Hostname: page-component-788cddb947-wgjn4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-19T04:03:34.273Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Maternal prenatal and postnatal psychological distress trajectories and impact on cognitive development in 4-year-old children: the Japan Environment and Children’s Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2024

Hidekazu Nishigori*
Affiliation:
Department of Development and Environmental Medicine, Fukushima Medical Center for Children and Women, Fukushima Medical University Graduate School of Medicine, Fukushima, Japan Fukushima Regional Center for the Japan Environmental and Children’s Study, Fukushima, Japan
Toshie Nishigori
Affiliation:
Fukushima Regional Center for the Japan Environmental and Children’s Study, Fukushima, Japan Department of Pediatrics, Fukushima Medical University School of Medicine, Fukushima, Japan
Taeko Suzuki
Affiliation:
Department of Development and Environmental Medicine, Fukushima Medical Center for Children and Women, Fukushima Medical University Graduate School of Medicine, Fukushima, Japan Fukushima Regional Center for the Japan Environmental and Children’s Study, Fukushima, Japan Department of Midwifery and Maternal Nursing, Fukushima Medical University School of Nursing, Fukushima, Japan
Miyuki Mori
Affiliation:
Department of Development and Environmental Medicine, Fukushima Medical Center for Children and Women, Fukushima Medical University Graduate School of Medicine, Fukushima, Japan Fukushima Regional Center for the Japan Environmental and Children’s Study, Fukushima, Japan Department of Midwifery and Maternal Nursing, Fukushima Medical University School of Nursing, Fukushima, Japan
Mika Yamada
Affiliation:
Fukushima Regional Center for the Japan Environmental and Children’s Study, Fukushima, Japan Department of Pediatrics, Fukushima Medical University School of Medicine, Fukushima, Japan
Hirotaka Isogami
Affiliation:
Fukushima Regional Center for the Japan Environmental and Children’s Study, Fukushima, Japan Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Fukushima Medical University School of Medicine, Fukushima, Japan
Tsuyoshi Murata
Affiliation:
Fukushima Regional Center for the Japan Environmental and Children’s Study, Fukushima, Japan Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Fukushima Medical University School of Medicine, Fukushima, Japan
Hyo Kyozuka
Affiliation:
Fukushima Regional Center for the Japan Environmental and Children’s Study, Fukushima, Japan Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Fukushima Medical University School of Medicine, Fukushima, Japan
Yuka Ogata
Affiliation:
Fukushima Regional Center for the Japan Environmental and Children’s Study, Fukushima, Japan
Akiko Sato
Affiliation:
Fukushima Regional Center for the Japan Environmental and Children’s Study, Fukushima, Japan
Hirohito Metoki
Affiliation:
Division of Public Health, Hygiene and Epidemiology, Faculty of Medicine, Tohoku Medical and Pharmaceutical University, Sendai, Japan
Kosei Shinoki
Affiliation:
Fukushima Regional Center for the Japan Environmental and Children’s Study, Fukushima, Japan
Seiji Yasumura
Affiliation:
Fukushima Regional Center for the Japan Environmental and Children’s Study, Fukushima, Japan Department of Public Health, Fukushima Medical University School of Medicine, Fukushima, Japan
Mitsuaki Hosoya
Affiliation:
Fukushima Regional Center for the Japan Environmental and Children’s Study, Fukushima, Japan Department of Pediatrics, Fukushima Medical University School of Medicine, Fukushima, Japan
Koichi Hashimoto
Affiliation:
Fukushima Regional Center for the Japan Environmental and Children’s Study, Fukushima, Japan Department of Pediatrics, Fukushima Medical University School of Medicine, Fukushima, Japan
Keiya Fujimori
Affiliation:
Fukushima Regional Center for the Japan Environmental and Children’s Study, Fukushima, Japan Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Fukushima Medical University School of Medicine, Fukushima, Japan
*
Corresponding author: H. Nishigori; Email: nishigo@fmu.ac.jp
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Maternal prenatal and postnatal psychological distress, including depression and anxiety, may affect children’s cognitive development. However, the findings have been inconsistent. We aimed to use the dataset from the Japan Environment and Children’s Study, a nationwide prospective birth cohort study, to examine this association. We evaluated the relationship between the maternal six-item version of the Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K6) scores and cognitive development among children aged 4 years. K6 was administered twice during pregnancy (M-T1; first half of pregnancy, M-T2; second half of pregnancy) and 1 year postpartum (C-1y). Cognitive development was assessed by trained testers, using the Kyoto Scale of Psychological Development 2001. Multiple regression analysis was performed with the group with a K6 score ≤ 4 for both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y as a reference. Records from 1,630 boys and 1,657 girls were analyzed. In the group with K6 scores ≥ 5 in both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1Y groups, boys had significantly lower developmental quotients (DQ) in the language-social developmental (L-S) area (partial regression coefficient: −4.09, 95% confidence interval: −6.88 – −1.31), while girls did not differ significantly in DQ for the L-S area. Among boys and girls, those with K6 scores ≤ 4 at any one or two periods during M-T1, M-T2, or C-1y did not have significantly lower DQ for the L-S area. Persistent maternal psychological distress from the first half of pregnancy to 1 year postpartum had a disadvantageous association with verbal cognitive development in boys, but not in girls aged 4 years.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press in association with The International Society for Developmental Origins of Health and Disease (DOHaD)

Introduction

Maternal prenatal and postnatal psychological distress, including depressive symptoms and anxiety, is known to affect the children’s neurodevelopment, including cognitive development, motor development, mental health, and temperament, influencing the developing fetus up to infancy (first 1000 days from conception). This process is commonly known as the “Developmental Origins of Health and Disease hypothesis.” Reference Gentile1Reference Oyetunji and Chandra4

However, inconsistencies exist in the clinical periods from pregnancy to postpartum that are vulnerable to the effects of maternal psychological distress on children’s neurodevelopment. Reference van den Bergh, van den Heuvel and Lahti2,Reference Aoyagi and Tsuchiya3,Reference Rees, Channon and Waters5 When focusing solely on the pregnancy period, reports indicated higher susceptibility in the early term, while others suggest increased susceptibility in the mid or late term of pregnancy. In a comparison between pregnancy and postpartum periods, some studies report higher susceptibility in the postpartum period than in the pregnancy period, while others find greater susceptibility during the pregnancy period than in the postpartum period. Reference van den Bergh, van den Heuvel and Lahti2,Reference Aoyagi and Tsuchiya3,Reference Rees, Channon and Waters5

Furthermore, inconsistencies exist in the sex differences regarding vulnerability to maternal psychological distress effects on children’s neurodevelopment. Generally, reports suggest boys are more susceptible than girls. Conversely, some reports show girls as more susceptible, particularly in aspects of temperament. Reference Gentile1,Reference van den Bergh, van den Heuvel and Lahti2,Reference Rees, Channon and Waters5

In Japan, the Japan Environment and Children Study (JECS), a nationwide birth cohort study of approximately 100,000 pairs of parents and children, was launched in 2011. Reference Kawamoto, Nitta and Murata6,Reference Michikawa, Nitta and Nakayama7 JECS is ongoing and will continue until the participating children turn 40 years old. As a Sub-Cohort Study of the JECS, trained testers have conducted evaluations of the cognitive development of approximately 5,000 children randomly selected from the sample. In the present study, we used this dataset to evaluate the association between maternal prenatal/postnatal psychological distress trajectories and cognitive development in 4-year-old children, considering sex differences.

Methods

Design and participants

The JECS protocol was reviewed and approved by the Ministry of the Environment’s Institutional Review Board on Epidemiological Studies (no. 100910001) and have been approved by the Ethics Committees of all participating institutions. JECS recruitment occurred between January 2011 and March 2014 and included pregnant women nationwide. Participants were recruited from 15 Regional Centers. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. In the JECS Main Study, the eligibility criteria for participants (expecting mothers) are as follows: (1) they must reside in the study areas at the time of recruitment and are expected to continue residing in Japan for the foreseeable future; (2) they must be capable of participating in the study without difficulty, i.e., they must be able to comprehend the Japanese language and complete the self-administered questionnaire. Reference Kawamoto, Nitta and Murata6

From the JECS Main Study, a Sub-Cohort Study, which included 5% of the participating children who were randomly selected for each Regional Centre at regular intervals, was extracted. Reference Sekiyama, Yamazaki and Michikawa8 The eligibility criteria were as follows: (1) children born after April 1, 2013; (2) all questionnaire and medical record data from children and their mothers collected from the first half of pregnancy to 6 months of age; and (3) biospecimens (except umbilical cord blood) from children and their mothers collected at the first to the second half of pregnancy and delivery. Reference Sekiyama, Yamazaki and Michikawa8 For this Sub-Cohort Study, extended outcome measurements were planned, including face-to-face interviews with specialized staff to evaluate neurological development based on the Kyoto Scale of Psychological Development 2001 (KSPD) for 4-year-old children. Reference Sekiyama, Yamazaki and Michikawa8

The present study used the jecs-qa-20210401 dataset, which was released in April 2021 and revised in April 2023. It contains the cognitive developmental results of 4-year-old children based on the KSPD. As this study included children from singleton pregnancies, multiple-birth children were excluded. Children with congenital anomalies were also excluded from the analysis. Reference Mezawa, Tomotaki and Yamamoto-Hanada9 Congenital anomalies were assessed by physicians who diagnosed them immediately after delivery and during the first month at a regular checkup. Participants who had congenital anomalies reported either at delivery or at 1-month data collection were excluded from the analysis. Reference Mezawa, Tomotaki and Yamamoto-Hanada9

Maternal psychological distress

The JECS administered the six-item version of the Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K6) during the first (M-T1) and second (M-T2) half of pregnancy and at one year postpartum (C-1y). K6 is widely used to assess psychological distress during perinatal and postnatal periods. Reference Kessler, Andrews and Colpe10,Reference Kessler, Barker and Colpe11 It is a self-administered questionnaire comprising six questions that evaluate depressive mood and anxiety according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV), over the preceding 4 weeks on a scale of 0 to 4. The total score is the sum of the scores of the six items, with a possible range of 0 to 24. We used a Japanese version of K6 with a cutoff score of ≥ 5 to identify cases of psychological distress, as used in previous studies of populations and affected communities in Japan. Reference Furukawa, Kawakami and Saitoh12Reference Mori, Nishigori and Ogata15

We analyzed the data to determine the association between K6 scores of ≥ 5 and cognitive development in 4-year-old children.

