Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T22:58:29.017Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Course of Cognitive Functioning in Institutionalized Persons With Moderate to Severe Dementia: Evidence From the Severe Impairment Battery Short Version

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 November 2018

Evelien T. Wolf*
Affiliation:
Section Clinical Neuropsychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Wouter D. Weeda
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, the Netherlands
Roland B. Wetzels
Affiliation:
Department of Primary and Community Care, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Radboud Alzheimer Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Zzg, Elderly Care Organization, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
Jos F. M. de Jonghe
Affiliation:
Geriatric Medicine, North West Hospital Group, Alkmaar, The Netherlands
Raymond C. T. M. Koopmans
Affiliation:
Department of Primary and Community Care, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Radboud Alzheimer Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Joachim en Anna, Centre for Specialized Geriatric Care, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Evelien Wolf, Section Clinical Neuropsychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Van der Boechorststraat 7, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands. E-mail address: e.t.wolf@vu.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objectives: To adequately monitor the course of cognitive functioning in persons with moderate to severe dementia, relevant cognitive tests for the advanced dementia stages are needed. We examined the ability of a test developed for the advanced dementia stages, the Severe Impairment Battery Short version (SIB-S), to measure cognitive change over time. Second, we examined type of memory impairment measured with the SIB-S in different dementia stages. Methods: Participants were institutionalized persons with moderate to severe dementia (N = 217). The SIB-S was administered at 6-month intervals during a 2-year period. Dementia severity at baseline was classified according to Global Deterioration Scale criteria. We used mixed models to evaluate the course of SIB-S total and domain scores, and whether dementia stage at baseline affected these courses. Results: SIB-S total scores declined significantly over time, and the course of decline differed significantly between dementia stages at baseline. Persons with moderately severe dementia declined faster in mean SIB-S total scores than persons with moderate or severe dementia. Between persons with moderate and moderately severe dementia, there was only a difference in the rate of decline of semantic items, but not episodic and non-semantic items. Conclusions: Although modest floor and slight ceiling effects were noted in severe and milder cases, respectively, the SIB-S proved to be one of few available adequate measures of cognitive change in institutionalized persons with moderate to severe dementia. (JINS, 2019, 25, 204–214)

Type
Regular Research
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2018

INTRODUCTION

Dementia due to neurodegenerative disease is primarily characterized by a progressive deterioration of cognitive functioning (Grand, Caspar, & Macdonald, Reference Grand, Caspar and Macdonald2011). This deterioration eventually leads to increased dependency on others, and it is a strong indicator of institutionalization (Gaugler, Yu, Krichbaum, & Wyman, Reference Gaugler, Yu, Krichbaum and Wyman2009). Consequently, the vast majority of persons with dementia reside in institutions 8 years after the initial dementia diagnosis (Luppa, Luck, Brähler, König, & Riedel-Heller, Reference Luppa, Luck, Brähler, König and Riedel-Heller2008). Institutionalized persons with dementia have on average more severe dementia than community dwelling persons with dementia (Boersma, Eefsting, Van Den Brink, & Van Tilburg, Reference Boersma, Eefsting, Van Den Brink and Van Tilburg1997), and relevant cognitive tests may be needed to monitor the course of cognitive functioning in more advanced dementia stages. In this way, pharmacological and non-pharmacological interventions targeting cognition, which are increasingly implemented in institutionalized persons with dementia, can be adequately evaluated.

One of the major problems with measuring the course of cognitive functioning in persons with dementia is floor and ceiling effects of cognitive tests. Although the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975) has often been used to monitor the course of cognitive functioning in persons with mild to moderate dementia (Galasko, Gould, Abramson, & Salmon, Reference Galasko, Gould, Abramson and Salmon2000; Thomas, Albert, Petersen, & Aisen, Reference Thomas, Albert, Petersen and Aisen2016), it shows a floor effect in more advanced dementia (Galasko et al., Reference Galasko, Gould, Abramson and Salmon2000). Several tests for advanced dementia have been developed. Examples of commonly used tests are the Severe Cognitive Impairment Profile (SCIP; Peavy et al., Reference Peavy, Salmon, Rice, Samuel, Taylor, Ernesto and Thal1996), Severe Mini-Mental State Examination (SMMSE; Harrell, Marson, Chatterjee, & Parrish, Reference Harrell, Marson, Chatterjee and Parrish2000), and Severe Impairment Battery (SIB; Saxton, McGonigle-Gibson, Swihart, Miller, & Boller, Reference Saxton, McGonigle-Gibson, Swihart, Miller and Boller1990; Panisset, Roudier, Saxton, & Boiler, Reference Panisset, Roudier, Saxton and Boiler1994).

Compared to the MMSE, these three tests have shown reduced floor effects in advanced dementia. Although an ability to detect cognitive change over time was established for both the SCIP (Peavy et al., Reference Peavy, Salmon, Rice, Samuel, Taylor, Ernesto and Thal1996) and the SIB (Black et al., Reference Black, Doody, Li, McRae, Jambor, Xu and Richardson2007; Feldman et al., Reference Feldman, Gauthier, Hecker, Vellas, Xu, Ieni and Willmer2005; Schmitt et al., Reference Schmitt, Ashford, Ernesto, Saxton, Schneider and Clark1997; Wild & Kaye, Reference Wild and Kaye1998), these tests take 30 minutes or more to administer, and this may be too long for persons with an MMSE-score of 5 or below or for those who are very agitated (Saxton et al., Reference Saxton, Kastango, Hugonot-Diener, Boller, Verny, Sarles and DeKosky2005). The SMMSE can be administered in 5 minutes, but has some problems with measuring cognitive change over time (Harrell et al., Reference Harrell, Marson, Chatterjee and Parrish2000). It is important that a test for advanced dementia has a short administration time and is able to detect cognitive change over time.

