Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-767nl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-13T17:19:27.152Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive implications of labels for voices

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 February 2009

Roger Brown
Affiliation:
(University of Edinburgh)

Summary

An experiment is reported which investigates the acceptability for English speakers of various syntactic constructions in conjunction with labels for voices (Laver, 1974). A regularity is observed in the co-occurrence possibilities; subjects differentiate between labels for temporal aspects of speech (tempo, continuity and rhythmicality) and for non-temporal (pitch, loudness and quality) in terms of the constructions which may accompany them. No purely syntactic motivation is found for this regularity and it is suggested instead that it is indicative of a basic phonetic difference between cognitively governed temporal parameters in speech and physiologically determined non-temporal parameters. This view is supported by examples from other languages.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Journal of the International Phonetic Association 1980

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abercrombie, D. (1964). ‘A phonetician's view of verse structure’, Linguistics, 6:513. Also in D. Abercrombie (1965), Studies in Phonetics and Linguistics, Oxford University Press, London, 16–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abercrombie, D. (1967). Elements of General Phonetics, Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Boomer, D. S., and Laver, J. (1968). ‘Slips of the tongue’, British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 3: 212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, I., and Magdics, K. (1960). ‘Speed of utterance in phrases of different lengths’, Language & Speech, 3: 179–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman-Eisler, F. (1967). ‘Sequential temporal patterns and cognitive processes in speech’, Language & Speech, 10: 122–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henderson, A., Goldman-Eisler, F., and Skarbek, A. (1965). ‘Temporal patterns of cognitive activity and breath control in speech’, Language & Speech, 8: 236–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jespersen, O. (1933). Essentials of English Grammar,Allen & Unwin, London.Google Scholar
Laver, J. (1974). ‘Labels for voices’, Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 4: 6275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laver, J. (1980). The Phonetic Description of Voice Quality, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological Foundations of Language, Wiley, New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liefrink, F. (1973). Semantico-Syntax, Longman, London.Google Scholar
Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G., and Svartvik, J. (1972). A Grammar of Contemporary English, Longman, London.Google Scholar
Sapir, E. (1927). ‘Speech as a personality trait’, American Journal of Sociology, 32: 892905. Also in Mandelbaum, D. G. (ed.) (1949), Selected Writings of Edward Sapir in Language, Culture and Personality, University of California Press, Berkeley & Los Angeles, 533–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar