Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-tn8tq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-27T19:58:39.489Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Preserved memory monitoring but impaired memory control during episodic encoding in patients with schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 February 2007

ELISABETH BACON
Affiliation:
Physiopathologie clinique et expérimentale de la schizophrénie, Université Louis Pasteur, Strasbourg, France
MARIE IZAUTE
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Psychologie Sociale de la Cognition, Université Blaise Pascal, Clermont-Ferrand, France
JEAN-MARIE DANION
Affiliation:
Physiopathologie clinique et expérimentale de la schizophrénie, Université Louis Pasteur, Strasbourg, France

Abstract

Metamemory awareness refers to the ability to monitor and control how well information is processed depending on the loads and needs of the task at hand. There is some evidence that metamemory functions are impaired in schizophrenia at the time of memory retrieval. This study investigated whether patients with schizophrenia exhibit metamemory abnormalities during the encoding of new information. The frequency of item presentation was varied. Both memory control and memory monitoring were assessed using study-time allocation and Judgments of Learning (JOL), respectively. Repeated items were recalled better by both groups, but memory performance was lower in patients than controls. Patients' behavior patterns were abnormal in terms of the study-time allocated for each item according to presentation frequency. Patients' JOLs were lower than those of controls but remained sensitive to item repetition. Patients' predictive values on memory accuracy were no different to those measured in controls. In addition, none of the patients reported using efficient strategies to help memorize target items. The results show a dissociation between memory control, which was impaired, and memory monitoring, which was spared, in patients with schizophrenia during encoding of new information (JINS, 2007, 13, 219–227.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2007 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Aleman, A., Hijman, R., de Haan, E.H., & Kahn, R.S. (1999). Memory impairment in Schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 13581366.Google Scholar
Bacon, E., Danion, J.M., Kauffmann-Muller, F., & Bruant, A. (2001). Consciousness in schizophrenia: A metacognitive approach to semantic memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 10, 473484.Google Scholar
Bacon, E. & Huet, N. (2005). Metamemory knowledge and beliefs in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31, 349.Google Scholar
Bilder, R.M., Goldman, R.S., Robinson, D., Reiter, G., Bell, L., Bates, J.A., Pappadopulos, E., Willson, D.F., Alvir, J.M., Woerner, M.G., Geisler, S., Kane, J.M., & Lieberman, J.A. (2000). Neuropsychology of first-episode schizophrenia: Initial characterization and clinical correlates. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 549559.Google Scholar
Brebion, G., Amador, X., Smith, M.J., & Gorman, J.M. (1997). Mechanisms underlying memory impairment in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 27, 383393.Google Scholar
Calev, A. (1984a). Recall and recognition in chronic nondemented schizophrenics: Use of matched tasks. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 172177.Google Scholar
Calev, A. (1984b). Recall and recognition in mildly disturbed schizophrenics: The use of matched tasks. Psychology and Medicine, 14, 425429.Google Scholar
Costermans, J., Lories, G., & Ansay, C. (1992). Confidence level and feeling of knowing in question answering: The weight of inferential processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 142150.Google Scholar
Danion, J.M., Gokalsing, E., Robert, P., Massin-Krauss, M., & Bacon, E. (2001). Defective relationship between subjective experience and behavior in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 20642066.Google Scholar
Danion, J.M., Rizzo, L., & Bruant, A. (1999). Functional mechanisms underlying impaired recognition memory and conscious awareness in patients with schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 639644.Google Scholar
Dickinson, D., Iannone, V.N., Wilk, C.M., & Gold, J.M. (2004). General and specific cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 826833.Google Scholar
Ferrand, L. & Alario, F.X. (1998). Note méthodologique. Normes d'associations verbales pour 366 noms d'objets concrets. L'Année Psychologique, 98, 659709.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. (1979). Metacognitive and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906911.Google Scholar
Gold, J.M., Randolph, C., Carpenter, C.J., Goldberg, T.E., & Weinberger, D.R. (1992). Forms of memory failure in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 487494.Google Scholar
Goodman, L.A. & Kruskal, W.H. (1954). Measures of association for cross classifications. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 49, 732764.Google Scholar
Green, M.F. (1996). What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 321330.Google Scholar
Heinrichs, W.R. & Zakzanis, K.K. (1998). Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: A quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology, 12, 426445.Google Scholar
Kelemen, W.L. & Weawer, C.A. (1997). Enhanced metamemory at delays: Why do judgements of learning improve over time? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 23, 13941409.Google Scholar
Knoblich, G., Stottmeister, F., & Kircher, T. (2004). Self-monitoring in patients with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 34, 15611569.Google Scholar
Koh, S.D., Kayton, L., & Berry, R. (1973). Mnemonic organization in young nonpsychotic schizophrenics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 81, 299310.Google Scholar
Koh, S.D. & Peterson, R.A. (1978). Encoding orientation and the remembering of schizophrenic young adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 303313.Google Scholar
Koren, D., Seidman, L.J., Poyurovsky, M., Goldsmith, M., Viksman, P., Zichel, S., & Klein, E. (2004). The neuropsychological basis of insight in first-episode schizophrenia: A pilot metacognitive study. Schizoprenia Research, 70, 195202.Google Scholar