A short version of the SIB (SIB-S) was developed to make the SIB more suitable for persons with a short attention span and thus to minimize floor effects (Saxton et al., Reference Saxton, Kastango, Hugonot-Diener, Boller, Verny, Sarles and DeKosky2005). The SIB-S takes approximately 10–15 min to administer, and was validated in several studies (Ahn, Kim, Saxton & Kim, Reference Ahn, Kim, Saxton and Kim2007; de Jonghe, Wetzels, Mulders, Zuidema, & Koopmans, Reference de Jonghe, Wetzels, Mulders, Zuidema and Koopmans2009; Saxton et al., Reference Saxton, Kastango, Hugonot-Diener, Boller, Verny, Sarles and DeKosky2005). Low SIB-S scores were associated with cognitive impairment as measured with the MMSE, dementia severity, and functional dependency in activities of daily living. In addition, very minimal floor effects were evidenced (Ahn et al., Reference Ahn, Kim, Saxton and Kim2007; de Jonghe et al., Reference de Jonghe, Wetzels, Mulders, Zuidema and Koopmans2009; Saxton et al., Reference Saxton, Kastango, Hugonot-Diener, Boller, Verny, Sarles and DeKosky2005). The ability of the SIB-S to measure cognitive change in persons with moderate to severe dementia, however, has not yet been studied.

Our main objective was to examine the ability of the SIB-S to measure cognitive change in institutionalized persons with moderate to severe dementia. To examine to what extent the SIB-S is susceptible to floor and ceiling effects in the long-term, we also took into account the different dementia stages. In addition, as during the early phases of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) the decline of semantic memory is often less pronounced and less consistent compared to episodic memory (Hodges & Patterson, Reference Hodges and Patterson1995; Perry, Watson, & Hodges, Reference Perry, Watson and Hodges2000), it is possible that aspects of semantic memory are better preserved in the advanced stages of dementia. Therefore, we explored if the ability of participants to complete semantic, non-semantic, and episodic memory items of the SIB-S was related to their diagnosed dementia stage and to their overall cognitive functioning.

METHODS

Subjects

This cohort study was part of a larger prospective cohort study in which participants were enrolled from 14 dementia special care units from 9 nursing homes in the Netherlands (Wetzels, Zuidema, de Jonghe, Verhey, & Koopmans, Reference Wetzels, Zuidema, de Jonghe, Verhey and Koopmans2010). This same sample has been used to validate the SIB-S in the Netherlands (de Jonghe et al., Reference de Jonghe, Wetzels, Mulders, Zuidema and Koopmans2009). Participants’ elderly care physicians (Koopmans, Pellegrom, & van der Geer, Reference Koopmans, Pellegrom and van der Geer2017) systematically screened all participants for inclusion. Participants were considered for inclusion provided they: (1) met the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders fourth edition criteria for dementia (American Psychiatric Association, 2000); (2) had no life-threatening disease at the time of inclusion; (3) had to reside in a nursing home for at least 4 weeks. Participants were followed for 2 years and all assessments were administered at baseline and subsequently during 4 biannual follow up visits.

Measures

The course of cognitive functioning was assessed with the Severe Impairment Battery Short version (SIB-S; Saxton et al., Reference Saxton, Kastango, Hugonot-Diener, Boller, Verny, Sarles and DeKosky2005). This test, which was developed for the more advanced dementia stages, has a total score ranging from 0 to 50. See Table 1 for a representation of the individual items of the SIB-S per subscale. The SIB-S has been translated into Dutch and has been validated (de Jonghe et al., Reference de Jonghe, Wetzels, Mulders, Zuidema and Koopmans2009). Furthermore, the SIB-S has a high reliability and a high concurrent validity with the MMSE (de Jonghe et al., Reference de Jonghe, Wetzels, Mulders, Zuidema and Koopmans2009).

Table 1 SIB-S items per subscale

To create a domain of items that relied mostly on Semantic memory, we added the Language, Praxis, Visuospatial ability, and Construction subscales, creating a score ranging from 0 to 31. For the Episodic memory domain, we used the Memory subscale minus item 3 (immediate recall of the examiner’s name) with a score ranging from 0 to 5. To create a domain of items that relied least on semantic memory (Memory-Orientation-Attention), we added the Memory, Orientation, and Attention subscales with a score ranging from 0 to 13. In case a participant could not communicate due to severe dementia a total score of 0 was given. If a participant did not cooperate at a particular assessment, this assessment was registered as missing.

Type of dementia diagnosis was established by the elderly care physicians using international consensus criteria (McKhann et al., Reference McKhann, Drachman, Folstein, Katzman, Price and Stadlan1984; Roman et al., Reference Roman, Tatemichi, Erkinjuntti, Cummings, Masdeu, Garcia and Scheinberg1993) and the Dutch consensus guidelines for AD, vascular dementia (VaD), mixed AD/VaD, and other diagnosis. Independently of the elderly care physicians, one of the authors (R.B.W.) checked eligibility and diagnosis of all participants against their clinical notes. In cases of doubt, consensus meetings were organized to ensure inclusion of participants who met the inclusion criteria. See Table 2 for a representation of dementia subtypes diagnosed in the sample.

Table 2 Subtypes of dementia for the whole sample

AD, Alzheimer’s disease; VaD, vascular dementia.