We classified participants into eight trajectory groups based on K6 scores ≥ 5 and K6 scores ≤ 4 at M-T1, M-T2, and C-1y (Table 1).

Table 1. Characteristics of participants (total = 3287)

Abbreviations: Kyoto Scale of Psychological Development 2001(KSPD), developmental quotient (DQ), cognitive-adaptive (C-A), language-social (L-S), standard deviation (SD), interquartile range (IQR), the 6-item Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K6; total point scores ranged from 0 to 24),1 year postpartum (C-1y).

Cognitive development in 4-year-old children

The KSPD, a standardized developmental assessment tool for Japanese children, covers the cognitive-adaptive (C-A) and language-social (L-S) areas of development. Reference Koyama, Osada, Tsujii and Kurita16Reference Nishigori, Nishigori and Obara18 The C-A and L-S areas correspond to nonverbal and verbal cognitive development, respectively. Scores were combined to form the developmental quotient (DQ), which was calculated, in days, by dividing the developmental age in days by the chronological age and multiplying the quotient by 100. Administrative procedures and evaluations were strictly standardized to ensure tester reliability in this survey. To ensure the reliability of the administration, the testers received rigorous training before they were certified to conduct the testing. Specifically, the testers' training included general lectures by instructors, technique confirmation through watching actual examination videos, learning techniques by observing and conducting examinations, confirming correct evaluations via examination videos, and undergoing evaluation by performing examinations on mock children. The JECS and Kyoto International Social Welfare Exchange Centre certified the testers. As sex-specific differences in children’s cognitive development have been suggested, we examined this issue separately for boys and girls. Reference van den Bergh, van den Heuvel and Lahti2

Statistical analysis and covariables

We compared the characteristics of mothers and their children with data on cognitive development to those obtained with an analysis of variance (ANOVA). Bivariate and multiple regression analyses were then used to assess the association between maternal psychological distress and children’s cognitive development.

First, multiple regression analyses were adjusted for maternal age at delivery, whether the pregnancy was unplanned, use of infertility treatment, marital status, highest level of education (maternal and paternal), smoking during pregnancy (maternal and paternal), alcohol consumption during pregnancy, annual household income, whether the mother had any neuropsychiatric disorders, psychoactive drug use during pregnancy, pregnancy complications, obstetric labor complications, mode of delivery, children’s birth weight, gestational week of delivery, feeding method at 6 months postpartum, family structure, number of children (including the subject), children’s age at the time of beginning attendance at a daycare center, location of the Regional Center, and children’s sex. Information regarding maternal neuropsychiatric disorders, pregnancy complications, obstetric labor complications, mode of delivery, children’s birth weight, and gestational week of delivery was transcribed from physicians’ records. All other information was obtained from participants’ responses to the questionnaire, which was not validated. These covariate factors were mostly chosen with reference to previous relevant studies. Reference Gentile1Reference Oyetunji and Chandra4,Reference Mori, Nishigori and Ogata15 No multicollinearity was found in this analysis (VIF < 2), except for parity and number of children.

Second, multiple regression analyses were adjusted for variables selected through a stepwise method, with the significance level for entry into the model set at 0.20 and for staying in the model at 0.15.

All analyses were performed using the SAS statistical software (version 9.4; SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA).

Results

Of the 104,059 records in this dataset, records from 3,287 children were analyzed (Fig. 1). Table 1 shows the characteristics of the participants as evaluated by the KSPD. In total, there were 1,630 boys and 1,657 girls.

Figure 1. Flow chart depicting research participants’ selection.

In the group of boys, at M-T1, the maternal prenatal K6 score was estimated at 14.6 (interquartile range (IQR) 12.0–18.1) weeks of gestation; at M-T2, it was estimated at 27.4 (IQR 25.3–30.3) weeks of gestation. In the group of girls, at M-T1, the maternal prenatal K6 score was estimated at 14.4 (IQR 11.9–17.7) weeks of gestation; at M-T2, it was estimated at 27.0 (IQR 25.1–29.9) weeks of gestation.

Boys

The participants were divided into eight trajectory groups (Table 1). Table 2 depicts the one-way ANOVA results of maternal K6 and KSPD. Table 3 depicts the bivariate analysis of maternal K6 and KSPD.

Table 2. ANOVA of Maternal K6 and KSPD in eight trajectories groups.

Abbreviations: Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K6), Kyoto Scale of Psychological Development 2001(KSPD), developmental quotient (DQ), cognitive-adaptive (C-A), language-social (L-S), interquartile range (IQR),1 year postpartum (C-1y).

M-T1; Overall median 14.6 (IQR 11.9–17.9), Boys median 14.6 (IQR 12.0–18.1), Girls median 14.4 (IQR 11.9–17.7) pregnant week.

M-T2; Overall median 27.3 (IQR 25.1–30.0), Boys median 27.4 (IQR 25.3–30.3), Girls median 27.0 (IQR 25.1–29.9) pregnant weeks.

Table 3. Bivariate analysis of Maternal K6 and KSPD in eight trajectories groups.

Abbreviations: Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K6), Kyoto Scale of Psychological Development 2001(KSPD), developmental quotient (DQ), cognitive-adaptive (C-A), language-social (L-S), partial regression coefficient (B), confidence interval (CI), standardized partial regression coefficients (β), interquartile range (IQR),1 year postpartum (C-1y).

