Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T04:20:52.946Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Impact of age and apolipoprotein E ε4 status on regional white matter hyperintensity volume and cognition in healthy aging

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 March 2024

Emily J. Van Etten
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
Pradyumna K. Bharadwaj
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
Matthew D. Grilli
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA Department of Neurology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
David A. Raichlen
Affiliation:
Human and Evolutionary Biology Section, Department of Biological Sciences, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Georg A. Hishaw
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
Matthew J. Huentelman
Affiliation:
Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA Neurogenomics Division, The Translational Genomics Research Institute (TGen), Phoenix, AZ, USA
Theodore P. Trouard
Affiliation:
Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium, Phoenix, AZ, USA Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
Gene E. Alexander*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium, Phoenix, AZ, USA Department of Psychiatry, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA Neuroscience Graduate Interdisciplinary Program, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA Physiological Sciences Graduate Interdisciplinary Program, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
*
Corresponding author: Gene E. Alexander; Email: gene.alexander@arizona.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

White matter hyperintensity (WMH) volume is a neuroimaging marker of lesion load related to small vessel disease that has been associated with cognitive aging and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) risk.

Method:

The present study sought to examine whether regional WMH volume mediates the relationship between APOE ε4 status, a strong genetic risk factor for AD, and cognition and if this association is moderated by age group differences within a sample of 187 healthy older adults (APOE ε4 status [carrier/non-carrier] = 56/131).

Results:

After we controlled for sex, education, and vascular risk factors, ANCOVA analyses revealed significant age group by APOE ε4 status interactions for right parietal and left temporal WMH volumes. Within the young-old group (50-69 years), ε4 carriers had greater right parietal and left temporal WMH volumes than non-carriers. However, in the old-old group (70-89 years), right parietal and left temporal WMH volumes were comparable across APOE ε4 groups. Further, within ε4 non-carriers, old-old adults had greater right parietal and left temporal WMH volumes than young-old adults, but there were no significant differences across age groups in ε4 carriers. Follow-up moderated mediation analyses revealed that, in the young-old, but not the old-old group, there were significant indirect effects of ε4 status on memory and executive functions through left temporal WMH volume.

Conclusions:

These findings suggest that, among healthy young-old adults, increased left temporal WMH volume, in the context of the ε4 allele, may represent an early marker of cognitive aging with the potential to lead to greater risk for AD.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Neuropsychological Society

Introduction

White matter hyperintensity (WMH) volume is a neuroimaging marker of white matter lesion load that is thought to reflect chronic ischemia related to cerebral small vessel disease (Biesbroek et al., Reference Biesbroek, Weaver and Biessels2017; Prins & Scheltens, Reference Prins and Scheltens2015). Over the past several years, research has highlighted the important role of WMH volume in Alzheimer’s disease (AD), with previous studies observing WMH’s may influence both the development and progression of AD (Birdsill et al., Reference Birdsill, Koscik, Jonaitis, Johnson, Okonkwo, Hermann, LaRue, Sager and Bendlin2014; Brickman, Reference Brickman2013; Brickman et al., Reference Brickman, Schupf, Manly, Stern, Luchsinger, Provenzano, Narkhede, Razlighi, Collins-Praino, Artero, Akbaraly, Ritchie, Mayeux and Portet2014, Reference Brickman, Zahodne, Guzman, Narkhede, Meier, Griffith, Provenzano, Schupf, Manly, Stern, Luchsinger and Mayeux2015). Even within healthy older adults, elevated total WMH volume has been related to poorer cognition, particularly in age-sensitive functions, including memory, executive functions, and processing speed (Alexander et al., Reference Alexander, Ryan, Bowers, Foster, Bizon, Geldmacher and Glisky2012b; Glisky, Reference Glisky2007, Park & Reuter-Lorenz, Reference Park and Reuter-Lorenz2009; Salthouse, Reference Salthouse1992). Fewer studies, however, have considered how the regional distribution of WMH volume may differentially affect these cognitive functions. Additionally, the extant findings with regional WMH volumes are mixed, and separate studies have observed significant associations between different cognitive functions and WMH volume within various cerebral lobes (frontal, parietal, temporal, and/or occipital; Brugulat-Serrat et al., Reference Brugulat-Serrat, Salvadó, Sudre, Grau-Rivera, Suárez-Calvet, Falcon, Sánchez-Benavides, Gramunt, Fauria, Cardoso, Barkhof, Molinuevo and Gispert2020; Garnier-Crussard et al., Reference Garnier-Crussard, Bougacha, Wirth, André, Delarue, Landeau, Mézenge, Kuhn, Gonneaud, Chocat, Quillard, Ferrand-Devouge, de La Sayette, Vivien, Krolak-Salmon and Chételat2020; Gunning-Dixon & Raz, Reference Gunning-Dixon and Raz2003; Lampe et al., Reference Lampe, Kharabian-Masouleh, Kynast, Arelin, Steele, Löffler, Witte, Schroeter, Villringer and Bazin2019; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Salat, Jeng, McCreary, Fischl, Schmahmann, Dickerson, Viswanathan, Albert, Blacker and Greenberg2011). More research is needed to further elucidate how regional WMH volumes impact cognition in healthy older adults, which may contribute to greater cognitive aging and AD risk.

Although WMH volume is often related to increased vascular risk factors, WMH’s are also commonly observed in older adults without significant vascular conditions (Wardlaw et al., Reference Wardlaw, Allerhand, Doubal, Hernandez, Morris, Gow and Deary2014) suggesting other factors may contribute to their aggregation in aging. APOE ε4 carrier status is a strong genetic risk factor for late-onset AD (Bertram et al., Reference Bertram, Lill and Tanzi2010; Corder et al., Reference Corder, Saunders, Strittmatter, Schmechel, Gaskell, Small, Roses, Haines and Pericak-Vance1993) that is also associated with vascular disease mechanisms (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Kanekiyo, Xu and Bu2013; Raichlen & Alexander, Reference Raichlen and Alexander2014), including greater total (Godin et al., Reference Godin, Tzourio, Maillard, Alpérovitch, Mazoyer and Dufouil2009; Rojas et al., Reference Rojas, Brugulat-Serrat, Bargallo, Minguillon, Tucholka, Falcon and Gispert2018; Schilling et al., Reference Schilling, DeStefano, Sachdev, Choi, Mather, DeCarli, Wen, Høgh, Raz, Au, Beiser, Wolf, Romero, Zhu, Lunetta, Farrer, Dufouil, Kuller, Mazoyer, Seshadri, Tzourio and Debette2013) and regional WMH volumes, particularly in parietal, temporal, and occipital lobes (Brickman et al., Reference Brickman, Schupf, Manly, Stern, Luchsinger, Provenzano, Narkhede, Razlighi, Collins-Praino, Artero, Akbaraly, Ritchie, Mayeux and Portet2014). Additionally, previous studies have suggested the influence of regional WMH volume on aging and AD risk may, in part, depend on APOE ε4 status. Brickman et al. (Reference Brickman, Schupf, Manly, Stern, Luchsinger, Provenzano, Narkhede, Razlighi, Collins-Praino, Artero, Akbaraly, Ritchie, Mayeux and Portet2014) observed that ε4 carriers with increased parietal WMH volume were at greater risk of AD than ε4 carriers with low levels of WMH volume in parietal regions and ε4 non-carriers with high or low parietal WMH volumes. Moreover, a previous study from our lab (Van Etten et al., Reference Van Etten, Bharadwaj, Hishaw, Huentelman, Trouard, Grilli and Alexander2021) found that hippocampal volume, a neuroimaging marker associated with preclinical AD, may be vulnerable to the impact of elevated temporal WMH volumes in ε4 carriers, but not non-carriers, in middle-aged to older adults. Thus, the accumulation of regional WMH volume may have greater detrimental effects on brain aging and the risk for AD in APOE ε4 carriers than non-carriers.

Although the APOE ε4 genotype is associated with subsequent development of dementia, its association with cognition before the onset of cognitive impairment and/or AD has been variable (Small et al., Reference Small, Rosnick, Fratiglioni and Bäckman2004; Wisdom et al., Reference Wisdom, Callahan and Hawkins2011). Some studies have not observed a significant relationship between ε4 status and cognition (Driscoll et al., Reference Driscoll, McDaniel and Guynn2005; O'Donoghue et al., Reference O’Donoghue, Murphy, Zamboni, Nobre and Mackay2018), whereas others have found that, compared to non-carriers, ε4 carriers demonstrate poorer performance across multiple cognitive domains in healthy older adults (Luck et al., Reference Luck, Then, Luppa, Schroeter, Arélin, Burkhardt, Thiery, Löffler, Villringer and Riedel-Heller2015; O’Hara et al., Reference O’Hara, Sommer, Way, Kraemer, Taylor and Murphy2008; Wetter et al., Reference Wetter, Delis, Houston, Jacobson, Lansing, Cobell and Bondi2005). Notably, memory decline, a hallmark feature of AD, may be especially affected by APOE genotype (Bondi et al., Reference Bondi, Salmon, Monsch, Galasko, Butters, Klauber, Thal and Saitoh1995; Caselli et al., Reference Caselli, Reiman, Osborne, Hentz, Baxter, Hernandez and Alexander2004; Caselli et al., Reference Caselli, Dueck, Osborne, Sabbagh, Connor, Ahern, Baxter, Rapcsak, Shi, Woodruff, Locke, Snyder, Alexander, Rademakers and Reiman2009; Jacobson et al., Reference Jacobson, Delis, Lansing, Houston, Olsen, Wetter, Bondi and Salmon2005). These cognitive differences may reflect the accumulation of brain alterations that occur in ε4 carriers prior to the onset of overt clinical symptoms, including the aggregation of WMH volume (Tondelli et al., Reference Tondelli, Wilcock, Nichelli, De Jager, Jenkinson and Zamboni2012).

