Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-sh8wx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-21T16:28:18.378Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Retransition as Sign: Listener-Orientated Approaches to Tonal Closure in Haydn's Sonata-Form Movements

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2020

Michael Spitzer*
Affiliation:
University of Durham

Extract

Music psychologists interested in the perception of tonal closure might do worse than investigate the Classical retransition as a sign of impending resolution. Haydn tends to attract more than his fair share of ‘listener-orientated’ approaches, perhaps on account of his attested sensitivity to his audience's cognitive ‘processing limits’. His well-known remark to Griesinger that, isolated at Eszterháza, he ‘could make experiments, observe what elicited or weakened an impression, and thus correct, add, delete, take risks’ conveys the impression of an artist clearly in touch with his public. And yet this most playful of composers exploited this rapport to mislead his listeners, making aesthetic capital out of systematic disinformation.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Royal Musical Association, 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

1 See Bonds, Mark Evan, Wordless Rhetoric Musical Form and the Metaphor of the Oration (Cambridge, Mass, 1991).Google Scholar

2 Edited and translated in Vernon Gotwals, Joseph Haydn. Eighteenth-Century Gentleman and Genius (Madison, 1963), 17Google Scholar

3 See Rosen, Charles, The Classical Style (New York, 1972), 337 ‘A great deal of ingenuity is expended upon the return of the theme, almost always with rollicking effect the trick is to keep suggesting the return but to delay it until the listener no longer knows when to expect it.‘Google Scholar

4 See Clarke, Eric, ‘Mind the Gap Formal Structures and Psychological Processes in Music’, Contemporary Music Review, 3 (1989), 113 For a broad survey, see Cook, Nicholas, ‘Perception A Perspective from Music Theory’, Musical Perceptions, ed Rita Aiello (Oxford, 1994), 64–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar

5 Cook, Nicholas, ‘The Perception of Large-Scale Tonal Closure’, Music Perception, 5 (1987), 197205CrossRefGoogle Scholar

6 B S Rosner and Leonard B Meyer, ‘The Perceptual Roles of Melodic Process, Contour and Form’, Music Perception, 4 (1986), 139 In Meyer's theory of gap-fill motion, disjunct intervals in a melody may be resolved by countervailing conjunct progressionsGoogle Scholar

7 Sloboda, John, ‘Psychological Structures in Music Core Research 1980–1990’, Companion to Contemporary Musical Thought, ed John Paynter et al (London, 1992), 803–39 ‘Implicit in the work of Cook and Rosner and Meyer is the notion that there are processing limits on human cognitive capacity that limit what can be extracted from music, at least on early hearings Their findings do not show that people cannot hear out large-scale structure They simply show that they do not usually do so in early listening given the experimental tasks required of them […] With longer exposure and/or different tasks, it is still quite possible that we may find evidence of large-scale psychological representations' (p 833)Google Scholar

8 Meyer, Leonard B, Style and Music Theory, History and Ideology (Philadelphia. 1989) ‘In the tonic version of the changing-note schema, the melody revolves around the first degree of the scales, usually moving 1–7/2–1, but sometimes using the opposite order (1–2/7–1)’ (p 227) Meyer thinks that the former version makes a good opening theme, because it ‘rises sequentially, implying continuation to the third of the scale or beyond’ The latter version is more suitable for cadential gestures, since ‘the closure that the leading-tone-to-tonic motion (7–1) creates is too strong for the generation of more extensive melodic structures’.Google Scholar

9 V. Kofi Agawu, Playing with Signs A Semiotic Interpretation of Classic Music (Princeton, 1991), 5172.Google Scholar

10 See Trehub, Sandra E. and Trainor, Laurel J, ‘Listening Strategies in Infancy The Roots of Music and Language Development’, Thinking in Sound The Cognitive Psychology of Human Audition, ed Stephen McAdams and Emmanuel Bigand (Oxford, 1993), 278327Google Scholar

11 See Jerry Fodor and Ernest Lepore, Holism A Shopper's Guide (Oxford, 1993), 209Google Scholar

13 Ibid., 27.Google Scholar

14 Fodor, Jerry, The Modularity of Mind (Cambridge, Mass., 1983) See also his The Language of Thought (New York, 1975)Google Scholar

15 The distinguishing features of modules are informational encapsulation, domain specificity, mandatory operation and innateness. This means that modules function independently of one another, are charged with specific tasks (‘language’ modules are independent of ‘thought’ modules), and are not subject to voluntary control or change they are inborn.Google Scholar