M-T1; Overall median 14.6 (IQR 11.9–17.9), Boys median 14.6 (IQR 12.0–18.1), Girls median 14.4 (IQR 11.9–17.7) pregnant weeks. M-T2; Overall median 27.3 (IQR 25.1–30.0), Boys median 27.4 (IQR 25.3–30.3), Girls median 27.0 (IQR 25.1–29.9) pregnant weeks.

Multiple regression analysis without the stepwise method showed significantly lower scores with maternal K6 scores ≥ 5 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y for the L-S DQ (partial regression coefficient (B): −4.09, 95% confidence interval [CI]: −6.88 – −1.31, β: −0.075, p = 0.004) areas, compared to those with maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y (Table 4). The groups with maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at any one or two periods during M-T1, M-T2, and C-1y were not significantly different from those with maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y (Table 4). There were no significant differences in the C-A DQ area (Table 4).

Table 4. Multiple regression analysis of Maternal K6 and KSPD in eight trajectories groups.

Abbreviations: Kyoto Scale of Psychological Development 2001(KSPD), developmental quotient (DQ), cognitive-adaptive (C-A), language-social (L-S), partial regression coefficient (B), confidence interval (CI), standardized partial regression coefficients (β), interquartile range (IQR),1 year postpartum (C-1y).

M-T1; Overall median 14.6 (IQR 11.9–17.9), Boys median 14.6 (IQR 12.0–18.1), Girls median 14.4 (IQR 11.9–17.7) pregnant weeks.

M-T2; Overall median 27.3 (IQR 25.1–30.0), Boys median 27.4 (IQR 25.3–30.3), Girls median 27.0 (IQR 25.1–29.9) pregnant weeks.

Adjusted for age of mother at the delivery, unplanned pregnancy, infertility treatment, marital status, maternal highest level of education, paternal highest level of education, maternal smoking during pregnancy, paternal smoking during pregnancy, maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy, annual household income, maternal neuropsychiatric disorders, psychoactive drug use during pregnancy, pregnancy complications, obstetric labor complications, mode of delivery, birth weight of children, gestational week of delivery, feeding method at 6 months postpartum, family structure, number of children included subject, attendance age of daycare center, location of regional center, and sex of children for overall.

Multiple regression analysis with the stepwise method showed significantly lower scores with maternal K6 scores ≥ 5 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y for the L-S DQ (partial regression coefficient (B): −3.70, 95% confidence interval [CI]: −6.41 – −0.98, β: −0.068, p = 0.008) areas, compared to those with maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y (Table 5). The groups with maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at any one or two periods during M-T1, M-T2, and C-1y were not significantly different from those with maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y (Table 5). There were no significant differences in the C-A DQ area (Table 5).

Table 5. Multiple regression analysis with stepwise method of Maternal K6 and KSPD in eight trajectories groups.

Abbreviations: Kyoto Scale of Psychological Development 2001(KSPD), developmental quotient (DQ), cognitive-adaptive (C-A), language-social (L-S), partial regression coefficient (B), confidence interval (CI), standardized partial regression coefficients (β), interquartile range (IQR),1 year postpartum (C-1y).

M-T1; Overall median 14.6 (IQR 11.9–17.9), Boys median 14.6 (IQR 12.0–18.1), Girls median 14.4 (IQR 11.9–17.7) pregnant weeks.

M-T2; Overall median 27.3 (IQR 25.1–30.0), Boys median 27.4 (IQR 25.3–30.3), Girls median 27.0 (IQR 25.1–29.9) pregnant weeks.

Overall; C-A DQ: Adjusted for unplanned pregnancy, marital status, maternal highest level of education, paternal highest level of education, paternal smoking during pregnancy, annual household income (×1000 yen/year) during pregnancy, maternal neuropsychiatric disorders, sex of children, birth weight of children (grams), attendance at daycare center (attendance age), Regional Center.

Overall; L-S DQ: Adjusted for maternal highest level of education, paternal highest level of education, paternal smoking during pregnancy, annual household income (×1000 yen/year) during pregnancy, pregnancy complications, sex of children, birth weigt of children (grams), feeding method at postpartum 6 months, number of children included subject, attendance at daycare center (attendance age), Regional Center.

Boys; C-A DQ: Adjusted for marital status, maternal highest level of education, paternal highest level of education, paternal smoking during pregnancy, annual household income (×1000 yen/year) during pregnancy, birth weight of children (grams), number of children included subject, Regional Center.

Boys; L-S DQ: Adjusted for age of mother at the delivery (years), maternal highest level of education, paternal highest level of education, paternal smoking during pregnancy, annual household income (×1000 yen/year) during pregnancy, birth weight of children (grams), gestation week of delivery, number of children included subject, Regional Center.

Girls; C-A DQ: Adjusted for marital status, maternal highest level of education, paternal highest level of education, paternal smoking during pregnancy, annual household income (×1000 yen/year) during pregnancy, maternal neuropsychiatric disorders, mode of delivery, birth weight of children (grams), gestation week of delivery, feeding method at postpartum 6 months, family structure, attendance at daycare center (attendance age), Regional Center.

Girls; L-S DQ: Adjusted for maternal highest level of education, paternal highest level of education, annual household income (×1000 yen/year), during pregnancy, maternal neuropsychiatric disorders, obstetric labor complications, feeding method at postpartum 6 months, number of children included subject, attendance at daycare center (attendance age), Regional Center.