Age differences may be an important factor to consider in relation to the influence of APOE ε4 status on dementia risk. Previous findings have indicated that the ε4 allele has the greatest observable impact on risk of AD in middle-aged to young-older adults, with diminishing effects in later age groups (Bonham et al., Reference Bonham, Geier, Fan, Leong, Besser, Kukull, Kornak, Andreassen, Schellenberg, Rosen, Dillon, Hess, Miller, Dale, Desikan and Yokoyama2016; Farrer et al., Reference Farrer, Cupples, Haines, Hyman, Kukull, Mayeux and Van Duijn1997; Valerio et al., Reference Valerio, Raventos, Schmeidler, Beeri, Villalobos, Bolaños-Palmieri and Silverman2014), and a meta-analysis suggested that this may occur at an age of 70 years (Farrer et al., Reference Farrer, Cupples, Haines, Hyman, Kukull, Mayeux and Van Duijn1997). However, fewer studies have investigated if this pattern is observed when examining brain and cognitive differences associated with the APOE ε4 allele. In individuals with AD and MCI, previous findings indicate that, in young-old (YO) adults (55–75; 60–75 years), ε4 carriers had reductions in hippocampal volume, greater memory decline, and poorer executive functions and processing speed compared to non-carriers (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Fennema‐Notestine, Holland, McEvoy, Stricker, Salmon, Dale and Bondi2014; Tang et al., Reference Tang, Holland, Dale and Miller2015). In contrast, there were no significant gray matter or cognitive differences between ε4 groups in the old-old (OO) adults (80–92 years; Chang et al., Reference Chang, Fennema‐Notestine, Holland, McEvoy, Stricker, Salmon, Dale and Bondi2014; Tang et al., Reference Tang, Holland, Dale and Miller2015). Whether and how age and APOE ε4 status interact to affect cognitive and brain aging, including regional WMH volumes in cognitively healthy adults, remains unclear.

The present study investigated the interactive effects of age group and APOE ε4 status on the regional lobar distribution of WMH volumes in a cohort of healthy adults. Further, we examined whether differences in regional WMH volume mediate the relationship between ε4 status and cognition. Similar to findings examining the effects of the ε4 allele on regional WMH volumes (Brickman et al., Reference Brickman, Schupf, Manly, Stern, Luchsinger, Provenzano, Narkhede, Razlighi, Collins-Praino, Artero, Akbaraly, Ritchie, Mayeux and Portet2014), we hypothesized that, compared to YO ε4 non-carriers, YO ε4 carriers would have significantly greater regional WMH volumes, particularly within parietal, temporal, and occipital lobes, and this difference between APOE ε4 carriers and non-carriers would be diminished within the OO group. As previous studies examining associations between regional WMH volumes and cognitive functions are mixed, we sought to test the general hypothesis that these age group by APOE ε4 carrier differences in regional WMH volumes would be associated with cognition. In this case, we hypothesized that within the YO group, ε4 carriers would display poorer cognitive performance than non-carriers, and this difference between APOE ε4 status would be attenuated within the OO group.

Method

Participants

Participants included 187 individuals aged 50–89 years that were drawn from a cohort of 210 community-dwelling healthy adults, as part of a study on cognitive aging. The sample was largely white non-Hispanic (89.8%), with an average education of 15.98 years (SD = 2.56). Seven participants were excluded due to missing data. Outliers who were ±3.5 or more SD’s away from the mean in one or more lobes of WMH volumes (n = 16; Van Etten et al., Reference Van Etten, Bharadwaj, Hishaw, Huentelman, Trouard, Grilli and Alexander2021) were removed from the analyses to enhance normality, as their values remained skewed after a log transformation and adjustment for total intracranial volume (TIV). Participants were split into groups by median age of our original cohort (Franchetti et al., Reference Franchetti, Bharadwaj, Nguyen, Van Etten, Klimentidis, Hishaw, Trouard, Raichlen and Alexander2020) into YO (ages = 50–69 years; n = 90) and OO (ages = 70–89 years; n = 97) groups.

The data for the current study was drawn from a larger cohort of the Brain Aging and Memory Study at the University of Arizona, and participants were recruited from the Tucson-metro area community through local newspaper advertisements. The larger study was aimed at investigating how vascular health, aerobic fitness, and APOE genotype influence regional brain structural changes, and participants were asked to complete a blood draw, brain MRI scans, an aerobic fitness test, health status questionnaires, and a neuropsychological test battery. Participants were screened for exclusion if they were not able to take part in these aspects of the study (more details of the larger study are provided in Supplemental materials and are also described in Nguyen et al., Reference Nguyen, Haws, Fitzhugh, Torre, Hishaw and Alexander2016; Franchetti et al., Reference Franchetti, Bharadwaj, Nguyen, Van Etten, Klimentidis, Hishaw, Trouard, Raichlen and Alexander2020; and Van Etten et al., Reference Van Etten, Bharadwaj, Hishaw, Huentelman, Trouard, Grilli and Alexander2021). To exclude individuals with significant neurological, medical, or psychiatric disorders before enrollment in the study, participants underwent a comprehensive medical screen, which included a physical and neurological examination performed by a neurologist specialized in aging. Participants completed the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D; Hamilton, Reference Hamilton1960) and the Mini Mental Status Exam (MMSE; Folstein et al., Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975) and were excluded from the study if they had a HAM-D score greater than 9 or MMSE score less than 26. The research was completed in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration, all participants provided written consent, and procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Arizona.

APOE genotyping

As described previously (Van Etten et al., Reference Van Etten, Bharadwaj, Hishaw, Huentelman, Trouard, Grilli and Alexander2021), APOE genotype was determined with extracted DNA assayed via restriction fragment length polymorphism according to published methods (Addya et al., Reference Addya, Wang and Leonard1997). There were 56 APOE ε4 carriers (homozygous: n = 7, heterozygous: n = 49) and 131 APOE ε4 non-carriers in our sample.

Cognitive measures

Measures of processing speed, executive functions, and memory known to be sensitive to aging (Alexander et al., Reference Alexander, Ryan, Bowers, Foster, Bizon, Geldmacher and Glisky2012b; Glisky, Reference Glisky2007, Park & Reuter-Lorenz, Reference Park and Reuter-Lorenz2009; Salthouse, Reference Salthouse1992) were selected from a larger battery of neuropsychological tests. Processing speed was measured with the coding subtest from the WAIS-IV (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2008) and the Trail Making Test part A (TMT A; Reitan, Reference Reitan1956). Aspects of executive functions were evaluated with the Trail Making Test part B (TMT B; Reitan, Reference Reitan1956) and Stroop Color-Word Interference (Golden & Freshwater, 1978). Finally, the 12-item, 12-trial Buschke Selective Reminding Test (BSRT) total sum recall and consistent long-term retrieval (CLTR; Buschke, Reference Buschke1973) were used to measure memory.

We additionally included measures of language naming and fluency, as aspects of language abilities tend to decline early in the AD process and have been associated with temporal lobe atrophy (Monsch et al., Reference Monsch, Bondi, Butters, Salmon, Katzman and Thal1992). These measures included the total score from the Boston Naming Test (BNT; Kaplan et al., Reference Kaplan, Goodglass and Weintraub1983) and category fluency (animals; Rosen, Reference Rosen1980).

Health measures

Participant height and weight were measured and utilized to calculate body mass index (BMI). Cholesterol, hypertension, and statin medication status and number of years smoking were recorded from participants self-reported history.

Magnetic resonance imaging

The MRI scans were acquired on a 3T GE Signa scanner (HD Signa Excite, General Electric, Milwaukee, WI), including volumetric T1-weighted Spoiled Gradient Echo (SPGR; slice thickness = 1.0 mm, TR = 5.3 ms, TE = 2.0 ms, TI = 500 ms) and T2 Fluid-Attenuation Inversion Recovery (FLAIR) scans (slice thickness = 2.6 mm, TR = 11000 ms, TE = 120 ms, TI = 2250 ms).