Koriat, A. (1997). Monitoring one's knowledge during study: A cue utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 4, 349370.Google Scholar
Koriat, A. & Goldsmith, M. (1996). Monitoring and control processes in the strategic regulation of memory accuracy. Psychological Review, 103, 490517.Google Scholar
Kubicki, M., McCarley, R.W., Nestor, P.G., Huh, T., Kikinis, R., Shenton, M.E., & Wible, C.G. (2003). An fMRI study of semantic processing in men with schizophrenia. Neuroimage, 20, 19231933.Google Scholar
Lewis, R. (2004). Should cognitive deficit be a diagnostic criterion for schizophrenia? Journal of Psychiatry Neurosciences, 29, 102113.Google Scholar
Lussier, I. & Stip, E. (2001). Memory and attention deficits in drug naïve patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 48, 4555.Google Scholar
Lysaker, P.H., Carcione, A., Dimaggio, G., Johannesen, J.K., Nicolò, G., Procacc, M., & Semerari, A. (2005). Metacognition amidst narratives of self and illness in schizophrenia: Associations with neurocognition, symptoms, insight and quality of life. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 112, 6471.Google Scholar
Mazzoni, G. & Nelson, T.O. (1995). Judgments of learning are affected by the kind of encoding in ways that cannot be attributed to the level of recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 12631274.Google Scholar
Medalia, A., Revheim, N., & Casey, M. (2000). Remediation of memory disorders in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 6, 14511459.Google Scholar
Moulin, C.J.A., Perfect, T.J., & Jones, R.W. (2000a). The effects of repetition on allocation of study time and judgements of learning in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia, 38, 748756.Google Scholar
Moulin, C.J.A., Perfect, T.J., & Jones, R.W. (2000b). Evidence for intact memory monitoring in Alzheimer's disease: Metamemory sensitivity at encoding. Neuropsychologia, 38, 12421250.Google Scholar
Nelson, T.O. (1984). A comparison of current measures of the accuracy of feeling-of-knowing predictions. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 109133.Google Scholar
Nelson, T.O. (1987). The Goodman-Kruskal Gamma coefficient as an alternative to signal-detection theory's measures of absolute-judgement accuracy. In E. Roskam & R. Suck (Eds.), Progress in mathematical psychology (pp. 299306). Amsterdam: Elsevier/North Holland.
Nelson, T.O. & Dunlosky, J. (1991). When people's judgments of learning (JOLs) are extremely accurate at predicting subsequent recall: The “Delayed-JOL Effect”. Psychological Science, 2, 267270.Google Scholar
Nelson, T.O. & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 25, 125173.Google Scholar
Opler, L.A., Kay, S.R., Lindenmayer, J.P., & Fiszbein, A. (1991). Structured clinical interview for the PANSS (SCI-PANSS), Traduction française Lindenmayer, JP.
Palmer, B.W., Heaton, R.K., Paulsen, J.S., Kuck, J., Braff, D., Harris, M.J., Zisook, S., & Jeste, D.V. (1997). Is it possible to be schizophrenic yet neuropsychologically normal? Neuropsychology, 11, 437446.Google Scholar
Paul, B.M., Elvevag, B., Bokat, C.E., Weinberger, D.R., & Goldberg, T.E. (2005). Levels of processing effects on recognition memory in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 74, 101110.Google Scholar
Ragland, J.D., Gur, R.C., Valdez, J.N., Loughead, J., Elliott, M., Kohler, C., Kanes, S., Siegel, S.J., Moelter, S.T., & Gur, R.E. (2005). Levels-of-processing effect on frontotemporal function in schizophrenia during word encoding and recognition. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 18401848.Google Scholar
Ragland, J.D., Moelter, S.T., McGrath, C., Hill, S.K., Gur, R.E., Bilker, W.B., Siegel, S.J., & Gur, R.C. (2003). Levels-of-processing effect on word recognition in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 11541161.Google Scholar
Rushe, T.M., Woodruff, P.W., Murray, R.M., & Morris, R.G. (1999). Episodic memory and learning in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 35, 8596.Google Scholar
Saykin, A.J., Gur, R.C., Gur, R.E., Mozley, P.D., Mozley, L.H., Resnick, S.M., Kester, D.B., & Stafiniak, P. (1991). Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia. Selective impairment in memory and learning. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 618624.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B.L., Rosse, R.B., & Deutsch, S.I. (1993). Limits of the processing view in accounting for dissociations among memory measures in a clinical population. Memory and Cognition, 21, 6372.Google Scholar
Silverstein, A.B. (1982). Two- and four-subtest short forms of the Wechlsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. Journal of Consultations in Clinical Psychology, 50, 415418.Google Scholar
Son, L.K. & Metcalfe, J. (2000). Metacognitive and control strategies in study-time allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 26, 204221.Google Scholar
Son, L.K. & Metcalfe, J. (2005). Judgments of learning: Evidence for a two-stage process. Memory and Cognition, 33, 11161129.Google Scholar
Souchay, C., Bacon, E., & Danion, J.M. (2006). Episodic feeling-of-knowing in schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 828840.Google Scholar
Traupmann, K.L., Berzofsky, M., & Kesselman, M. (1976). Encoding of taxonomic word categories by schizophrenics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85, 350355.Google Scholar
Truscott, I.P. (1973). Contextual constraint and the language function of schizophrenic outpatients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 41, 468.Google Scholar
Vidailhet, P., Christensen, B.K., Danion, J.M., & Kapur, S. (2001). Episodic memory impairment in schizophrenia: A view from cognitive psychopathology. In M. Naveh-Benjamin, M. Moscovitch, & H.L. Roediger (Eds.), Perspectives on human memory and cognitive aging. Essays in honour of Fergus Craik. London: Psychology Press.
Weiss, A.P., Schacter, D.L., Goff, D.C., Rauch, S.L., Alpert, N.M., Fischman, A.J., & Heckers, S. (2003). Impaired hippocampal recruitment during normal modulation of memory performance in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 4855.Google Scholar