Girls

The participants were divided into eight trajectory groups (Table 1). Table 2 depicts the one-way ANOVA results of maternal K6 and KSPD. Table 3 depicts the bivariate analysis of maternal K6 and KSPD.

Multiple regression analysis without the stepwise method showed no significant differences in maternal K6 scores ≥ 5 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y for the L-S DQ (B: −2.22, 95% CI: −4.60 − 0.17, β: −0.045, p = 0.07) areas, compared to maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y (Table 4). The groups with maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at any one or two periods during M-T1, M-T2, and C-1y were also not significantly different compared to those with maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y (Table 4). There were no significant differences in the C-A DQ area (Table 4).

Multiple regression analysis with the stepwise method showed no significant differences in maternal K6 scores ≥ 5 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y for the L-S DQ (B: −2.24, 95% CI: −4.58 – 0.10, β: −0.046, p = 0.06) areas, compared to maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y (Table 5). The groups with maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at any one or two periods during M-T1, M-T2, and C-1y were also not significantly different compared to those with maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y (Table 5). There were no significant differences in the C-A DQ area (Table 5).

Overall

Multiple regression analysis without the stepwise method showed significantly low scores with maternal K6 ≥ 5 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y for the C-A DQ (B: −2.00, 95% CI: −3.65 − −0.35, β: −0.043, p = 0.02) and L-S DQ areas (B: −3.14, 95% CI: −4.96 − −1.33, β: −0.061, p = 0.001), compared to those with maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y (Table 4). The groups with maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at any one or two periods during M-T1, M-T2, and C-1y were also not significantly different from those with maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y (Table 4).

Multiple regression analysis with the stepwise method showed significantly low scores with maternal K6 ≥ 5 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y for the C-A DQ (B: −2.01, 95% CI: −3.65 − −0.38, β: −0.043, p = 0.02) and L-S DQ areas (B: −3.27, 95% CI: −5.04 − −1.50, β: −0.063, p = 0.0003), compared to those with maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y (Table 5). The groups with maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at any one or two periods during M-T1, M-T2, and C-1y were also not significantly different from those with maternal K6 scores ≤ 4 at both M-T1 and M-T2 and C-1y (Table 5).

Discussion

In the present study of 4-year-old children, persistent maternal psychological distress (K6 scores ≥ 5) from the first half of pregnancy to 1 year postpartum tended to be associated with lower verbal cognitive development in boys, but not in girls. In contrast, in the group without persistent maternal psychological distress from the first half of pregnancy to 1 year postpartum, no significant impact was observed on verbal cognitive development, regardless of the children’s sex.

Sex-specific differences

Our study showed that maternal perinatal psychological distress tended to lead to lower verbal cognitive development in boys, but not in girls, at 4 years of age. At present, the reasons and mechanisms behind this phenomenon remain unknown, and further research is warranted. It is worth noting that although not specific to verbal cognitive development, several studies have found sex-specific effects, suggesting that boys are more vulnerable to maternal perinatal stress than girls. Reference Gentile1,Reference van den Bergh, van den Heuvel and Lahti2,Reference Rees, Channon and Waters5 King et al. reported a linear decline in IQ with increasing maternal prenatal objective stress exposure among boys. However, no such effect was observed among girls aged 11 years. Reference King, Dancause, Turcotte-Tremblay, Veru and Laplante19 Simcock et al. reported that higher levels of objective flood exposure predicted a more irritable temperament in boys, but not girls, at the age of 6 months. Reference Simcock, Elgbeili and Laplante20 Gerardin et al. reported that infants of mothers with prenatal depression presented higher scores on generalized anxiety, activity/impulsivity, and sleep problems than controls, particularly boys aged 1 year. Reference Gerardin, Wendland and Bodeau21 Loomans et al. reported that prenatal maternal anxiety was associated with hyperactivity/inattention in boys aged 5 years. Reference Loomans, van der Stelt and Van Eijsden22 Li et al. reported that prenatal maternal exposure to severe stress increases the risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in boys. Reference Li, Olsen, Vestergaard and Obel23 Zhu et al. also reported that in mothers who experienced severe stressful prenatal life events, there was an increased risk for ADHD in boys, but not in girls. Reference Zhu, Hao and Tao24 Fineberg et al. reported that maternal daily-life stress during pregnancy was associated with significantly increased odds of schizophrenia spectrum disorders in boys. Reference Fineberg, Ellman and Schaefer25 Glasheen et al. reported that maternal prenatal and postnatal anxiety was associated with a higher risk of conduct disorder among boys; however, it was less likely in girls aged 16 years. Reference Glasheen, Richardson and Kim26

Others have found girls to be more vulnerable, particularly to emotional problems. Buss et al. reported that higher maternal cortisol levels during early gestation were associated with more affective problems in girls aged 7.5 years. Reference Buss, Davis and Shahbaba27 Braithwaite et al. reported that girls exposed to high levels of maternal prenatal cortisol were more emotionally negative than boys at 2 months of age. Reference Braithwaite, Murphy, Ramchandani and Hill28,Reference Braithwaite, Pickles and Sharp29 Wright et al. reported that an elevated maternal prenatal cortisol level was associated with lower callous-unemotional traits in girls, but not in boys, at 2.5–5.0 years of age. Reference Wright, Pickles, Braithwaite, Sharp and Hill30 As this study was based solely on K6 points based on the questionnaire and did not examine cortisol levels, future studies are needed to determine how the effects of K6 trajectories and cortisol levels on children differ by sex.