Image processing

T1 and T2 FLAIR scans were used to compute total WMH volume with the lesion segmentation toolbox (LST; Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Gaser, Arsic, Buck, Förschler, Berthele, Hoshi, Ilg, Schmid, Zimmer, Hemmer and Mühlau2012) for SPM12. As described in our prior work (Franchetti et al., Reference Franchetti, Bharadwaj, Nguyen, Van Etten, Klimentidis, Hishaw, Trouard, Raichlen and Alexander2020; Van Etten et al., Reference Van Etten, Bharadwaj, Hishaw, Huentelman, Trouard, Grilli and Alexander2021), in a subset of 35 participants, the LST’s lesion growth algorithm (LGA) accuracy was assessed across an array of kappa thresholds (0.05–1.00) using manually segmented reference WMH maps produced with ITK-SNAP (www.itksnap.org; Yushkevich et al., Reference Yushkevich, Piven, Hazlett, Smith, Ho, Gee and Gerig2006). Global WMH maps generated at the 0.35 kappa threshold produced the highest spatial and volumetric correspondence with the reference WMH maps. Then, LGA lesion probability maps were generated for all participants at this optimal kappa threshold (0.35) and were visually inspected for accuracy before computing total WMH volumes.

Our approach for processing regional WMH volumes has been detailed previously (Franchetti et al., Reference Franchetti, Bharadwaj, Nguyen, Van Etten, Klimentidis, Hishaw, Trouard, Raichlen and Alexander2020; Van Etten et al., Reference Van Etten, Bharadwaj, Hishaw, Huentelman, Trouard, Grilli and Alexander2021). Briefly, the MNI152 template was initially processed using FreeSurfer v5.3. Cortical labels for the four major brain lobes for each cerebral hemisphere were generated by combining the regional labels from the Desikan-Killiany atlas (Desikan et al., Reference Desikan, Ségonne, Fischl, Quinn, Dickerson, Blacker, Buckner, Dale, Maguire, Hyman, Albert and Killiany2006) per FreeSurfer’s standard lobar schema (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/CorticalParcellation) and propagated into the white matter to generate the lobar template. The Advanced Normalization Tools’ Greedy SyN algorithm (ANTs; Avants et al., Reference Avants, Tustison, Song, Cook, Klein and Gee2011) was used to non-linearly register the lobar template to each participant’s T1 scan to generate T1 native space lobar ROIs. These ROIs were used with the LST-generated lesion probability maps to extract the regional WMH volumes for each participant. The eight hemispheric lobar WMH volumes were then log-transformed and residualized using linear regressions to adjust for differences in TIV, which was computed in native brain space (Alexander et al., Reference Alexander, Bergfield, Chen, Reiman, Hanson, Lin, Bandy, Caselli and Moeller2012a).

Statistical analyses

Demographic characteristic differences between APOE ε4 carriers and non-carriers were evaluated using independent sample t-tests or chi-square tests, where appropriate. Separate two-way analysis of covariance’s (ANCOVA’s) were used to test the effects of age group (YO vs OO), APOE ε4 status (carriers vs non-carriers), and their interaction on regional WMH volumes. Covariates included sex, education, BMI, years smoking, and hypertension, cholesterol, and statin medication status. False discovery rate (FDR) corrections (Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995), which have been recommended for use in health studies (Glickman et al., Reference Glickman, Rao and Schultz2014), were used to adjust for multiple comparisons. All ANCOVA models with significant two-way interactions after FDR corrections were followed by FDR-corrected simple effect analyses.

Moderated mediation analyses were performed using the PROCESS macro for SPSS (v3.5; Hayes, 2017) to examine whether observed significant differences in regional WMH volumes (related to APOE ε4 status and age group interactive effects) were, in turn, associated with cognitive performance differences. Since moderated mediation models in PROCESS do not allow for corrections of multiple comparisons, we initially performed linear regressions to identify candidates for the full moderated mediation analyses. Each regression model included age group, APOE ε4 status, and their interaction entered first, followed by those regional WMH volumes that had significant interactive effects from the ANCOVA analyses, and the dependent variables were cognitive measures, which were adjusted for multiple comparisons using FDR corrections.

Cognitive measures that were significantly associated with regional WMH volumes in our linear regression analyses were then followed by the full moderated mediation analyses to examine whether differences in regional WMH volumes mediated the relationship between APOE ε4 status and cognition that was moderated by age group. All moderated mediation analyses were performed using the PROCESS macro for SPSS (v3.5; Hayes, 2017), using non-parametric percentile bootstrap resampling with 10,000 iterations to produce 95% percentile confidence intervals, which indicate significance when they do not include zero. Separate moderated mediation models were performed to test the mediation of the relation between APOE ε4 status (independent variable [x]) and cognition (dependent variable [y]) by regional WMH volume (mediator [m]) with age group (YO = 50–69 years, OO = 70–89 years) as the moderator (w). Sex, education, BMI, years smoking, and hypertension, cholesterol, and statin medication status were included as covariates. Given the primary motivation of the present study was to understand whether and how the interactive effects of APOE ε4 status and age group relate to regional WMH volumes and associated differences in cognition, the models tested the moderation of age group on the relation between ε4 status and regional WMH volume, as well as between ε4 status and cognitive performance (see Figure 1). Each analysis tested the direct and indirect effects of the relations between APOE ε4 status, regional WMH volume, and cognition and how these associations differ between YO and OO adults within one model (Hayes, 2017).

Figure 1. Illustration of the hypothesized moderated mediation model of the relationship between APOE ε4 status and cognition mediated by regional white matter hyperintensity volume and moderated by age group (young-old and old-old).

Results

Demographics

As shown in Table 1, the APOE ε4 carrier and non-carrier groups significantly differed in the distribution of cholesterol status and statin medication use but did not differ in any other demographic or clinical characteristic. Although there were no significant differences in the distribution of ε4 status between age groups, the OO group had a numerically higher percentage of ε4 carriers (31.96%) than the YO group (27.78%).

Table 1. Table of demographic characteristics

Note: M (SD) = Mean (standard deviation, YO/OO = young-old/old-old, F/M = female/male, y/n = yes/no.

ANCOVA analyses

After we controlled for all covariates, significant main effects of age group were observed for left (F(1,185) = 24.14, FDRp = 8.0E-6, η p 2 = .121) and right (F(1,185) = 24.82, FDRp = 8.0E-6, η p 2 = .124) frontal, left (F(1,185) = 22.06, FDRp = 8.0E-6, η p 2 = .111) and right (F(1,185) = 20.62, FDRp = 1.3E-5, η p 2 = .105) parietal, and left (F(1,185) = 23.23, FDRp = 8.0E-6, η p 2 = .117) and right (F(1,185) = 17.53, FDRp = 8.0E-6, η p 2 = .091) temporal WMH volumes, with the OO group having greater WMH volumes than the YO group across all regions. There were no significant main effects of age group for left (F(1,185) = 2.55, FDRp = .128, η p 2 = .014) or right (F(1,185) = 1.69, FDRp = .195, η p 2 = .010) occipital WMH volumes. There were no significant main effects of APOE ε4 status observed in left (F(1,185) = .113, FDRp = .998, η p 2 = .001) or right (F(1,185) = 2.40, FDRp = .984, η p 2 = .013) frontal, left (F(1,185) = .000, FDRp = .998, η p 2 = 2.2E-8), or right (F(1,185) = .382, FDRp = .998, η p 2 = .002) parietal, left (F(1,185) = 1.08, FDRp = .998, η p 2 = .006) or right (F(1,185) = .259, FDRp = .998, η p 2 = .001) temporal, or left (F(1,185) = .040, FDRp = .998, η p 2 = 2.3E-4) or right (F(1,185) = .004, FDRp = .998, η p 2 = 2.2E-5) occipital WMH volumes with all covariates included (see Table 2).

Table 2. Effects of APOE ε4 status, age group, and their interaction on regional white matter hyperintensity volumes

Note: Means and standard deviation for each group are presented. White matter hyperintensity volumes are log-transformed and corrected for total intracranial volume. **p < .001, *p < .01. Significant effects are bolded after FDR correction. Abbreviations: WMH = white matter hyperintensity; FDR = false discovery rate.

With all covariates included, significant interactive effects for age group and APOE ε4 status were observed for left temporal (F(1,185) = 9.25, FDRp = .022, η p 2 = .050) and right parietal (F(1,185) = 7.61, FDRp = .026, η p 2 = .041) WMH volumes (see Figures 2 and 3). There were no significant interactive effects of age group and ε4 status for left (F(1,185) = 3.64, FDRp = .116, η p 2 = .020) or right (F(1,185) = 5.36, FDRp = .058, η p 2 =.030) frontal, left (F(1,185) = 1.19, FDRp = .278, η p 2 = .007) or right (F(1,185) = 3.27, FDRp = .116, η p 2 = .018) occipital, left parietal (F(1,185) = 2.58, FDRp = .147, η p 2 = .014), or right temporal (F(1,185) = 1.59, FDRp = .240, η p 2 = .009) WMH volumes. Follow-up simple effect analyses of the significant interactions revealed that, within the YO group, ε4 carriers had greater right parietal (FDRp = .046) and left temporal (FDRp = .012) WMH volumes than non-carriers. However, in the OO group, right parietal (FDRp = .165) and left temporal (FDRp = .203) WMH volumes were comparable across ε4 status. Within ε4 non-carriers, OO adults had greater right parietal (FDRp = 8.20E-8) and left temporal (FDRp = 4.64E-11) WMH volumes than YO adults, but there were no significant differences across age groups in ε4 carriers for right parietal (FDRp = .235) or left temporal (FDRp = .233).

