Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-m9kch Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-06T10:59:15.726Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The episodic structure of maternal speech to young children*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

David J. Messer
Affiliation:
University of Strathclyde

Abstract

An investigation was made of the episodic structure of maternal speech to young children during a free-play session. The findings indicate that maternal speech was organized so as to provide a high degree of redundancy with regard to the referential component of speech. The beginning of episodes, in which the same object was repeatedly referred to, was clearly marked; furthermore speech inside episodes with a common referent was highly repetitive. All this suggests that maternal speech is organized in a way that increases the predictability in the child's language environment.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1980

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was supported by a S.S.R.C. grant to Professor H. R. Schaffer. I am indebted to Professor Schaffer and G. M. Collis for helpful advice and criticism, and to N. Sharp for technical assistance. The author's current address is Child and Family Research Branch, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, Bethesda, Maryland 20014, U.S.A.

References

REFERENCES

Attneave, F. (1959). Application of information theory to psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Benedict, H. (1976). Language comprehension in 10 to 16 month old infants. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Yale University.Google Scholar
Bridges, A. (1979). Directing two-year-olds' attention; some clues to understanding. JChLang 6. 211–26.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1977). Early social interaction and language acquisition. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Collis, G. M. (1977). Visual co-orientation and maternal speech. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1971). An overview of theories of grammatical development. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The ontogenesis of grammar. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1973). Some strategies for the first two years. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fleiss, J. L. (1973). Statistical methods for rates and proportions. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Fogel, A. (1977). Temporal organization in mother-infant face-to-face interaction. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Friedlander, B., Jacobs, A., Davis, B. & Wetstone, H. (1972). Time-sampling analysis of infants' natural language environments in the home. ChDev 43. 730–40.Google ScholarPubMed
Gleason, J. B. (1977). Talking to children: some notes on feedback. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Talking to children. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. (1977). Towards the origin of dialogue. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kobashigawa, B. (1968). Repetition in mother's speech to her child. Manuscript, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Landes, J. E. (1975). Speech addressed to children: issues and characteristics of parental input. Language and Learning 25. 355–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lempers, J. D., Flavell, E. R. & Flavell, J. H. (1977). The development in very young children of tacit knowledge concerning visual perception. GeneticPsychMonogr 95. 353.Google ScholarPubMed
MacRae, A. (1971). On calculating unbiased information measures. PsychBull 75. 270–7.Google Scholar
Messer, D. J. (1978). The integration of mothers' referential speech with joint play. ChDev 49. 781–7Google Scholar
Murphy, C. M. & Messer, D. J. (1977). Mothers, infants and pointing: a study of a gesture. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ryan, M. L. (1978). Contour in context. In Campbell, R. N. & Smith, P. T. (eds), Recent advances in the psychology of language. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Scaife, M. & Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, 253. 265–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaffer, H. R., Collis, G. M. & Parsons, G. (1977). Vocal interchange and visual regard in verbal and pre-verbal children. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sherrod, K. B., Friedman, S., Crawley, S., Drake, D. & Devieux, J. (1977). Maternal language to prelinguistic infants: syntactic aspects. ChDev 48. 1662–5.Google ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. (1972). Mothers' speech to children learning language. ChDev 43. 549–65.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1977). Mothers' speech research: from input to interaction. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Talking to children. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E., Arlman-Rupp, A., Jobse, J., Joosten, J. & Vorster, J. (1976). Mothers' speech in three social classes. JPsycholingRes 5. 120.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N., Beebe, B., Jaffe, J. & Bennett, S. L. (1977). The infant's stimulus world during social interaction: a study of caregiver behaviours with particular reference to repetition and timing. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Vorster, J. (1975). Mommy linguist: the case for motherese. Lingua 37. 281312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warden, D. A. (1976). The influence of context on children's use of identifying expressions and references. BJPsych 67. 101112.Google Scholar