Sex differences have been observed in rodents. For example, Weinstock found that male rats subjected to prenatal stress had greater learning deficits than did female rats, with greater long-term hippocampal potentiation, hippocampal neurogenesis, and reduced dendritic spine density in the prefrontal cortex. Reference Weinstock31Reference Zagron and Weinstock33 These sex differences may be due to the developing brain regions' sensitivity to stress hormones. Decreased testosterone and aromatase activity combined with the effects of other corticosteroids may contribute to learning deficits in male rats. Reference Weinstock34 In contrast, estrogen has protective effects on brain regions associated with learning and memory in rats and mice. Reference Liu, Day and Muñiz35Reference Weinstock37

Clinical periods vulnerable to the effect of maternal psychological distress trajectories

Our study examined maternal psychological distress during three periods: the first half of pregnancy at approximately 14 weeks, the second half of pregnancy at approximately 27 weeks, and 1 year postpartum. We observed that persistent maternal psychological distress from the first half of pregnancy to 1 year postpartum had a negative association with verbal cognitive development in boys. Even overall, persistent maternal psychological distress from the first half of pregnancy to 1 year postpartum had a disadvantageous association with verbal and nonverbal cognitive development. No such association was noted in mothers who were not psychologically distressed (K6 scores ≤ 4) at any one or two of these three periods.

In a similar study that focused specifically on children’s cognitive development, Lin et al. reported that children’s cognitive development may be more susceptible to prenatal exposure to maternal emotional stress than postnatal emotional stress, at the age of 24–30 months. Reference Lin, Xu and Huang38

Although not specific to cognitive development, several studies have reported clinical periods associated with children’s vulnerability to the effects of maternal psychological distress trajectories on their neurodevelopment. Van der Waerden et al. reported that children whose mothers had persistent depressive symptoms, either intermediate or severe, from pregnancy to the postpartum period, had the greatest emotional and behavioral difficulties at the age of 5 years. Reference van der Waerden, Galéra and Larroque39 Fransson et al. reported that maternal prenatal and postpartum depression is associated with more behavioral problems in children. Girls are affected to a greater degree at 18 months of age. Reference Fransson, Sörensen and Kunovac Kallak40 Lin et al. reported that children’s temperamental development may be more susceptible to postnatal exposure to maternal emotional stress than to prenatal exposure, at the age of 24–30 months. Reference Lin, Xu and Huang38

For reference, although the research design blended prenatal and postpartum periods, Srinivasan et al. reported that children of mothers who experience depression in the perinatal period are more likely to report psychotic experiences at 18 years of age. Reference Srinivasan, Pearson, Johnson, Lewis and Lewis41 Oh et al. also reported that maternal perinatal depression up to 2 years postpartum affects childhood behavioral problems and executive function at 9 years of age. Reference Oh, Joung, Baek and Yoo42

Clinical implications

Undoubtedly, prenatal and postpartum maternal depression, anxiety, and stress negatively affect neurodevelopment, including cognitive performance, in children.

In our previous study using JECS data, persistent maternal psychological distress from the first to the second half of pregnancy was a risk factor for autism spectrum disorder among children. Reference Nishigori, Hashimoto and Mori14 Our current study also suggests that persistent maternal psychological distress from the first half of pregnancy to the first year postpartum had a negative effect on boys' verbal cognitive development at 4 years of age. In light of this finding, assessing the mental health of pregnant women during the early stages of pregnancy is essential. For women who experience psychological distress during early pregnancy, appropriate interventions to prevent continued psychological distress throughout the pregnancy and at least in the postpartum period are important.

Limitations

This study has several limitations. First, K6 is a self-administered questionnaire; therefore, the mothers’ psychological distress was not medically diagnosed. However, this questionnaire can be used because previous studies have shown reasonable results with it. Reference Furukawa, Kawakami and Saitoh12,Reference Sakurai, Nishi, Kondo, Yanagida and Kawakami13 Second, the Sub-Cohort Study was based on 5,000 participants (5%) who were selected from the total births. In reality, 3,287 participants (3.3%) were analyzed in this study. There may be an intrinsic bias in the sub-sample used for the study.

Strengths of the study

This was a large prospective birth cohort study in which certified testers objectively assessed the cognitive development of 3,287 children at 4 years of age.

Conclusion

Persistent maternal psychological distress from the first half of pregnancy to 1 year postpartum had a disadvantageous association with verbal cognitive development in boys, but not in girls, at 4 years of age. The JECS is a prospective study that plans to follow and evaluate the development of the targeted children until they reach 40 years of age. In the future, we plan to further evaluate children’s neurodevelopment, including changes in the effects of maternal prenatal and postnatal psychological distress on children’s development.