The Global Deterioration Scale (GDS) for Primary Degenerative Dementia (Reisberg, Ferris, de Leon, & Crook, Reference Reisberg, Ferris, De Leon and Crook1982) was used to determine the severity/stage of dementia at baseline. This assessment scale consists of a seven-point scale: (1) no cognitive decline; (2) mild memory loss reported by the participant, but the tests show no loss of function; (3) mild cognitive impairment (MCI); (4) early dementia; (5) moderate dementia; (6) moderately severe dementia; and (7) severe cognitive decline/late dementia. In this particular cohort study, only participants with a GDS-score of 5, 6, or 7 were included.

The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975) was administered for baseline characteristics.

Activities of daily living (ADL) dependency was measured with section G of the Interrai Long-Term Care Facility scale (2005, version 07). For this study, the hierarchical ADL scale was used, which consists of 4 of the ADL items of the Interrai (Morris, Fries, & Morris, Reference Morris, Fries and Morris1999). A hierarchical decision tree was used to obtain ADL dependency scores: (0) independent (in all 4 ADL items), (1) supervision, (2) limited, (3) extensive 1, (4) extensive 2, (5) dependent, and (6) total dependence. The hierarchical ADL scale has good to excellent interrater reliability and a Cronbach’s alpha of .90 (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Fries and Morris1999).

Ethical Considerations

This study was approved by the regional research ethics committee and completed in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration. The participants and/or their relatives and legal guardians were informed about the study and gave their written consent.

Statistical Analysis

The analyses were conducted with Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version 23 and R version 3.4.2 (R Core Team, 2017). To compare the baseline characteristics between the different dementia stages (GDS-score), Pearson χ2-tests for gender, education, medication use, and ADL dependency were conducted. The data for age, years in nursing home, years in nursing home unit, MMSE-score, and SIB-S score were not normally distributed in all separate GDS-groups. Therefore, non-parametric Independent Samples Kruskal-Wallis Tests were conducted to compare those variables between GDS-groups.

To examine concurrent validity, Kendall’s tau correlations between total SIB-S score and ADL dependency were calculated for all five assessments. To get an indication of how much measured SIB-S scores were spread around a “true” score, we calculated standard error of measurement (SEM) for all five assessments with the following formula: SEM = S* √(1–rkk).

Growth curve models were used to measure the course of SIB-S scores over time. These models can handle missing data and take into account multiple assessments within one subject. The standard model consisted of two levels: SIB-S scores at level 1, nested within subjects (level 2). Time (measured in days between T0 (representing 0 days) and T1, T0 and T2, T0 and T3, and T0 and T4) was the continuous independent variable and age at baseline (centered) was added as additional covariate.

To assess whether the course of SIB-S scores over time showed non-linearity, an additional quadratic Time regressor was added to the standard model. To analyze the difference in the course of SIB-S scores over time between dementia stages, GDS-score (3 categories) and a GDS-score by Time interaction were added as independent variables to the standard model. In case of a significant GDS-score by Time interaction, separate models for GDS-score were run to analyze the effect of Time on SIB-S scores.

To analyze if there were different effects for SIB-S domains, above mentioned growth curve models were also run for the domains Semantic memory, Episodic memory, and Memory-Orientation-Attention.

All models included a random intercept across participants. Random slopes for Time (heterogeneous first-order autoregressive structure) were added if the Likelihood Ratio Test between the random intercept model and the model with additional random slopes was significant (p < .05).

A Pearson χ2-test was conducted to examine if the number of dropouts at the fifth assessment differed between GDS-groups. In addition, a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to assess for significant differences in the percentage of total missing assessments between different GDS-groups. Furthermore, a one-way ANOVA, Pearson χ2-test or Mann-Whitney U (dependent on the outcome variable) was conducted to check whether there were differences in baseline characteristics between participants that completed and participants that did not complete the fifth assessment.

To check if GDS-score at baseline could predict the amount of individual variability in SIB-S scores over time, we conducted a non-parametric Independent Samples Kruskal-Wallis Test. To characterize individual progression characteristics of SIB-S scores over time, we performed a linear regression analysis [with SIB-S as dependent and Time (in days) as predictor] for each separate participant with at least three SIB-S assessments. The residual sums-of-squares (rss) was taken as the variability of the decline. To correct for missing assessments, we divided the rss by the number of assessments. In case of a significant effect of GDS-score on the variability of decline, Independent Samples Mann-Whitney U Tests were conducted to check which GDS-groups differed in the variability of decline.

To gain insight about the relationship between individual items of the SIB-S and overall cognitive functioning measured with the SIB-S, items were first ranked by how many participants could still successfully complete the item. As a measure for the items’ completion rate, per item the mean normalized SIB-S score was calculated, where the average and associated standard deviation were taken over all participants and all tests. Normalization was necessary before ranking, because item 17 and 25 are on a 0–1 scale, whereas the rest of the items are on a 0–1–2 scale. After ranking the items, test subjects were separated into 6 groups by their total SIB-S score. The scores 0 and 50 were each taken as a separate group, because these scores would show no variation in individual item scores. The rest of the population was separated in 4 equally sized groups. Then we plotted per item the completion rate by different stages of overall cognitive functioning. The plots show the items ranked based on the whole population, but with the mean normalized SIB-S score per item per group.

Alpha-level was set at .05. For post hoc analyses, Bonferroni correction was applied (α = .017).