Figure 2. The mean and standard error of left temporal WMH volume for age group and APOE ε4 status. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) showed that, after controlling for sex, education, BMI, years smoking, hypertension, cholesterol, and statin medication status, there was a significant main effect for age group (FDRp = 8.0E-6); there was no main effect for APOE ε4 status (FDRp = .998); and there was a significant age group by APOE ε4 status interaction (FDRp = .022). *Simple effects analyses revealed young-old APOE ε4 carriers had significantly greater left temporal (FDRp = .012) WMH volumes than young-old APOE ε4 non-carriers; old-old APOE ε4 non-carriers had significantly greater left temporal (FDRp = 4.64E-11) WMH volumes young-old APOE ε4 non-carriers; there were no significant differences across age groups within ε4 carriers; there were no significant differences across ε4 groups within the old-old. Blue bars represent APOE ε4 non-carriers and red bars represent APOE ε4 carriers. Note. WMH = white matter hyperintensity; YO = young-old; OO = old-old; APOE = apolipoprotein E.

Figure 3. The mean and standard error of right parietal WMH volume for age group and APOE ε4 status. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) showed that, after controlling for sex, education, BMI, years smoking, hypertension, cholesterol, and statin medication status, there was a significant main effect for age group (FDRp = 1.3E-5); there was no main effect for APOE ε4 status (FDRp = .998); and there was a significant age group by APOE ε4 status interaction (FDRp = .026). *Simple effects analyses revealed young-old APOE ε4 carriers had significantly greater right parietal WMH volumes (FDRp = .046) than young-old APOE ε4 non-carriers; old-old APOE ε4 non-carriers had significantly greater right parietal WMH volumes (FDRp = 8.20E-8) young-old APOE ε4 non-carriers; there were no significant differences across age groups within ε4 carriers; there were no significant differences across ε4 groups within the old-old. Blue bars represent APOE ε4 non-carriers and red bars represent APOE ε4 carriers. Note. WMH = white matter hyperintensity; YO = young-old; OO = old-old; APOE = apolipoprotein E.

Linear regression analyses

After we controlled for age group, APOE ε4 status, and their interactive effects, linear regressions revealed significant relationships between left temporal WMH volume and TMT A (β = 2.19, FDRp = .036, R2 change = .023), TMT B (β = 7.92, FDRp = .016, R2 change = .038), Stroop Color-Word Interference (β = −2.29, FDRp = .010, R2 change = .045), BSRT sum recall (β = −3.90, FDRp = .026, R2 change = .027), and BSRT CLTR (β = −7.01, FDRp = .026, R2 change = 0.026), but not the WAIS-IV coding subtest (β = −1.32, FDRp = .232, R2 change = .006). There were no significant relationships between right parietal WMH volume and any cognitive measure before or after an FDR correction (p’s > .05).

Linear regressions with language measures revealed no significant relationships between left temporal WMH volume and Boston Naming Test (β = −.202, FDRp = .487, R2 change = .002) or category fluency (β = −.778, FDRp = .337, R2 change = .015). There were no significant relationships between right parietal WMH volume and Boston Naming Test (β = −.323, FDRp = .337, R2 change = .007) or category fluency (β = −.504, FDRp = .337, R2 change = .007).

Moderated mediation models

Given we only included cognitive measures that were significantly related to regional WMH volumes in the regression analyses, we limited our moderated mediation models to those showing significant associations with regional WMH volumes, which included TMT A, TMT B, Stroop Color-Word Interference, BSRT sum recall, and BSRT CLTR. Moderated mediation models, after controlling for all covariates, revealed that the mediation of the relationship between APOE ε4 status and TMT B (−6.82(SE = 3.57), 95%CI, [−14.88, −1.14]; see Figure 4A), Stroop Color-Word Interference (1.97(SE = .967), 95%CI, [.444, 4.12]; see Figure 4B), BSRT sum recall (3.19(SE = 1.75), 95%CI, [.418, 7.11]; see Figure 4C), and BSRT CLTR (6.01(SE = 3.32), 95%CI, [.796, 13.71]; see Figure 4D) by left temporal WMH volume were each moderated by age group. YO adults showed significant indirect effects of ε4 status on TMT B (4.60(SE = 2.67), 95%CI, [.545, 10.81]), Stroop Color-Word Interference (−1.33(SE = .707), 95%CI, [−2.96, −.229]), BSRT sum recall (−2.15(SE = 1.24), 95%CI, [−4.98, −.216]), and BSRT CLTR (−4.05(SE = 2.36), 95%CI, [−9.56, −.387]) through left temporal WMH volumes. However, in the OO group, left temporal WMH volume did not significantly mediate the relation between APOE ε4 status and TMT B (−2.22(SE = 1.75), 95%CI, [−6.07, .751]), Stroop Color-Word Interference (.643(SE = .501), 95%CI, [−.212, 1.77]), BSRT sum recall (1.04(SE = .899), 95%CI, [−.325, 3.14]), and BSRT CLTR(1.96 (SE = 1.71), 95%CI, [−.634, 6.05]). There was no significant moderated mediation between ε4 status and TMT A by left temporal WMH volumes moderated by age group with all covariates (−1.69(SE = 1.17), 95%CI [−4.31, .233]).

Figure 4. A. The relationship between APOE ε4 status and Trail Making Test part B mediated by left temporal WMH volume and moderated by age group. Coefficients of the moderated mediation model with sex, education, BMI, years smoking, hypertension, cholesterol, and statin medication status included as covariates. Percentile bootstrap resampling was performed with 10,000 iterations to produce 95% confidence intervals (CI). Regression coefficients significant; *p < .05, **p < .01; WMH, white matter hyperintensity. B. The relationship between APOE ε4 status and Stroop Color-Word Interference mediated by left temporal WMH volume and moderated by age group. Coefficients of the moderated mediation model with sex, education, BMI, years smoking, hypertension, cholesterol, and statin medication status included as covariates. Percentile bootstrap resampling was performed with 10,000 iterations to produce 95% confidence intervals (CI). Regression coefficients significant; *p < .05, **p < .01; WMH, white matter hyperintensity. C. The relationship between APOE ε4 status and Buschke Selective Reminding Test sum recall mediated by left temporal WMH volume and moderated by age group. Coefficients of the moderated mediation model with sex, education, BMI, years smoking, hypertension, cholesterol, and statin medication status included as covariates. Percentile bootstrap resampling was performed with 10,000 iterations to produce 95% confidence intervals (CI). Regression coefficients significant; *p < .05, **p < .01; WMH, white matter hyperintensity; BSRT, Buschke Selective Reminding Test. D. The relationship between APOE ε4 status and Buschke Selective Reminding Test consistent long-term retrieval mediated by left temporal WMH volume and moderated by age group. Coefficients of the moderated mediation model with sex, education, BMI, years smoking, hypertension, cholesterol, and statin medication status included as covariates. Percentile bootstrap resampling was performed with 10,000 iterations to produce 95% confidence intervals (CI). regression coefficients significant; *p < .05, **p < .01; WMH, white matter hyperintensity; BSRT, Buschke Selective Reminding Test.

Examination of the individual associations of the significant moderated mediation models showed there were no significant direct relations between APOE ε4 status and cognition for TMT B (−.375(SE = 8.03), 95%CI, [−16.22, 15.47], p = .963), Stroop Color-Word Interference (.751(SE = 2.06), 95%CI, [−3.32, 4.82, p = .716]), BSRT sum recall (.374(SE = 4.33), 95%CI, [−8.18, 8.93], p = .931), and BSRT CLTR (7.38(SE = 8.07), 95%CI, [−8.54, 23.30], p = .362). Further, these direct relations were not significantly moderated by age group for TMT B (−7.43(SE = 10.93), 95%CI, [−14.13, 29.00], p = .497), Stroop Color-Word Interference (−2.35(SE = 2.81), 95%CI, [−7.89, 3.20], p = .405), BSRT sum recall (−2.08(SE = 5.90), 95%CI, [−13.72, 9.56], p = .723), and BSRT CLTR (−9.85(SE = 10.98), 95%CI, [−31.51, 11.82], p = .371). For indirect associations, each model showed a significant overall positive relation between ε4 status and left temporal WMH volume (.571(SE = .205), 95%CI, [.166, .976], p = .006]), and this association was moderated by age group (−.846(SE = .278), 95%CI, [−1.40, −.297], p = .003). In the YO group, APOE ε4 status was significantly positively related to left temporal WMH volume (.571(SE = .205), 95%CI, [.166, .976], p = .006), but in the OO group, ε4 status was not significantly related to left temporal WMH volume (−.276(SE = .192), 95%CI, [−.654, .103], p = .152). There were significant overall relations between left temporal WMH volume and TMT B (8.06(SE = 2.89), 95%CI, [2.37, 13.75], p = .006), Stroop Color-Word Interference (−2.33(SE = .741), 95%CI, [−3.79, −.867], p = .002), BSRT sum recall (−3.76(SE = 1.56), 95%CI, [−6.84, −.690], p = .017), and BSRT CLTR (−7.10(SE = 2.90), 95%CI, [−12.82, −1.38], p = .015), indicating higher left temporal volume was associated with poorer cognitive functions.