Data availability statement

Data are unsuitable for public deposition due to ethical restrictions and the legal framework of Japan. It is prohibited by the Act on the Protection of Personal Information (Act No.57 of 30 May 2003, amendment on 9 September 2015) to publicly deposit data containing personal information. Ethical Guidelines for Epidemiological Research enforced by the Japan Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology and the Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare also restricts the open sharing of epidemiologic data. All inquiries regarding access to data should be sent to: . The person responsible for handling enquiries sent to this email address is Dr Shoji F. Nakayama, JECS Programme Office, National Institute for Environmental Studies.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to all the participants of the study. Members of the JECS group as of 2023 are as follows: Michihiro Kamijima (principal investigator, Nagoya City University, Nagoya, Japan), Shin Yamazaki (National Institute for Environmental Studies, Tsukuba, Japan), Yukihiro Ohya (National Center for Child Health and Development, Tokyo, Japan), Reiko Kishi (Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan), Nobuo Yaegashi (Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan), Koichi Hashimoto (Fukushima Medical University, Fukushima, Japan), Chisato Mori (Chiba University, Chiba, Japan), Shuichi Ito (Yokohama City University, Yokohama, Japan), Zentaro Yamagata (University of Yamanashi, Chuo, Japan), Hidekuni Inadera (University of Toyama, Toyama, Japan), Takeo Nakayama (Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan), Tomotaka Sobue (Osaka University, Suita, Japan), Masayuki Shima (Hyogo Medical University, Nishinomiya, Japan), Seiji Kageyama (Tottori University, Yonago, Japan), Narufumi Suganuma (Kochi University, Nankoku, Japan), Shoichi Ohga (Kyushu University, Fukuoka, Japan), and Takahiko Katoh (Kumamoto University, Kumamoto, Japan).

Author contribution

Hidekazu Nishigori and Toshie Nishigori are contributed equally to this work.

Financial support

This study was funded by the Ministry of the Environment, Japan. The findings and conclusions of this article are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not represent the official views of the above government.

Competing interests

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the Ministry of the Environment’s Institutional Review Board on Epidemiological Studies (no. 100910001) and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008, and have been approved by the institutional committees of all participating institutions. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