RESULTS

Sample Characteristics

A total of 290 participants were eligible for inclusion. For 15 participants, no SIB-S data were available, and 29 participants did not meet inclusion criteria for this particular cohort. Twenty-nine participants scored zero on all SIB-S assessments. These participants probably did not understand test instructions at any assessment, and were excluded from further analyses. In the 217 remaining participants, no significant gender, age, education, or medication use differences were found between GDS-groups at baseline. As expected, MMSE-scores and SIB-S scores differed between GDS-groups, where persons with severe dementia had the lowest mean scores on the MMSE and SIB-S. Furthermore, persons with more severe dementia were on average most ADL dependent and were institutionalized the longest. See Table 3 for baseline characteristics and comparison between GDS-groups.

Table 3 Baseline characteristics of the sample

Note. GDS-score 5, moderate dementia; GDS-score 6, moderately severe dementia; GDS-score 7, severe cognitive decline/late dementia; p-value, results of the Pearson χ²-test for gender, education, medication use, ADL dependency, and Independent Samples Kruskal-Wallis Test for age, years in nursing home (unit), MMSE-score, and SIB-S score to compare between GDS-scores; η2 , eta squared, effect size for Independent Samples Kruskal-Wallis Test; V, Cramer’s V, effect size for Pearson ‘s χ²-test.

N/n, number of participants; SD, standard deviation; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; SIB-S, Severe Impairment Battery Short version.

A minimum of 1 SIB-S assessment (n = 217) and a maximum of 5 SIB-S assessments (n = 55) were available for each participant. Twenty participants had missing data at an assessment before their last assessment; thus, the number of participants at T4 (n = 70) was different from the number of participants that completed all 5 SIB-S assessments. Participants missed an average of two of five assessments. Reasons for dropping out were the move to another institution, death, too ill to be tested, or refusal of the participant to further cooperate. Participants who dropped out were older (on average 2.73 years; F(1,216) = 7.834; p = .006; η 2 = .035) and were more likely to use hypnotics (χ2(1) = 11.889; p = .020; η 2 = .026) than participants who completed the fifth assessment. There were no other significant differences in baseline characteristics between participants who completed and participants who dropped out at the fifth assessment.

Concurrent Validity

Low scores on SIB-S were associated with high ADL dependency scores on all five assessments, (T0: Kendall’s tau = -.363, p < .001; T1: Kendall’s tau = -.452, p < .001; T2: Kendall’s tau = -.478, p < .001; T3: Kendall’s tau = -.558, p < .001; T4: Kendall’s tau = -.665, p < .001).

SEM

For the SIB-S, the SEM was 3.21 points for T0, 2.89 points for T1, 2.91 points for T2, 2.96 points for T3, and 2.62 points for T4.

Growth Curve Models

For coefficients and standard deviations for all growth curve models, see Table 4.

Table 4 Growth curve models for SIB-S total and domain scores

Note. GDS-score 5, moderate dementia; GDS-score 6, moderately severe dementia; GDS-score 7, severe cognitive decline/late dementia

a Group with GDS-score 6 was used as reference group.

* p < .05.

** p < . 01.

*** p < .001.

SIB-S, Severe Impairment Battery Short version; M-O-ATT, Memory-Orientation-Attention domain.

Course of total SIB-S scores over time

Average total SIB-S scores declined significantly over time, F(1,114.058) = 65.240, p < .001. The addition of a quadratic time variable (Time²) to this model was not significant, χ2(1) = 1.245, p > .05. See Table 5 for number of participants and mean SIB-S scores per assessment. Not every participant declined in SIB-S scores between two assessments. Of the 217 participants, 152 had more than one assessment; whereas 67.1% of the participants scored lower, 9.2% were equivalent, and 23.7% scored higher at their last assessment in comparison to their first assessment.

Table 5 Mean SIB-S scores on successive assessments for the whole sample

N, number of participants; SD, standard deviation.

Effect of baseline dementia stage (GDS-score) on the course of SIB-S total scores over time

Baseline GDS-score had an effect on the rate of decline in SIB-S scores over time, F(2,104.960) = 8.517, p < .001. Mean SIB-S scores of persons with GDS-score 6 declined significantly faster than persons with GDS-score 5 (t(110.993) = 2.700; p = .008) or GDS-score 7 (t(103.490) = 3.912; p < .001). By contrast, there was no significant difference in the course of SIB-S scores over time between persons with GDS-score 5 and persons with GDS-score 7 (t(99.799) = 1.083; p = .282). The average decline on the SIB-S per half year (182.5 days) was 1.41 points for persons with GDS-score 5 (F(1,37.789) = 8.597; p = .006), 3.26 points for persons with GDS-score 6 (F(1,49.912) = 51.533; p < .001), and .82 points for persons with GDS-score 7 (F(1,119.121) = 9.559; p = .002). See Figure 1 for a visual representation of the progression of SIB-S scores over time for each dementia stage. As can be seen in Figure 1, there is considerable variability between individual participants with the same GDS-score in their course of SIB-S scores over time.

Fig. 1 Course of SIB-S scores over time for persons with moderate to severe dementia according to dementia stage (without a correction for age). The bold lines represent the mean course of SIB-S scores per GDS-score and the thin lines represent the course of SIB-S scores for each individual participant.

In addition, an Independent Samples Kruskal-Wallis Test showed a significant difference between GDS-groups in individual variability in SIB-S scores over time, H = 20.332, p < .001, η 2 = .150. Persons with GDS-score 7 had less individual variability than persons with GDS-score 5 (U = 313.000; p = .004; η 2 = .035) or GDS-score 6 (U = 427.000; p < .001; η 2 = .208).