Discussion

In a cohort of cognitively healthy adults, we found that in YO (50–69 years) but not OO (70–89 years) groups, ε4 carriers had greater WMH volumes in the left temporal and right parietal lobes than ε4 non-carriers. Additionally, OO adults had greater right parietal and left temporal WMH volumes than YO adults within ε4 non-carriers, yet there were no significant differences across age groups in ε4 carriers. This indicates that the regional distribution of WMH volumes may differ by APOE ε4 status and age group among healthy adults. Using moderated mediation models, we observed that differences in left temporal WMH volumes were, in turn, related to poorer memory and executive function performance within YO ε4 carriers. In contrast, right parietal WMH volume did not significantly predict differences in cognition. Notably, our healthy adult sample had low vascular risk, and all analyses included covariates to adjust for vascular health conditions, indicating these effects may be separable from the influence of vascular risk on brain structure and cognition in older adults. Our findings suggest that, among healthy YO ε4 carriers, increased left temporal WMH volume may represent an early marker of cognitive aging, and could be a harbinger of increased risk for AD.

One previous study observed that ε4 carriers showed greater WMH volume accumulation in parietal, temporal, occipital lobes than ε4 non-carriers (Brickman et al., Reference Brickman, Schupf, Manly, Stern, Luchsinger, Provenzano, Narkhede, Razlighi, Collins-Praino, Artero, Akbaraly, Ritchie, Mayeux and Portet2014). Our study found no significant main effects of APOE ε4 status on regional WMH volumes, but significant ε4 status and age group interactions only within the right parietal and left temporal lobes. Although not statistically significant after FDR correction, it is notable that a trend for an ε4 status and age group interaction was observed within right frontal WMH volume that followed the same pattern as the left temporal and right parietal WMH volumes. Thus, this interaction could emerge for right frontal WMH volume in a larger sample of healthy adults that had more power to detect smaller effects. Given the ε4 allele has been associated with multiple vascular mechanisms, including increased cerebrovascular disease and breakdown of the blood brain barrier (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Kanekiyo, Xu and Bu2013; Montagne et al., Reference Montagne, Nation, Sagare, Barisano, Sweeney, Chakhoyan, Pachicano, Joe, Nelson, D’Orazio, Buennagel, Harrington, Benzinger, Fagan, Ringman, Schneider, Morris, Reiman, Caselli, Chui, TCW, Chen, Pa, Conti, Law, Toga and Zlokovic2020; Raichlen & Alexander, Reference Raichlen and Alexander2014), our findings suggest that left temporal and right parietal WMH volumes may be especially vulnerable to ε4-related vascular mechanisms in YO adults, which appears to be lessened in OO ages. Previous studies in individuals with MCI and/or AD have observed significantly reduced hippocampal volume and morphology, poorer cognition, and increased parietal and temporal cortical thinning in YO ε4 carriers, compared to YO ε4 non-carriers, and no significant differences in gray matter or cognitive functions between ε4 status in OO groups (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Fennema‐Notestine, Holland, McEvoy, Stricker, Salmon, Dale and Bondi2014; Tang et al., Reference Tang, Holland, Dale and Miller2015). Furthermore, the APOE ε4 genotype has been associated with greater AD-related pathology, including increased amyloid and tau deposition (Therriault et al., Reference Therriault, Benedet, Pascoal, Mathotaarachchi, Chamoun, Savard and Rosa-Neto2020; Zerbinatti et al., Reference Zerbinatti, Wozniak, Cirrito, Cam, Osaka, Bales, Zhuo, Paul, Holtzman and Bu2004), and it has been suggested that temporal and parietal lobes may be preferentially vulnerable to amyloid or tau accumulation in the earliest stages of AD (Berron et al., Reference Berron, Vogel, Insel, Pereira, Xie, Wisse, Yushkevich, Palmqvist, Mattsson-Carlgren, Stomrud, Smith, Strandberg and Hansson2021; Insel et al., Reference Insel, Mormino, Aisen, Thompson and Donohue2020; Ossenkoppele et al., Reference Ossenkoppele, Zwan, Tolboom, van Assema, Adriaanse, Kloet, Boellaard, Windhorst, Barkhof, Lammertsma, Scheltens, van der Flier and van Berckel2012). Thus, the results of the present study, along with previous findings, suggest that the ε4 allele may affect multiple pathologies within the temporal and parietal lobes early in the AD course, which is observable in cognitively healthy YO adults.

Our findings may indicate that the impact of age on regional WMH volume may differ between APOE ε4 groups. It is important to note, however, as a study of healthy aging, the participants in our cohort underwent an extensive screening and were only eligible if they were determined to be neurologically and cognitively healthy. It is possible that older ε4 carriers in the community may have been less likely than non-carriers to meet our study’s inclusion criteria because of detectable cognitive difficulties in OO ages. However, both groups were acquired from the community in an identical manner, and there were no significant differences in the distribution of APOE ε4 status between the YO and OO groups. In fact, the OO group had a slightly numerically higher percentage of ε4 carriers (31.96%) than the YO group (27.78%), suggesting at least some of the OO ε4 carriers were not implicitly systematically excluded by our inclusion criteria for healthy aging. Our findings could indicate ε4 carriers that are neurologically healthy at older ages may rely on other health, lifestyle, or genetic factors to compensate for higher burden of regional WMH volumes. Given the current study is cross-sectional, research with longitudinal data is needed to examine how APOE ε4 status and age may interact to influence changes in regional WMH volume over time.

Previous findings with regional WMH volumes in relation to cognition are mixed, with separate studies observing significant associations between different cognitive functions and WMH volume within various cerebral lobes (Brugulat-Serrat et al., Reference Brugulat-Serrat, Salvadó, Sudre, Grau-Rivera, Suárez-Calvet, Falcon, Sánchez-Benavides, Gramunt, Fauria, Cardoso, Barkhof, Molinuevo and Gispert2020; Garnier-Crussard et al., Reference Garnier-Crussard, Bougacha, Wirth, André, Delarue, Landeau, Mézenge, Kuhn, Gonneaud, Chocat, Quillard, Ferrand-Devouge, de La Sayette, Vivien, Krolak-Salmon and Chételat2020; Gunning-Dixon & Raz, Reference Gunning-Dixon and Raz2003; Lampe et al., Reference Lampe, Kharabian-Masouleh, Kynast, Arelin, Steele, Löffler, Witte, Schroeter, Villringer and Bazin2019; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Salat, Jeng, McCreary, Fischl, Schmahmann, Dickerson, Viswanathan, Albert, Blacker and Greenberg2011). However, we found only left temporal WMH volumes was associated with performance on memory and executive function measures. While the mechanisms of how WMH volume contribute to cognitive decline is not fully understood, one potential explanation is that WMH volume accumulation may influence disconnection among networks in aging that leads to poorer cognition (Reijmer et al., Reference Reijmer, Schultz, Leemans, O’Sullivan, Gurol, Sperling, Greenberg, Viswanathan and Hedden2015). Another possibility is that WMH volume may impact cognitive functions by promoting ischemic-related axonal loss and subsequent cortical atrophy through Wallerian degeneration (Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Schmidt, Haybaeck, Loitfelder, Weis, Cavalieri, Seiler, Enzinger, Ropele, Erkinjuntti, Pantoni, Scheltens, Fazekas and Jellinger2011) or tau hyperphosphorylation (Zlokovic, Reference Zlokovic2011). Although the present study did not examine potential mechanisms, it is possible that left temporal WMH volumes are positioned in a region that could preferentially disrupt frontal-temporal connections or impact reductions in proximal neuroanatomical structures, such as the hippocampus, that in turn, influence poorer memory and executive function abilities. Additionally, WMH volumes may reflect a biomarker of cognitive dysfunction through its potential association with other causal vascular mechanisms.

Within healthy aging, APOE ε4 differences in cognition have been variable, with some studies observing no significant associations between ε4 status and cognition (Driscoll et al., Reference Driscoll, McDaniel and Guynn2005; O’Donoghue et al., Reference O’Donoghue, Murphy, Zamboni, Nobre and Mackay2018), and other studies finding ε4 carriers demonstrating poorer cognitive performance (Luck et al., Reference Luck, Then, Luppa, Schroeter, Arélin, Burkhardt, Thiery, Löffler, Villringer and Riedel-Heller2015; O’Hara et al., Reference O’Hara, Sommer, Way, Kraemer, Taylor and Murphy2008; Wetter et al., Reference Wetter, Delis, Houston, Jacobson, Lansing, Cobell and Bondi2005), particularly on memory measures (Bondi et al., Reference Bondi, Salmon, Monsch, Galasko, Butters, Klauber, Thal and Saitoh1995; Caselli et al., Reference Caselli, Reiman, Osborne, Hentz, Baxter, Hernandez and Alexander2004; Caselli et al., Reference Caselli, Dueck, Osborne, Sabbagh, Connor, Ahern, Baxter, Rapcsak, Shi, Woodruff, Locke, Snyder, Alexander, Rademakers and Reiman2009; Jacobson et al., Reference Jacobson, Delis, Lansing, Houston, Olsen, Wetter, Bondi and Salmon2005) relative to ε4 non-carriers. We did not find significant ε4 group differences or ε4 status and age group interactions directly related to cognitive performance but observed that ε4 status impacted memory and executive functions only through the mediational role of left temporal WMH volume. These findings suggest elevated left temporal WMH volume may influence memory and executive function, but not processing speed or language abilities, in YO ε4 carriers. Further, this demonstrates the benefit of moderated mediation models, which may help in detecting the earliest effects of the ε4 allele on brain and cognitive aging.