16 Fred Lerdahl and Ray Jackendoff, A Generative Theory of Tonal Music (Cambridge, Mass., 1983).Google Scholar

17 Ibid., 222Google Scholar

18 Sisman, Elaine, ‘Small and Expanded Forms Koch's Model and Haydn's Music’, Musical Quarterly, 68 (1982), 444–75 Sisman shows how Koch and Haydn employ similar expansion techniques to arrive at larger sonata-form structures from a small period. See also Rosen, The Classical Style, 83–8 ‘The second variation of this second theme is a complete sonata movement, and it is amusing to see how Haydn expands a 20-measure theme into a larger work’ (p. 83).Google Scholar

19 Dahlhaus, Carl, ‘Harmony’, The New Grove Dictionary of Music and Musicians (London, 1980), viii, 175–88 (p 181)Google Scholar

20 See Dowling, W Jay, ‘Context Effects on Melody Recognition: Scale-Step versus Interval Representations’, Music Perception, 3 (1986), 281–96.Google Scholar

21 Davidson, Lyle, ‘Songsinging by Young and Old A Developmental Approach to Music’, Musical Perceptions, ed Aiello, 99–130 (p 109).Google Scholar

22 See Narmour, Eugene, Beyond Schenkerism (Chicago, 1977), 41–7Google Scholar

23 See Leonard B Meyer, Explaining Music (Chicago, 1973), 85 ‘A semicadence might be defined as one in which a mobile, goal-directed, harmonic process is temporarily stabilised by decisive rhythmic closure In other words, a semicadence is a case of parametric noncongruence which has become archetypal in the stylistic syntax of tonal music‘Google Scholar

24 Schachter, Carl, ‘Rhythm and Linear Analysis A Preliminary Study’, Music Forum, 5 (New York, 1976), 281334 ‘There would be little drama in this phrase were it not for the conflict between durational and tonal groupings’ (p 332).Google Scholar

25 Mandler, Jean, ‘Categorical and Schematic Organization in Memory’, Memory Organization and Structure, ed. Richard Puff (New York, 1979), 259–99 (p. 263)Google Scholar

26 Gjerdingen, Robert, A Classic Turn of Phrase (Philadelphia, 1988)Google Scholar

27 Eco, Umberto, A Theory of Semiotics (Bloomington, 1979), 69Google Scholar

28 Style and Music, 140Google Scholar

29 Browne, Richmond, ‘Tonal Implications of the Diatonic Set’, In Theory Only, 5 (1981), 321Google Scholar

30 Lerdahl, and Jackendoff, , A Generative Theory, 245Google Scholar

31 William S Newman, ‘The Climax of Music’, Music Review, 13 (1952), 283–93Google Scholar

32 David Butler and Helen Brown, ‘Describing the Mental Representation of Tonality in Music’, Musical Perceptions, ed. Aiello, 191212.Google Scholar

33 Baker, James, ‘Chromaticism in Classical Music’, Music Theory and the Exploration of the Past, ed. Christopher Hatch and David W Bernstein (Chicago, 1993), 233307.Google Scholar

34 Ibid., 263Google Scholar

35 Ibid., 277.Google Scholar

36 Lerdahl, and Jackendoff, , A Generative Theory, 183Google Scholar

37 Ibid., 179Google Scholar

38 Kermode, Frank, The Sense of an Ending (New York, 1967), 1819 ‘Peripeteia […] is present in every story of the least structural sophistication [It] depends on our confidence of the end, it is a disconfirmation followed by a consonance; the interest of having our expectations falsified is obviously related to our wish to reach the discovery or recognition by an unexpected and instructive route.‘Google Scholar

39 See Bonds, Mark Evan, ‘Haydn's False Recapitulations and the Perception of Sonata Form in the Eighteenth Century’ (Ph.D dissertation, Harvard University, 1988). See also Oliver Strunk, ‘Haydn's Divertimenti for Baryton’, Essays on Music in the Western World (New York, 1932), 126–70Google Scholar

40 Hatten, Robert S, Musical Meaning in Beethoven Markedness, Correlation, and Interpretation (Bloomington and Indianapolis, 1994)Google Scholar

41 Ibid., 34Google Scholar

42 Ibid., 36Google Scholar

43 Ibid., 79Google Scholar

44 See also the definition of Beethoven's heroic schema in Anthony Newcomb, ‘Once More “Between Absolute and Program Music” Schumann's Second Symphony’, 19th Century Music, 7 (1984), 233–50Google Scholar