The number of dropouts at the fifth assessment was significantly different between persons with GDS-score 5 (64.4%), GDS-score 6 (75%), and GDS-score 7 (56%), Pearson χ2(2) = 5.520, p = .049, V = 0.159. However, there was no significant difference in the percentage of total missing assessments between persons with GDS-score 5 (39.3%), GDS-score 6 (44.3%), and GDS-score 7 (34%), F(2,214) = 1.862, p = .158, η 2 = .017.

Course of SIB-S domains over time

For the whole sample, there was a significant average decline in Semantic memory (F(1,115.267) = 55.425; p < .001), Episodic memory (F(1,476.310) = 26.045; p < .001), and Memory-Orientation-Attention (F(1,451.032) = 61.151; p < .001).

Baseline GDS-score had a significant effect on the rate of decline in Semantic memory (F(2,106.473) = 9.418; p < .001) and Memory-Orientation-Attention (F(2,92.064) = 4.992; p = .009), but a non-significant effect on Episodic memory (F(2,496.335) = 3.103; p = .050). As the effect on Episodic memory was marginally significant, we also conducted post hoc tests for this subscale. In accordance with total SIB-S scores, Semantic memory declined significantly faster for persons with GDS-score 6 compared to persons with GDS-score 5 (t(109.734) = 2.856; p = .005) or GDS-score 7 (t(107.059) = 4.102; p < .001). For Episodic memory, however, only persons with GDS-score 6 declined significantly faster than persons with GDS-score 7, t(492.516) = 2.437, p = .015. In addition, also for Memory-Orientation-Attention only persons with GDS-score 6 declined significantly faster than persons with GDS-score 7, t(93.067) = 3.197, p = .002. For decline of domains per GDS-score, see Table 4.

Relation Between Individual Items and Overall Cognition

Figure 2 shows the ranking of the normalized mean score of the SIB-S items by how many patients could still successfully complete the item. The response on item 1 (shake hands) was mostly correct, while the response on item 12 (delayed recall of examiner’s name) was mostly incorrect. The large standard deviation of the mean normalized score of the items indicates substantial variation between participants in their response on the items. Furthermore, it was mainly the items concerning episodic memory (item 12 and 25) and praxis/construction (items 5, 14, and 24) that were the hardest for participants with moderate to severe dementia.

Fig. 2 Ranking of the items by how many participants could still successfully complete the item. As a measure for the items completion rate, per item the mean normalized SIB-S score was calculated. The average (represented by bullets) and associated standard deviation (represented by lines) were taken over all participants and all tests.

After ranking the items, we separated our participants into six groups based on their total SIB-S score: 0 (n = 78), 1–14 (n = 138), 15–31 (n = 140), 32–41 (n = 135), 42–49 (n = 148), and 50 (n = 6). Figure 3 shows the items ranked based on the whole population, but with the mean normalized SIB-S score per item per group. The pattern found earlier for the whole sample can also be seen in the ranking by subgroups. The variation of the mean normalized score of the items was smaller when divided by subgroups, especially in the groups with the lowest and highest total SIB-S scores. The hardest items showed little or no variation in the group with a SIB-S score 1–14. For example, all the participants with a SIB-S score 1–14 had an incorrect response on items concerning episodic memory (items 12 and 25) and praxis/construction (items 5, 6, and 14). In the group with a SIB-S score 42–49, the variation was the smallest for the easiest items.

Fig. 3 Ranking of the items by how many participants could still successfully complete the item per group (classified by total SIB-S score). The ranking was based on the whole population. As a measure for the item’s completion rate, per item the mean normalized SIB-S score was calculated per group. The mean normalized SIB-S score was calculated by taking the average (represented by bullets) and associated standard deviation (represented by lines) over all participants within one group.

DISCUSSION

This is the first study that examined the ability of the SIB-S to measure cognitive change in institutionalized persons with moderate to severe dementia. Mean total scores on the SIB-S declined over a 2-year period, and the rate of decline differed between dementia stages at baseline. The decline of SIB-S scores was more rapid for persons with moderately severe dementia than for persons with moderate or severe dementia suggesting a modest floor and slight ceiling effect for the SIB-S. Furthermore, we explored if the ability of participants to complete semantic, non-semantic, and episodic items of the SIB-S was related to their diagnosed dementia stage and to their overall cognitive functioning. For semantic memory items, the pattern of decline for different dementia stages was similar as for SIB-S total scores. However, for episodic and non-semantic memory items, only persons with moderately severe dementia declined faster than persons with severe dementia.

While we observed a decline for all three item categories for persons with moderately severe dementia, we only observed a decline in episodic and non-semantic memory items in persons with moderate dementia and a decline in semantic memory items in persons with severe dementia. In addition, our item rankings showed that persons with the lowest overall cognitive functioning had severe problems with items regarding episodic memory and praxis.

The decline over time found in persons with moderate to severe dementia suggests that mapping the course of cognitive functioning with the SIB-S is possible in this population. The faster decline in SIB-S scores for persons with moderately severe dementia than for person with moderate or severe dementia indicates, however, a modest floor and a slight ceiling effect. For some participants with moderate dementia, the questions of the SIB-S were probably too easy to adequately map their cognitive abilities. Indeed, Figure 1 shows that a larger proportion of the participants in the group with moderate dementia had relatively higher, more stable SIB-S scores than in the group with moderately severe dementia. This was probably due to the high number of semantic items in the SIB-S.