This study has several limitations. First, our sample largely consists of white individuals with a generally higher level of education and health status. This lack of diversity in our sample limits the generalizability of our findings and could, in part, reflect the methods used to include the healthy cognitively unimpaired individuals with low levels of vascular health factors in our sample. Recent findings have suggested that cognitive screening measures may not accurately reflect cognitive status of those belonging to some racial and ethnic groups (Carson et al., Reference Carson, Leach and Murphy2018). This may be due to bias in neuropsychological tests and multiple sociocultural factors that contribute to racial health disparities (Zahodne et al., Reference Zahodne, Sharifian, Kraal, Zaheed, Sol, Morris, Schupf, Manly and Brickman2021). Additionally, the APOE ε4 allele has been found to be a strong genetic AD risk factor within non-Hispanic white adults but may be less related to AD risk in other racial and ethnic groups (Raichlen & Alexander, Reference Raichlen and Alexander2014). More research is needed to investigate whether and how ε4 status, and APOE variants (Deters et al., Reference Deters, Mormino, Yu, Lutz, Bennett and Barnes2021), may influence brain and cognitive aging in diverse samples. Finally, the present study uses cross-sectional data. Our study presented important differences in regional WMH volumes and cognition within healthy older adults, but additional longitudinal research would be important to examine if APOE ε4 status leads to greater regional WMH volume accumulation and cognitive decline over time, and if these effects differ by age.

Conclusions

Within cognitively healthy older adults with lower vascular risk, YO carriers of the ε4 allele had elevated left temporal and right parietal WMH volumes, compared to YO ε4 non-carriers. This suggests these regions are sensitive to ε4-related vascular disease mechanisms and highlights the impact the ε4 allele has on brain aging in early old age. Moreover, increases in left temporal WMH volumes were related to poorer memory and executive functions in only the YO ε4 carriers. The results of the present study suggest that elevated left temporal WMH volume, within ε4 carriers in younger older adults, may be indicative of accelerated cognitive aging and may potentially lead to greater risk for AD. Evaluation of intervention therapies that lessen the accumulation of WMH’s may be particularly beneficial in ε4 carriers in YO age ranges to help reduce AD risk.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617724000122.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to acknowledge support from the National Institute on Aging (AG025526, AG019610, AG072980, AG072445, AG064587, and AG067200), the state of Arizona and Arizona Department of Health Services, and the McKnight Brain Research Foundation.

Competing interests

The authors have no actual or potential conflicts of interest.

