition in a trilingual environment: notes from a case study. Studia Linguistica 20. 935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. MonogrSocResChDevel 38. Serial No. 149.Google Scholar
Pfaff, C. W. (1979). Constraints on language mixing. Lg 55. 291318.Google Scholar
Poplack, S. (1980). Sometimes I'll start a sentence in Spanish y termina en español: toward a typology of code-switching. Linguistics 18. 581618.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poplack, S. (1981). Syntactic structure and social function of code-switching. In Duran, R. P. (ed.), Latino language and communicative behavior. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex.Google Scholar
Redlinger, W. E. & Park, T. (1980). Language mixing in young bilinguals. JChLang 7. 337–52.Google ScholarPubMed
Ronjat, J. (1913). Le développement du langage observé chez un enfant bilingue. Paris: Champion.Google Scholar
Saunders, G. (1980). Adding a second language in the home. JMultilingMulticultDevel 1. 113–44.Google Scholar
Swain, M. (1972). Bilinguaiism as a first language. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Irvine.Google Scholar
Tabouret-Keller, A. (1963). L'acquisition du langage parlé chez un petit enfant en milieu bilingue. Problèmes de Psycholinguistique 8. 205–19.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (1981 a). Language mixing and code-switching: conversations between a pair of bilingual siblings. Presented at the Second International Congress for the Study of Child Language,Vancouver,British Columbia.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (1981 b). Phonology and the development of the lexicon: evidence from children's errors. JChLang 8. 239–64.Google ScholarPubMed
Vihman, M. M. (1982). The acquisition of morphology by a bilingual child: a whole-word approach. AppPsycholing 3. 141–60.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. & Mclaughlin, B. (1982). Bilingualism and second language acquisition in preschool children. In Brainerd, C. J. & Pressley, M. (eds), Progress in cognitive development: verbal processes in children. New York: Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
Volterra, V. & Taeschner, T. (1978). The acquisition and development of language by bilingual children. JChLang 5. 311–26.Google Scholar
Wode, H. (1977). Four early stages in the development of L1 negation. JChLang 4. 87102.Google Scholar