Studies have shown that mild AD patients were able to visually recognize and correctly name and match words and pictures (word-picture matching), a finding that suggests that aspects of semantic memory were still intact (Hirono et al., Reference Hirono, Mori, Ishii, Imamura, Tanimukai, Kazui and Sasaki2001; Hodges & Patterson, Reference Hodges and Patterson1995). Probably, aspects of semantic memory are also intact in the moderate stage of dementia, and these aspects deteriorate mainly in the moderately severe stage of dementia. It was indeed only semantic memory that showed an accelerated decline in SIB-S scores in the moderately severe dementia group in comparison to the moderate dementia group.

Additionally, for some participants with severe dementia, the questions of the SIB-S were probably too difficult. For persons with a baseline SIB-S score close to zero, it is not possible to decline much further in SIB-S scores. As can be seen in Figure 1, part of the participants in the group with severe dementia scored close to zero at every assessment. Furthermore, in persons with severe dementia, we only observed a decline in semantic memory. Our item ranking showed that two episodic memory items were impossible for persons with the lowest overall cognitive scores. It is possible that semantic memory is the only memory system that can decline when persons are in the severe stage of dementia. Therefore, the small non-semantic part of the SIB-S could also have contributed to the slower decline in SIB-S scores in the severe dementia group in comparison to the moderately severe dementia group. Additional studies with tests specifically developed to tap into different types of memory in advanced dementia may provide additional insight.

A similar pattern, a slower decline in cognitive functioning in the groups with the lowest and highest baseline-scores in comparison to the middle group, has also been found for other cognitive tests for dementia, including the original SIB (Schmitt et al., Reference Schmitt, Ashford, Ernesto, Saxton, Schneider and Clark1997), MMSE (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Edland, Clark, Galasko, Koss, Mohs and Heyman1993), and Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale (Stern et al., Reference Stern, Mohs, Davidson, Schmeidler, Silverman, Kramer-Ginsberg and Davis1994). Only the population included in the study of the original SIB was comparable to our population, while the other studies also contained participants with mild dementia and had fewer participants with very severe dementia. Therefore, it seems difficult to draw conclusions about what dementia stage shows the fastest cognitive decline.

It seems more likely that found differences in rate of cognitive decline between dementia stages are a test effect instead of a population effect. An alternative explanation is that in our study the group with moderately severe dementia at baseline had the largest score range on the SIB-S. The value of a correlation between variables can be larger when there is more variability among test scores (Goodwin & Leech, Reference Goodwin and Leech2006). Although the SIB-S thus seems most suitable in the moderately severe stage of dementia, our finding of decline in all examined dementia stages suggests that the SIB-S is a useful tool to track cognition in the entire range of moderate to severe dementia.

There was, however, considerable individual variability in total SIB-S scores over time, especially among participants with moderately severe dementia at baseline. A striking finding was the relatively high percentage (23.7%) of participants that had a SIB-S score that was higher at their last than at their first assessment. One of the explanations for the variability in SIB-S scores is that cognitive fluctuations (spontaneous alterations in cognition, attention, and arousal) are quite common among persons with dementia (Lee, Taylor, & Thomas, Reference Lee, Taylor and Thomas2012).

Second, we did not control for time of day participants were tested. If a participant was most lucid in the morning, but was tested at different times of the day across assessments, this could have affected the results. Furthermore, participants received treatment when necessary. This was, however, not registered nor controlled for. If a significant number of participants received treatment with a positive effect on cognitive functioning, this could have led to the relatively high percentage of participants that improved on the SIB-S during our study. Future studies into the course of cognitive functioning in dementia should control for time of day of assessments, and whenever possible, also for received treatments with an effect on cognitive functioning.

Next to cognitive fluctuations, the individual variability in total SIB-S scores could also be caused by the relatively high SEM of the SIB-S. For example, for the first assessment, the 68% confidence interval around a participant’s “true” SIB-S score was ±3.21 points. Thus individual variability on the SIB-S could partly be explained by fluctuations around a participant’s “true” score. One method to estimate the probability that an individual’s change in test scores is not due to chance is calculating the Reliable Change Index (RCI) for that particular test (Chelune, Naugle, Lüders, Sedlak & Awad, Reference Chelune, Naugle, Lüders, Sedlak and Awad1993). Unfortunately, to calculate the RCI the test–retest reliability coefficient is required, which was not available in our study. Future studies could calculate the RCI for the SIB-S to draw stronger conclusions about what represents clinically significant decline in dementia.

There were some other limitations. There were far more women than men in our study. Women tend to have a faster age-related cognitive decline and a higher risk of developing AD than men (Li & Singh, Reference Li and Singh2014). The imbalance in gender could have affected results. However, women live longer than men on average, and consequently there are more women than men with advanced dementia in institutions. Therefore, our sample is representative for institutionalized persons with dementia, but caution is warranted when generalizing our results to institutionalized men with dementia.

Another limitation was that only 70 of the 217 participants completed the last assessment. Main reasons for dropping out were death and terminal disease. Participants that dropped out were on average older than participants that completed the last assessment. Unfortunately, this was a consequence of the included population, persons with more advanced dementia. To still include all participants in our analyses, we analyzed our data with mixed models to handle missing data.

Dementia subtype was not specifically examined in this study. A specific dementia diagnosis was often inconclusive as neuroimaging data were not routinely available. Neuroimaging can help to distinguish different dementia subtypes more reliably (Health Quality Ontario, 2014). However, it is not common practice to use neuroimaging for the diagnosis of dementia subtypes in institutionalized persons. Furthermore, distinguishing between dementia subtypes becomes harder above the age of 80 as there is mainly mixed pathology in this group (Fotuhi, Hachinski, & Whitehouse, Reference Fotuhi, Hachinski and Whitehouse2009). So, we believe our results bear relevance to institutionalized populations with moderate to severe dementia. The course of cognitive functioning can be affected by dementia subtype; however, the effects of dementia subtype on the course of SIB-S scores warrant further research.

