Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-xfwgj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-26T09:39:21.887Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Productivity and memory for newly formed words*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Eve V. Clark
Affiliation:
Stanford University
Sophia R. Cohen
Affiliation:
Stanford University

Abstract

The first word-formation devices children should learn, we proposed, are the most productive ones, that is, those that have the fewest structural constraints on their use and that appear most frequently in the formation of new words. Four- and five-year-old children who were asked to recall novel words with English agentive suffixes recalled the most productive suffix (-er) well and its less productive companions (-ist and -ian) very poorly. Other children who were asked to recall as agentive suffixes the non-agentive -ly (about equal in frequency to -er) together with -ist and -ian, did poorly on all three. They also substituted agentive suffixes (including -er) for the non-agentive -ly, with greater reliance on more productive forms. These data, together with previous observations, support the hypothesis that productivity affects the order in which children master word-formation devices.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1984

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adams, V. (1973). An introduction to modern English word formation. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Anglin, J. M. (1977). Word, object and conceptual development. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Archer, E. J. (1960). A re-evaluation of the meaningfulness of all possible CVC trigrams. PsychMonogr 74.Google Scholar
Arnoff, M. (1976). Word formation in generative grammar. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Baudouin de Courtenay, J. (1974). Spostrzezenia nadjezykiem dziecka [Observations on child language], ed. by Chmura-Klekotowa, M.. Wroclaw: Polska Akademia Nauk, Komitet Językoznawstwa.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1977). Introduction. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. L. & Slobin, D. I. (1982). Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past tense. Lg 58. 265–89.Google Scholar
Chmura-Klekotowa, M. (1970). Odbicie tendencji slowotworczych języka polskiego w neologizmach dzieci [Reflection of derivational trends in the Polish language in children's neologisms]. Prace Filologiczne (Warsaw) 20. 153–9.Google Scholar
Chmura-Klekotowa, M. (1971). Neologizmy slowotwórcze w mowie dzieci [Neologisms in children's speech]. Prace Filologiczne (Warsaw) 21. 99235.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1980). Convention and innovation in acquiring the lexicon. Keynote Address, Twelfth Annual Child Language Research Forum, Stanford University. PRCLD 19. 120.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1981). Lexical innovations: how children learn to create new words. In Deutsch, W.(ed.), The child's construction of language. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1982). The young word-maker: a case study of innovation in the child's lexicon. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (eds), Language acquisition: the state of the art. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1983). Meanings and concepts. In Mussen, P. H. (ed.), Handbook of child psychology (4th edn.) Vol. 3: Cognitive development (eds Flavell, J. H. & Markman, E. M.). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. & Berman, R. A. (1984). Structure and use in the acquisition of word-formation. Lg 60.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. & Hecht, B. F. (1982). Learning to coin agent and instrument nouns. Cognition 12. 124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubois, J. (1962). Etude sur la dérivation suffixale en français moderne et contemporain. Paris: Larousse.Google Scholar
Egger, M. E. (1887). Observations et réflexions sur le développement de l'intelligence et du langage chez les enfants, Paris: Picard.Google Scholar
Grégoire, A. (1947). L'apprentissage du langage, Vol. 2. Paris & Liège: Librairie Droz.Google Scholar
Hecht, B. F. (1983). Situations and language: children's use of plural allomorphs in familiar and unfamiliar settings. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Ljung, M. (1974). A frequency dictionary of English morphemes. Stockholm: AWE/Gebers.Google Scholar
Marchand, H. (1969). The categories and types of present-day English word-formation (2nd edn). München: C. H. Beck'sche Verlag.Google Scholar
Newport, E. L., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L. R. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Prudden, S. & Sussman, J. (1979). Spot reducing program. New York: Workman Publishing.Google Scholar
Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G. & Svartvik, J. (1972). A grammar of contemporary English. New York: Seminar Press.Google Scholar
Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W., Johnson, D. & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. CogPsychol 8. 382439.Google Scholar
Sterling, C. M. (1983). The psychological productivity of inflectional and derivational morphemes. In Rogers, D. & Sloboda, J. A. (eds), The acquisition of symbolic skills. London: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Stern, C. & Stern, W. (1928). Die Kindersprache: eine psychologische und sprachtheoretische Untersuchung (4th edn). Leipzig: Barth.Google Scholar
Vinson, J. (19151916). Observations sur le développement du langage chez l'enfant. Revue de Linguistique 48. 139.Google Scholar