References

Addya, K., Wang, Y. L., & Leonard, D. G. (1997). Optimization of apolipoprotein E genotyping. Molecular Diagnosis, 2(4), 271276. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1084-8592(97)80038-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexander, G. E., Bergfield, K. L., Chen, K., Reiman, E. M., Hanson, K. D., Lin, L., Bandy, D., Caselli, R. J., & Moeller, J. R. (2012a). Gray matter network associated with risk for Alzheimer’s disease in young to middle-aged adults. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(12), 27232732. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.01.014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexander, G. E., Ryan, L., Bowers, D., Foster, T. C., Bizon, J. L., Geldmacher, D. S., & Glisky, E. L. (2012b). Characterizing cognitive aging in humans with links to animal models. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 4, 21. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2012.00021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avants, B. B., Tustison, N. J., Song, G., Cook, P. A., Klein, A., & Gee, J. C. (2011). A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. NeuroImage, 54(3), 20332044. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Donoghue, M. C., Murphy, S. E., Zamboni, G., Nobre, A. C., & Mackay, C. E. (2018). APOE genotype and cognition in healthy individuals at risk of Alzheimer’s disease: A review. Cortex, 104, 103123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.03.025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caselli, R. J., Dueck, A. C., Osborne, D., Sabbagh, M. N., Connor, D. J., Ahern, G. L., Baxter, L. C., Rapcsak, S. Z., Shi, J., Woodruff, B. K., Locke, D. E. C., Snyder, C. H., Alexander, G. E., Rademakers, R., Reiman, E. M. (2009). Longitudinal modeling of age-related memory decline and the APOE ε4 effect. New England Journal of Medicine, 361(3), 255263. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa0809437 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal statistical society: series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289300.10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berron, D., Vogel, J. W., Insel, P. S., Pereira, J. B., Xie, L., Wisse, L. E. M., Yushkevich, P. A., Palmqvist, S., Mattsson-Carlgren, N., Stomrud, E., Smith, R., Strandberg, O., Hansson, O. (2021). Early stages of tau pathology and its associations with functional connectivity, atrophy and memory. Brain, 144(9), 27712783. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awab114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertram, L., Lill, C. M., & Tanzi, R. E. (2010). The genetics of Alzheimer disease: Back to the future. Neuron, 68(2), 270281. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.10.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biesbroek, J. M., Weaver, N. A., & Biessels, G. J. (2017). Lesion location and cognitive impact of cerebral small vessel disease. Clinical Science, 131(8), 715728. https://doi.org/10.1042/CS20160452 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birdsill, A. C., Koscik, R. L., Jonaitis, E. M., Johnson, S. C., Okonkwo, O. C., Hermann, B. P., LaRue, A., Sager, M. A., Bendlin, B. B. (2014). Regional white matter hyperintensities: Aging, Alzheimer’s disease risk, and cognitive function. Neurobiology of Aging, 35(4), 769776. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.10.072 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bondi, M. W., Salmon, D. P., Monsch, A. U., Galasko, D., Butters, N., Klauber, M. R., Thal, L. J., Saitoh, T. (1995). Episodic memory changes are associated with the APOE-epsilon 4 allele in nondemented older adults. Neurology, 45(12), 22032206. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.45.12.2203 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonham, L. W., Geier, E. G., Fan, C. C., Leong, J. K., Besser, L., Kukull, W. A., Kornak, J., Andreassen, O. A., Schellenberg, G. D., Rosen, H. J., Dillon, W. P., Hess, C. P., Miller, B. L., Dale, A. M., Desikan, R. S., Yokoyama, J. S. (2016). Age-dependent effects of APOE ε4 in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Annals of Clinical and Translational Neurology, 3(9), 668677. https://doi.org/10.1002/I3.333 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brickman, A. M. (2013). Contemplating Alzheimer’s disease and the contribution of white matter hyperintensities. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports, 13(12), 19. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11910-013-0415-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brickman, A. M., Schupf, N., Manly, J. J., Stern, Y., Luchsinger, J. A., Provenzano, F. A., Narkhede, A., Razlighi, Q. R., Collins-Praino, L. E., Artero, S., Akbaraly, T., Ritchie, K., Mayeux, R., &Portet, F. (2014). APOE ε4 and risk for Alzheimer’s disease: Do regionally distributed white matter hyperintensities play a role? Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 10(6), 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.07.155.10.1016/j.jalz.2014.07.155CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brickman, A. M., Zahodne, L. B., Guzman, V. A., Narkhede, A., Meier, I. B., Griffith, E. Y., Provenzano, F. A., Schupf, N., Manly, J. J., Stern, Y., Luchsinger, Jé A., Mayeux, R. (2015). Reconsidering harbingers of dementia: Progression of parietal lobe white matter hyperintensities predicts Alzheimer’s disease incidence. Neurobiology of Aging, 36(1), 2732. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.07.019 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brugulat-Serrat, A., Salvadó, G., Sudre, C. H., Grau-Rivera, O., Suárez-Calvet, M., Falcon, C., Sánchez-Benavides, G., Gramunt, N., Fauria, K., Cardoso, M. J., Barkhof, F., Molinuevo, Jé L., Gispert, J. D. (2020). Patterns of white matter hyperintensities associated with cognition in middle-aged cognitively healthy individuals. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 14(5), 20122023. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-019-00151-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buschke, H. (1973). Selective reminding for analysis of memory and learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12(5), 543550. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80034-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carson, N., Leach, L., & Murphy, K. J. (2018). A re‐examination of Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) cutoff scores. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 33(2), 379388. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.4756 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caselli, R. J., Reiman, E. M., Osborne, D., Hentz, J. G., Baxter, L. C., Hernandez, J. L., & Alexander, G. E. (2004). Longitudinal changes in cognition and behavior in asymptomatic carriers of the APOE e4 allele. Neurology, 62(11), 19901995. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.WNL.0000129533.26544.BFn CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, Y‐Ling, Fennema‐Notestine, C., Holland, D., McEvoy, L. K., Stricker, N. H., Salmon, D. P., Dale, A. M., Bondi, M. W., Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (2014). APOE interacts with age to modify rate of decline in cognitive and brain changes in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 10(3), 336348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2013.05.1763 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corder, E. H., Saunders, A. M., Strittmatter, W. J., Schmechel, D. E., Gaskell, P. C., Small, G. W., Roses, A. D., Haines, J. L., Pericak-Vance, M. A. (1993). Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease in late onset families. Science, 261(5123), 921923. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.8346443 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desikan, R. S., Ségonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., Dickerson, B. C., Blacker, D., Buckner, R. L., Dale, A. M., Maguire, R. P., Hyman, B. T., Albert, M. S., Killiany, R. J. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage, 31(3), 968980. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deters, K. D., Mormino, E. C., Yu, L., Lutz, M. W., Bennett, D. A., & Barnes, L. L. (2021). TOMM40-APOE haplotypes are associated with cognitive decline in non-demented Blacks. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 17(8), 12871296. https://doi.org/10.1002/alz.12295 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Driscoll, I., McDaniel, M. A., & Guynn, M. J. (2005). Apolipoprotein E and prospective memory in normally aging adults. Neuropsychology, 19(1), 2834. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.19.1.28 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farrer, L. A., Cupples, L. A., Haines, J. L., Hyman, B., Kukull, W. A., Mayeux, R., & Van Duijn, C. M. (1997). Effects of age, sex, and ethnicity on the association between apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer disease: A meta-analysis. JAMA, 278(16), 13491356.10.1001/jama.1997.03550160069041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189198. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franchetti, M. K., Bharadwaj, P. K., Nguyen, L. A., Van Etten, E. J., Klimentidis, Y. C., Hishaw, G. A., Trouard, T. P., Raichlen, D. A., Alexander, G. E. (2020). Interaction of age and self-reported physical sports activity on white matter hyperintensity volume in healthy older adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 12, 576025. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2020.576025 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garnier-Crussard, A., Bougacha, S., Wirth, M., André, C., Delarue, M., Landeau, B., Mézenge, F., Kuhn, E., Gonneaud, J., Chocat, A., Quillard, A., Ferrand-Devouge, E., de La Sayette, V., Vivien, D., Krolak-Salmon, P., Chételat, G. B.;l (2020). White matter hyperintensities across the adult lifespan: Relation to age, Aβ load, and cognition. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy, 12(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-020-00669-4 Google ScholarPubMed
Glickman, M. E., Rao, S. R., & Schultz, M. R. (2014). False discovery rate control is a recommended alternative to Bonferroni-type adjustments in health studies. Journal of clinical epidemiology, 67(8), 850857. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2014.03.012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glisky, E. L. (2007). Changes in cognitive function in human aging. In Brain aging: Models, methods, and mechanisms. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Godin, O., Tzourio, C., Maillard, P., Alpérovitch, A., Mazoyer, B., & Dufouil, C. (2009). Apolipoprotein E genotype is related to progression of white matter lesion load. Stroke, 40(10), 31863190. https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.555839 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golden, C. J., & Freshwater, S. M. (2017, Stroop color and word test. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 557.Google Scholar
Gunning-Dixon, F. M., & Raz, N. (2003). Neuroanatomical correlates of selected executive functions in middle-aged and older adults: A prospective MRI study. Neuropsychologia, 41(14), 19291941. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00129-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 23(1), 5662.10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, A. F. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Insel, P. S., Mormino, E. C., Aisen, P. S., Thompson, W. K., & Donohue, M. C. (2020). Neuroanatomical spread of amyloid β and tau in Alzheimer’s disease: Implications for primary prevention. Brain Communications, 2(1), fcaa007. https://doi.org/10.1093/braincomms/fcaa007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobson, M. W., Delis, D. C., Lansing, A., Houston, W., Olsen, R., Wetter, S., Bondi, M. W., Salmon, D. P. (2005). Asymmetries in global-local processing ability in elderly people with the apolipoprotein E-ε4 allele. Neuropsychology, 19(6), 822829. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.19.6.822 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983). Boston naming test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.Google Scholar
Lampe, L., Kharabian-Masouleh, S., Kynast, J., Arelin, K., Steele, C. J., Löffler, M., Witte, A. V., Schroeter, M. L., Villringer, A., Bazin, P.-L. (2019). Lesion location matters: The relationships between white matter hyperintensities on cognition in the healthy elderly. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism, 39(1), 3643. https://doi.org/10.1177/0271678X17740501 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, C. C., Kanekiyo, T., Xu, H., & Bu, G. (2013). Apolipoprotein E and Alzheimer disease: Risk, mechanisms and therapy. Nature Reviews Neurology, 9(2), 106118. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrneurol.2012.263 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luck, T., Then, F. S., Luppa, M., Schroeter, M. L., Arélin, K., Burkhardt, R., Thiery, J., Löffler, M., Villringer, A., Riedel-Heller, S. G. (2015). Association of the apolipoprotein E genotype with memory performance and executive functioning in cognitively intact elderly. Neuropsychology, 29(3), 382387. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000147 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monsch, A. U., Bondi, M. W., Butters, N., Salmon, D. P., Katzman, R., & Thal, L. J. (1992). Comparisons of verbal fluency tasks in the detection of dementia of the Alzheimer type. Archives of neurology, 49(12), 12531258. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1992.00530360051017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montagne, A., Nation, D. A., Sagare, A. P., Barisano, G., Sweeney, M. D., Chakhoyan, A., Pachicano, M., Joe, E., Nelson, A. R., D’Orazio, L. M., Buennagel, D. P., Harrington, M. G., Benzinger, T. L. S., Fagan, A. M., Ringman, J. M., Schneider, L. S., Morris, J. C., Reiman, E. M., Caselli, R. J., Chui, H. C., TCW, J., Chen, Y., Pa, J., Conti, P. S., Law, M., Toga, A. W., Zlokovic, B. V. (2020). APOE4 leads to blood-brain barrier dysfunction predicting cognitive decline. Nature, 581(7806), 7176. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-2247-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nguyen, L. A., Haws, K. A., Fitzhugh, M. C., Torre, G. A., Hishaw, G. A., & Alexander, G. E. (2016). Interactive effects of subjective memory complaints and hypertension on learning and memory performance in the elderly. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 23(2), 154170. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2015.1063580 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Hara, R., Sommer, B., Way, N., Kraemer, H. C., Taylor, J., & Murphy, G. (2008). Slower speed-of-processing of cognitive tasks is associated with presence of the apolipoprotein ε4 allele. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 42(3), 199204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.12.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ossenkoppele, R., Zwan, M. D., Tolboom, N., van Assema, D. M. E., Adriaanse, S. F., Kloet, R. W., Boellaard, R., Windhorst, A. D., Barkhof, F., Lammertsma, A. A., Scheltens, P., van der Flier, W. M., van Berckel, B. N. M. (2012). Amyloid burden and metabolic function in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease: Parietal lobe involvement. Brain, 135(7), 21152125. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/aws113 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, D. C., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology, 60(1), 173196.10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prins, N. D., & Scheltens, P. (2015). White matter hyperintensities, cognitive impairment and dementia: An update. Nature Reviews Neurology, 11(3), 157165.10.1038/nrneurol.2015.10CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raichlen, D. A., & Alexander, G. E. (2014). Exercise, APOE genotype, and the evolution of the human lifespan. Trends in Neurosciences, 37(5), 247255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2014.03.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reijmer, Y. D., Schultz, A. P., Leemans, A., O’Sullivan, M. J., Gurol, M. E., Sperling, R., Greenberg, S. M., Viswanathan, A., Hedden, T. (2015). Decoupling of structural and functional brain connectivity in older adults with white matter hyperintensities. NeuroImage, 117, 222229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.054 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reitan, R. M. (1956). Trail making test: Manual for administration, scoring and interpretation (pp. 134). Indiana University.Google Scholar
Rojas, S., Brugulat-Serrat, A., Bargallo, N., Minguillon, C., Tucholka, A., Falcon, C., & Gispert, J. D. (2018). Higher prevalence of cerebral white matter hyperintensities in homozygous APOE-ε4 allele carriers aged 45-75: Results from the ALFA study. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism, 38(2), 250261. https://doi.org/10.1177/0271678X17707397 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosen, W. G. (1980). Verbal fluency in aging and dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 2(2), 135146.10.1080/01688638008403788CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (1992). Influence of processing speed on adult age differences in working memory. Acta Psychologica, 79(2), 155170. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(92)90030-H CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schilling, S., DeStefano, A. L., Sachdev, P. S., Choi, S. H., Mather, K. A., DeCarli, C. D., Wen, W., Høgh, P., Raz, N., Au, R., Beiser, A., Wolf, P. A., Romero, J. R., Zhu, Y.-C., Lunetta, K. L., Farrer, L., Dufouil, C., Kuller, L. H., Mazoyer, B., Seshadri, S., Tzourio, C., Debette, S. (2013). APOE genotype and MRI markers of cerebrovascular disease: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurology, 81(3), 292300. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e31829bfda4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, P., Gaser, C., Arsic, M., Buck, D., Förschler, A., Berthele, A., Hoshi, M., Ilg, R. C.;diger, Schmid, V. J., Zimmer, C., Hemmer, B., Mühlau, M. (2012). An automated tool for detection of FLAIR-hyperintense white-matter lesions in multiple sclerosis. NeuroImage, 59(4), 37743783. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.032 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, R., Schmidt, H., Haybaeck, J., Loitfelder, M., Weis, S., Cavalieri, M., Seiler, S., Enzinger, C., Ropele, S., Erkinjuntti, T., Pantoni, L., Scheltens, P., Fazekas, F., Jellinger, K. (2011). Heterogeneity in age-related white matter changes. Acta Neuropathologica, 122(2), 171185. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00401-011-0851-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Small, B. J., Rosnick, C. B., Fratiglioni, L., & Bäckman, L. (2004). Apolipoprotein E and cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 19(4), 592600. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.4.592 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, E. E., Salat, D. H., Jeng, J., McCreary, C. R., Fischl, B., Schmahmann, J. D., Dickerson, B. C., Viswanathan, A., Albert, M. S., Blacker, D., Greenberg, S. M. (2011). Correlations between MRI white matter lesion location and executive function and episodic memory. Neurology, 76(17), 14921499. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e318217e7c8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tang, X., Holland, D., Dale, A. M., Miller, M. I., & Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (2015). APOE affects the volume and shape of the amygdala and the hippocampus in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: Age matters. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 47(3), 645660. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-150262 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Therriault, J., Benedet, A. L., Pascoal, T. A., Mathotaarachchi, S., Chamoun, M., Savard, M., & Rosa-Neto, P. (2020). Association of apolipoprotein E ε4 with medial temporal tau independent of amyloid-β. JAMA Neurology, 77(4), 470479. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaneurol.2019.4421 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tondelli, M., Wilcock, G. K., Nichelli, P., De Jager, C. A., Jenkinson, M., & Zamboni, G. (2012). Structural MRI changes detectable up to ten years before clinical Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(4), 825e25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.05.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valerio, D., Raventos, H., Schmeidler, J., Beeri, M. S., Villalobos, L. M., Bolaños-Palmieri, P., & Silverman, J. M. (2014). Association of apolipoprotein E-e4 and dementia declines with age. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22(10), 957960. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jagp.2014.03.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Etten, E. J., Bharadwaj, P. K., Hishaw, G. A., Huentelman, M. J., Trouard, T. P., Grilli, M. D., & Alexander, G. E. (2021). Influence of regional white matter hyperintensity volume and apolipoprotein E ε4 status on hippocampal volume in healthy older adults. Hippocampus, 31(5), 469480. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.23308 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wardlaw, J. M., Allerhand, M., Doubal, F. N., Hernandez, M. V., Morris, Z., Gow, A. J., & Deary, I. J. (2014). Vascular risk factors, large-artery atheroma, and brain white matter hyperintensities. Neurology, 82(15), 13311338. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000000312 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler adult intelligence scale - Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV), vol. 22, NCS Pearson. https://doi.org/10.2298/psi171001001l Google Scholar
Wetter, S. R., Delis, D. C., Houston, W. S., Jacobson, M. W., Lansing, A., Cobell, K., & Bondi, M. W. (2005). Deficits in inhibition and flexibility are associated with the APOE-E4 allele in nondemented older adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27(8), 943952. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390490919001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wisdom, N. M., Callahan, J. L., & Hawkins, K. A. (2011). The effects of apolipoprotein E on non-impaired cognitive functioning: A meta-analysis. Neurobiology of Aging, 32(1), 6374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.02.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yushkevich, P. A., Piven, J., Hazlett, H. C., Smith, R. G., Ho, S., Gee, J. C., & Gerig, G. (2006). User-guided 3D active contour segmentation of anatomical structures: Significantly improved efficiency and reliability. NeuroImage, 31(3), 11161128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahodne, L. B., Sharifian, N., Kraal, A. Z., Zaheed, A. B., Sol, K., Morris, E. P., Schupf, N., Manly, J. J., Brickman, A. M. (2021). Socioeconomic and psychosocial mechanisms underlying racial/ethnic disparities in cognition among older adults. Neuropsychology, 35(3), 265275. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000720 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zerbinatti, C. V., Wozniak, D. F., Cirrito, J., Cam, J. A., Osaka, H., Bales, K. R., Zhuo, M., Paul, S. M., Holtzman, D. M., Bu, G. (2004). Increased soluble amyloid-β peptide and memory deficits in amyloid model mice overexpressing the low-density lipoprotein receptor-related protein. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 101(4), 10751080. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0305803101 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zlokovic, B. V. (2011). Neurovascular pathways to neurodegeneration in Alzheimer’s disease and other disorders. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(12), 723738. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3114 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Illustration of the hypothesized moderated mediation model of the relationship between APOE ε4 status and cognition mediated by regional white matter hyperintensity volume and moderated by age group (young-old and old-old).

Figure 1

Table 1. Table of demographic characteristics

Figure 2

Table 2. Effects of APOE ε4 status, age group, and their interaction on regional white matter hyperintensity volumes

Figure 3

Figure 2. The mean and standard error of left temporal WMH volume for age group and APOE ε4 status. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) showed that, after controlling for sex, education, BMI, years smoking, hypertension, cholesterol, and statin medication status, there was a significant main effect for age group (FDRp = 8.0E-6); there was no main effect for APOE ε4 status (FDRp = .998); and there was a significant age group by APOE ε4 status interaction (FDRp = .022). *Simple effects analyses revealed young-old APOE ε4 carriers had significantly greater left temporal (FDRp = .012) WMH volumes than young-old APOE ε4 non-carriers; old-old APOE ε4 non-carriers had significantly greater left temporal (FDRp = 4.64E-11) WMH volumes young-old APOE ε4 non-carriers; there were no significant differences across age groups within ε4 carriers; there were no significant differences across ε4 groups within the old-old. Blue bars represent APOE ε4 non-carriers and red bars represent APOE ε4 carriers. Note. WMH = white matter hyperintensity; YO = young-old; OO = old-old; APOE = apolipoprotein E.

Figure 4

Figure 3. The mean and standard error of right parietal WMH volume for age group and APOE ε4 status. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) showed that, after controlling for sex, education, BMI, years smoking, hypertension, cholesterol, and statin medication status, there was a significant main effect for age group (FDRp = 1.3E-5); there was no main effect for APOE ε4 status (FDRp = .998); and there was a significant age group by APOE ε4 status interaction (FDRp = .026). *Simple effects analyses revealed young-old APOE ε4 carriers had significantly greater right parietal WMH volumes (FDRp = .046) than young-old APOE ε4 non-carriers; old-old APOE ε4 non-carriers had significantly greater right parietal WMH volumes (FDRp = 8.20E-8) young-old APOE ε4 non-carriers; there were no significant differences across age groups within ε4 carriers; there were no significant differences across ε4 groups within the old-old. Blue bars represent APOE ε4 non-carriers and red bars represent APOE ε4 carriers. Note. WMH = white matter hyperintensity; YO = young-old; OO = old-old; APOE = apolipoprotein E.

Figure 5

Figure 4. A. The relationship between APOE ε4 status and Trail Making Test part B mediated by left temporal WMH volume and moderated by age group. Coefficients of the moderated mediation model with sex, education, BMI, years smoking, hypertension, cholesterol, and statin medication status included as covariates. Percentile bootstrap resampling was performed with 10,000 iterations to produce 95% confidence intervals (CI). Regression coefficients significant; *p < .05, **p < .01; WMH, white matter hyperintensity. B. The relationship between APOE ε4 status and Stroop Color-Word Interference mediated by left temporal WMH volume and moderated by age group. Coefficients of the moderated mediation model with sex, education, BMI, years smoking, hypertension, cholesterol, and statin medication status included as covariates. Percentile bootstrap resampling was performed with 10,000 iterations to produce 95% confidence intervals (CI). Regression coefficients significant; *p < .05, **p < .01; WMH, white matter hyperintensity. C. The relationship between APOE ε4 status and Buschke Selective Reminding Test sum recall mediated by left temporal WMH volume and moderated by age group. Coefficients of the moderated mediation model with sex, education, BMI, years smoking, hypertension, cholesterol, and statin medication status included as covariates. Percentile bootstrap resampling was performed with 10,000 iterations to produce 95% confidence intervals (CI). Regression coefficients significant; *p < .05, **p < .01; WMH, white matter hyperintensity; BSRT, Buschke Selective Reminding Test. D. The relationship between APOE ε4 status and Buschke Selective Reminding Test consistent long-term retrieval mediated by left temporal WMH volume and moderated by age group. Coefficients of the moderated mediation model with sex, education, BMI, years smoking, hypertension, cholesterol, and statin medication status included as covariates. Percentile bootstrap resampling was performed with 10,000 iterations to produce 95% confidence intervals (CI). regression coefficients significant; *p < .05, **p < .01; WMH, white matter hyperintensity; BSRT, Buschke Selective Reminding Test.

Supplementary material: File

Van Etten et al. supplementary material

Van Etten et al. supplementary material
Download Van Etten et al. supplementary material(File)
File 17.8 KB