In conclusion, this is the first study that used the SIB-S to examine the course of cognitive functioning across different dementia stages. We found that it is feasible to measure cognitive decline in advanced dementia. Although certain SIB-S items regarding praxis and episodic memory will provide no additional information in persons with the lowest cognitive functioning, we recommend administering all SIB-S items in every dementia stage. It is a relatively short test to administer and there is a preconceived order in the SIB-S items. For persons in the moderate dementia stage who score near maximum on the SIB-S, additional tests less focused on semantic memory could be necessary to get a complete picture of their cognitive functioning at that moment. However, we recommend using the SIB-S from the moment a person reaches the moderate stage of dementia as the SIB-S is one of few available tests that can still track cognitive functioning even when a person eventually reaches the severe stage of dementia.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank Dr. Maarten G. Wolf for the help with ranking the SIB-S items in R and Dr. Rachel L. Fazio from the Research and Editing Consulting Program (RECP) for editing the English language. This work was supported by University of Nijmegen Department of Nursing Home Medicine; ZonMw; Foundation Johannes de Deo The Netherlands; and Foundation for the Promotion of Science in Nursing Home Care (SWBV). There were no potential conflicts of interest.

References

REFERENCES

Ahn, I.S., Kim, J.H., Saxton, J., & Kim, D.H. (2007). Reliability of a short form of the Severe Impairment Battery in Korean Alzheimer’s disease patients. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22, 682–287.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Black, S.E., Doody, R., Li, H., McRae, T., Jambor, K.M., Xu, Y., … Richardson, S. (2007). Donepezil preserves cognition and global function in patients with severe Alzheimer disease. Neurology, 69(5), 459469. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000266627.96040.5a Google Scholar
Boersma, F., Eefsting, J.A., Van Den Brink, W., & Van Tilburg, W. (1997). Care services for dementia patients: Predictors for service utilization. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 12(11), 11191126. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1166(199711)12:11<1119::AID-GPS702>3.0.CO;2-H 3.0.CO;2-H>Google Scholar
Chelune, G.J., Naugle, R.I., Lüders, H., Sedlak, J., & Awad, I.A. (1993). Individual change after epilepsy surgery: Practice effects and base-rate information. Neuropsychology, 7(1), 4152.Google Scholar
de Jonghe, J.F.M., Wetzels, R.B., Mulders, A., Zuidema, S.U., & Koopmans, R.T.C.M. (2009). Validity of the Severe Impairment Battery Short Version. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 80(9), 954959. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2008.163220 Google Scholar
Feldman, H., Gauthier, S., Hecker, J., Vellas, B., Xu, Y., Ieni, J.R., … Willmer, J. (2005). Efficacy and safety of donepezil in patients with more severe Alzheimer’s disease: A subgroup analysis from a randomized, placebo-controlled trial. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 20(6), 559569. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1325 Google Scholar
Folstein, M., Folstein, S., & McHugh, P. (1975). Mini-Mental State: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician, Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.Google Scholar
Fotuhi, M., Hachinski, V., & Whitehouse, P.J. (2009). Changing perspectives regarding late- life dementia. Nature Reviews. Neurology, 5(12), 649658. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrneurol.2009.175 Google Scholar
Galasko, D.R., Gould, R.L., Abramson, I.S., & Salmon, D.P. (2000). Measuring cognitive change in a cohort of patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Statistics in Medicine, 19(11–12), 14211432. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-0258(20000615/30)19:11/12<1421::AID-SIM434>3.0.CO;2-P 3.0.CO;2-P>Google Scholar
Gaugler, J.E., Yu, F., Krichbaum, K., & Wyman, J.F. (2009). Predictors of nursing home admission for persons with dementia. Medical Care, 47(2), 191198. https://doi.org/10.1097/MLR.0b013e31818457ce Google Scholar
Goodwin, L.D., & Leech, N.L. (2006). Understanding correlation: Factors that affect the size of r. Journal of Experimental Education, 74(3), 249266. http://doi.org/10.3200/JEXE.74.3.249–266 Google Scholar
Grand, J.H., Caspar, S., & Macdonald, S.W. (2011). Clinical features and multidisciplinary approaches to dementia care. Journal of Multidisciplinary Healthcare, 4, 125147. https://doi.org/10.2147/JMDH.S1777 3 Google Scholar
Harrell, L.E., Marson, D., Chatterjee, A., & Parrish, J.A. (2000). The severe mini-mental state examination: A new neuropsychologic instrument for the bedside assessment of severely impaired patients with Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 14(3), 168175. https://doi.org/10.1097/00002093-200007000-00008 Google Scholar
Health Quality Ontario. (2014). The appropriate use of neuroimaging in the diagnostic work- up of dementia: An evidence-based analysis. Ontario Health Technology Assessment Services, 14(1), 164.Google Scholar
Hirono, N., Mori, E., Ishii, K., Imamura, T., Tanimukai, S., Kazui, H., … Sasaki, M. (2001). Neuronal substrates for semantic memory: A positron emission tomography study in Alzheimer ’ s disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 12, 1521.Google Scholar
Hodges, J.R., & Patterson, K. (1995). Is semantic memory consistently impaired early in the course of Alzheimer’s disease? Neuroanatomical and diagnostic implications. Neuropsychologia, 33(4), 441459. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(94)00127-B Google Scholar
Koopmans, R.T.C.M., Pellegrom, M., & van der Geer, E.R. (2017). The Dutch move beyond the concept of nursing home physician specialists. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association, 18(9), 746749. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jamda.2017.05.013 Google Scholar