References

Gentile, S. Untreated depression during pregnancy: short- and long-term effects in offspring. A systematic review. Neuroscience. 2017; 342, 154166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van den Bergh, BRH, van den Heuvel, MI, Lahti, M, et al. Prenatal developmental origins of behavior and mental health: the influence of maternal stress in pregnancy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020; 117, 2664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aoyagi, SS, Tsuchiya, KJ. Does maternal postpartum depression affect children’s developmental outcomes? J Obstet Gynaecol Res. 2019; 45(9), 18091820.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oyetunji, A, Chandra, P. Postpartum stress and infant outcome: a review of current literature. Psychiatry Res. 2020; 284, 112769.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rees, S, Channon, S, Waters, CS. The impact of maternal prenatal and postnatal anxiety on children’s emotional problems: a systematic review. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2019; 28(2), 257280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kawamoto, T, Nitta, H, Murata, K, et al. Rationale and study design of the Japan environment and children’s study (JECS). BMC Public Health. 2014; 14(1), 25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michikawa, T, Nitta, H, Nakayama, SF, et al. Baseline profile of participants in the Japan environment and children’s study (JECS). J Epidemiol. 2018; 28(2), 99104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sekiyama, M, Yamazaki, S, Michikawa, T, et al. Study design and participants’ profile in the sub-cohort study in the Japan environment and children’s study (JECS). J Epidemiol. 2022; 32(5), 228236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mezawa, H, Tomotaki, A, Yamamoto-Hanada, K, et al. Prevalence of congenital anomalies in the Japan environment and children’s study. J Epidemiol. 2019; 29(7), 247256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, RC, Andrews, G, Colpe, LJ, et al. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychol Med. 2002; 32(6), 959976.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, RC, Barker, PR, Colpe, LJ, et al. Screening for serious mental illness in the general population. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003; 60(2), 184189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Furukawa, TA, Kawakami, N, Saitoh, M, et al. The performance of the Japanese version of the K6 and K10 in the world mental health survey Japan. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2008; 17(3), 152158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sakurai, K, Nishi, A, Kondo, K, Yanagida, K, Kawakami, N. Screening performance of K6/K10 and other screening instruments for mood and anxiety disorders in Japan. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2011; 65(5), 434441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishigori, T, Hashimoto, K, Mori, M, et al. Association between maternal prenatal psychological distress and autism spectrum disorder among 3-year-old children: the Japan environment and children’s study. J Dev Orig Health Dis. 2023; 14(1), 7076.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mori, M, Nishigori, T, Ogata, Y, et al. Maternal prenatal psychological distress and motor/cognitive development in two-year-old offspring: the Japan environment and children’s study. J Dev Orig Health Dis. 2023; 14(3), 389401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koyama, T, Osada, H, Tsujii, H, Kurita, H. Utility of the kyoto scale of psychological development in cognitive assessment of children with pervasive developmental disorders. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2009; 63(2), 241243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Society for the Kyoto Scale of Psychological Development Test. Shinpan K Shiki Hattatsu Kensahou 2001 Nenban (the Kyoto Scale of Psychological Development Test 2001), 2008. Nakanishiya Shuppan, Kyoto Japan.Google Scholar
Nishigori, H, Nishigori, T, Obara, T, et al. Prenatal folic acid supplement/dietary folate and cognitive development in 4-year-old offspring from the Japan environment and children’s study. Sci Rep. 2023; 13(1), 9541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, S, Dancause, K, Turcotte-Tremblay, AM, Veru, F, Laplante, DP. Using natural disasters to study the effects of prenatal maternal stress on child health and development. Birth Defects Res C Embryo Today. 2012; 96(4), 273288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simcock, G, Elgbeili, G, Laplante, DP, et al. The effects of prenatal maternal stress on early temperament: the 2011 Queensland flood study. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2017; 38(5), 310321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerardin, P, Wendland, J, Bodeau, N, et al. Depression during pregnancy: is the developmental impact earlier in boys? A prospective case-control study. J Clin Psychiatry. 2011; 72(03), 378387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loomans, EM, van der Stelt, O, Van Eijsden, M, et al. Antenatal maternal anxiety is associated with problem behaviour at age five. Early Hum Dev. 2011; 87(8), 565570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, J, Olsen, J, Vestergaard, M, Obel, C. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the offspring following prenatal maternal bereavement: a nationwide follow-up study in Denmark. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010; 19(10), 747753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhu, P, Hao, JH, Tao, RX, et al. Sex-specific and time-dependent effects of prenatal stress on the early behavioral symptoms of ADHD: a longitudinal study in China. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015; 24(9), 11391147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fineberg, AM, Ellman, LM, Schaefer, CA, et al. Fetal exposure to maternal stress and risk for schizophrenia spectrum disorders among offspring: differential influences of fetal sex. Psychiatry Res. 2016; 236, 9197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glasheen, C, Richardson, GA, Kim, KH, et al. Exposure to maternal pre- and postnatal depression and anxiety symptoms: risk for major depression, anxiety disorders, and conduct disorder in adolescent offspring. Dev Psychopathol. 2013; 25(4pt1), 10451063.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, C, Davis, EP, Shahbaba, B, et al. Maternal cortisol over the course of pregnancy and subsequent child amygdala and hippocampus volumes and affective problems. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012; 109(20), E1312E1319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braithwaite, EC, Murphy, SE, Ramchandani, PG, Hill, J. Associations between biological markers of prenatal stress and infant negative emotionality are specific to sex. Psychoneuroendocrino. 2017; 86, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braithwaite, EC, Pickles, A, Sharp, H, et al. Maternal prenatal cortisol predicts infant negative emotionality in a sex-dependent manner. Physiol Behav. 2017; 175, 3136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, N, Pickles, A, Braithwaite, EC, Sharp, H, Hill, J. Sex-dependent associations between maternal prenatal cortisol and child callous-unemotional traits: findings from the wirral child health and development study. Psychoneuroendocrino. 2019; 109, 104409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinstock, M. Gender differences in the effects of prenatal stress on brain development and behaviour. Neurochem Res. 2007; 32(10), 17301740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, KM, Pearson, JN, Neeley, EW, et al. Maternal stress during pregnancy causes sex-specific alterations in offspring memory performance, social interactions, indices of anxiety, and body mass. Physiol Behav. 2011; 104(2), 340347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zagron, G, Weinstock, M. Maternal adrenal hormone secretion mediates behavioural alterations induced by prenatal stress in male and female rats. Behav Brain Res. 2006; 175(2), 323328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinstock, M. Sex-dependent changes induced by prenatal stress in cortical and hippocampal morphology and behaviour in rats: an update. Ann Ny Acad Sci. 2011; 14(6), 604613.Google ScholarPubMed
Liu, F, Day, M, Muñiz, LC, et al. Activation of estrogen receptor-β regulates hippocampal synaptic plasticity and improves memory. Nat Neurosci. 2008; 11(3), 334343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ping, SE, Trieu, J, Wlodek, ME, Barrett, GL. Effects of estrogen on basal forebrain cholinergic neurons and spatial learning. J Neurosci Res. 2008; 86(7), 15881598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinstock, M. Prenatal stressors in rodents: effects on behavior. Neurobiol Stress. 2017; 6, 313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, Y, Xu, J, Huang, J, et al. Effects of prenatal and postnatal maternal emotional stress on toddlers’ cognitive and temperamental development. J Affect Disord. 2017; 207, 917.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Waerden, J, Galéra, C, Larroque, B, et al. Maternal depression trajectories and children’s behavior at Age 5 years. J Pediatr. 2015; 166(6), 14401448.e1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fransson, E, Sörensen, F, Kunovac Kallak, TK, et al. Maternal perinatal depressive symptoms trajectories and impact on toddler behavior – the importance of symptom duration and maternal bonding. J Affect Disord. 2020; 273, 542551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Srinivasan, R, Pearson, RM, Johnson, S, Lewis, G, Lewis, G. Maternal perinatal depressive symptoms and offspring psychotic experiences at 18 years of age: a longitudinal study. Lancet Psychiat. 2020; 7(5), 431440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oh, Y, Joung, YS, Baek, JH, Yoo, N. Maternal depression trajectories and child executive function over 9 years. J Affect Disord. 2020; 276, 646652 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Characteristics of participants (total = 3287)

Figure 1

Figure 1. Flow chart depicting research participants’ selection.

Figure 2

Table 2. ANOVA of Maternal K6 and KSPD in eight trajectories groups.

Figure 3

Table 3. Bivariate analysis of Maternal K6 and KSPD in eight trajectories groups.

Figure 4

Table 4. Multiple regression analysis of Maternal K6 and KSPD in eight trajectories groups.

Figure 5

Table 5. Multiple regression analysis with stepwise method of Maternal K6 and KSPD in eight trajectories groups.