Lee, D.R., Taylor, J.P., & Thomas, A.J. (2012). Assessment of cognitive fluctuation in dementia: A systematic review of the literature. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 27(10), 989998. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.2823 Google Scholar
Li, R., & Singh, M. (2014). Sex differences in cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 35(3), 385403. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2014.01.002 Google Scholar
Luppa, M., Luck, T., Brähler, E., König, H.H., & Riedel-Heller, S.G. (2008). Prediction of institutionalisation in dementia: A systematic review. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 26(1), 6578. https://doi.org/10.1159/000144027 Google Scholar
McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., & Stadlan, E.M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology, 34, 939944.Google Scholar
Morris, J.N., Fries, B.E., & Morris, S.A. (1999). Scaling ADLs Within the MDS. The Journals of Gerontology: Series A, 54(11), M546M553.Google Scholar
Morris, J., Edland, S., Clark, C., Galasko, D., Koss, E., Mohs, R., … Heyman, A. (1993). The consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD). Part IV. Rates of cognitive change in the longitudinal assessment of probable Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 43, 24572465.Google Scholar
Panisset, M., Roudier, M., Saxton, J., & Boiler, F. (1994). Severe impairment battery: A neuropsychological test for severely demented patients. Archives of Neurology, 51(1), 4145.Google Scholar
Peavy, G.M., Salmon, D.P., Rice, V.A., Samuel, W., Taylor, K.I., Ernesto, C., … Thal, L. (1996). Neuropsychological assessment of severely demented elderly: The severe cognitive impairment profile. Archives of Neurology, 53, 367372.Google Scholar
Perry, R.J., Watson, P., & Hodges, J.R. (2000). The nature and staging of attention dysfunction in early (minimal and mild) Alzheimer’s disease: Relationship to episodic and semantic memory impairment. Neuropsychologia, 38(3), 252271. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00079-2 Google Scholar
Reisberg, B., Ferris, S.H., De Leon, M.J., & Crook, T. (1982). Global Deterioration Scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 11361139.Google Scholar
Roman, G.C., Tatemichi, T.K., Erkinjuntti, T., Cummings, J.L., Masdeu, J.C., Garcia, J.H., … Scheinberg, P. (1993). Vascular dementia: Diagnostic criteria for research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop. Neurology, 43, 250260.Google Scholar
R Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org/ Google Scholar
Saxton, J., Kastango, K.B., Hugonot-Diener, L., Boller, F., Verny, M., Sarles, C.E., … DeKosky, S.T. (2005). Development of a short form of the Severe Impairment Battery. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 13, 9991005.Google Scholar
Saxton, J., McGonigle-Gibson, K.L., Swihart, A.A., Miller, V.J., & Boller, F. (1990). Assessment of the severely impaired patient: Description and validation of a new neuropsychological test battery. Psychological Assessment, 2(3), 298303.Google Scholar
Schmitt, F.A., Ashford, W., Ernesto, C., Saxton, J. Schneider, L.S., Clark, C.M., … the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study (1997). The Severe Impairment Battery: Concurrent validity and the assessment of longitudinal change in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 11(2), S51S56.Google Scholar
Stern, R., Mohs, R., Davidson, M., Schmeidler, J., Silverman, J., Kramer-Ginsberg, E., … Davis, K. (1994). A longitudinal study of Alzheimer’s disease: Measurement, rate, and predictors of cognitive deterioration. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 390396.Google Scholar
Thomas, R.G., Albert, M., Petersen, R.C., & Aisen, P.S. (2016). Longitudinal decline in mild-to-moderate Alzheimer’s disease: Analyses of placebo data from clinical trials. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 12(5), 598603. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2016.01.002 Google Scholar
Wetzels, R.B., Zuidema, S.U., de Jonghe, J.F.M., Verhey, F.R.J., & Koopmans, R.T.C.M. (2010). Course of neuropsychiatric symptoms in residents with dementia in nursing homes over 2-year period. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18(12), 10541065. https://doi.org/10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181f60fa1 Google Scholar
Wild, K.V., & Kaye, J.A. (1998). The rate of progression of Alzheimer’s disease in the later stages: Evidence from the Severe Impairment Battery. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(5), 512516. http://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617798005104 Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 SIB-S items per subscale

Figure 1

Table 2 Subtypes of dementia for the whole sample

Figure 2

Table 3 Baseline characteristics of the sample

Figure 3

Table 4 Growth curve models for SIB-S total and domain scores

Figure 4

Table 5 Mean SIB-S scores on successive assessments for the whole sample

Figure 5

Fig. 1 Course of SIB-S scores over time for persons with moderate to severe dementia according to dementia stage (without a correction for age). The bold lines represent the mean course of SIB-S scores per GDS-score and the thin lines represent the course of SIB-S scores for each individual participant.

Figure 6

Fig. 2 Ranking of the items by how many participants could still successfully complete the item. As a measure for the items completion rate, per item the mean normalized SIB-S score was calculated. The average (represented by bullets) and associated standard deviation (represented by lines) were taken over all participants and all tests.

Figure 7

Fig. 3 Ranking of the items by how many participants could still successfully complete the item per group (classified by total SIB-S score). The ranking was based on the whole population. As a measure for the item’s completion rate, per item the mean normalized SIB-S score was calculated per group. The mean normalized SIB-S score was calculated by taking the average (represented by bullets) and associated standard deviation (represented by lines) over all participants within one group.