45 Hatten, Musical Meaning in Beethoven, 92Google Scholar

46 Ibid., 96Google Scholar

47 Momigny's text is translated in Musical Analysis in the Nineteenth Century, ii, Hermeneutic Approaches, ed Ian Bent (Cambridge, 1994), 127–40 see also Cole, Malcolm, ‘Momigny's Analysis of Haydn's Symphony no 103’, Music Review. 30 (1969), 261–84Google Scholar

48 Music Analysis in the Nineteenth Century, ii, ed Bent, 129Google Scholar

49 Ibid., 140Google Scholar

50 Hatten, Musical Meaning in Beethoven, 119Google Scholar

51 Ibid., 120Google Scholar

52 Baker, ‘Chromaticism in Classical Music’, 266Google Scholar

53 Meyer, Explaining Music, 130Google Scholar

54 See Levy, Janet, ‘Texture as a Sign in Classic and Early Romantic Music’, Journal of the American Musicological Society, 35 (1982), 482531 (p. 483)Google Scholar

55 Ibid., 519, 497.Google Scholar

56 Levy, Texture as a Sign', 498, 507Google Scholar

57 Ibid., 520Google Scholar

58 Ibid., 498.Google Scholar

59 Ibid., 526, 507Google Scholar

60 Ibid., 520Google Scholar

61 Donald Francis Tovey. ‘Beethoven Symphony in E flat Major’, Symphonies and Other Orchestral Works (Oxford, 1989), 4452 (p. 46).Google Scholar

62 Levy, ‘Texture asa Sign’, 519Google Scholar

63 Shamgar, Beth, ‘On Locating the Retransition in Classic Sonata Form’, Music Review, 42 (1981), 130–43Google Scholar

64 Heinrich C. Koch, Versuch einer Anleitung zur Composition (Leipzig, 1797), 224Google Scholar

65 Five swings of a cycle of fifths (F♯–B; E–A, D–G, C–F♯, B–E) climax at bar 203 on the dominant of II. The passage at bars 206–15 may appear to be a false reprise, but it is really a disguised retransition, complementing the previous cycle of fifths with five swings in the opposite direction G–C, B–E; D–G; F♯–B; A–DGoogle Scholar

66 Versuch enter Anleitung, 224Google Scholar

67 Moving ahead to nineteenth-century treatises, we discover a significant change in the harmonic role of the retransition For the first time, theorists associate the retransition with the dominant pedal This linkage coincides with two basic changes in the nature of the opening border theorists no longer identify the end of the development with a cadence, and […] they now locate the start of the retransition at the onset of the dominant pedal’ (Shamgar, ‘On Locating the Retransition’, 133)Google Scholar

68 For another example of a double retransition, see Haydn's Piano Sonata in A♭, Hob. XVI:43, bars 77–100 A half close in F minor (bar 87) rhymes with a cadenza on V of A♭ (bars 93ff.) The two dominants, C and E♭, are prepared with an identical bass line E–F–A–B♭–B–C (bars 77–83) and G–A♭–C–D♭–D–E♭ (bars 90–3). The F/E♭, melodic crux at the halfway stage, bar 87, is reinterpreted as F–E♭ immediately before the reprise, bar 99 At 11 and 13 bars respectively, the two retransitions are broadly parallelGoogle Scholar

69 Schenker, Heinrich, Five Graphic Music Analyses (New York, 1969), 40–3.Google Scholar

70 Goodman, Nelson, Languages of Art (Indianapolis, 1968), 71 ‘A label along with others constituting a schema is in effect detached from the home realm of that schema and applied for the sorting and organizing of an alien realm’ (p. 72)Google Scholar

71 Playing with Signs, 90Google Scholar

72 Gasparini, Francesco, The Practical Harmonist at the Harpsichord, trans. Frank S. Stillings, ed. David L Burrows (New Haven, 1963), 39.Google Scholar

73 Hunter, Mary, ‘Text, Music, and Drama in Haydn's Italian Opera Arias’, Journal of Musicology, 7 (1989), 2957CrossRefGoogle Scholar

74 Webster, James, Haydn's ‘Farewell’ Symphony and the Idea of Classical Style (Cambridge, 1991), 142.Google Scholar

75 See Rosen, , The Classical Style, 382–3. ‘By op. 31 no. 1 [Beethoven] ventures to dispense with the dominant as a secondary tonality altogether: from G major he goes to the mediant B.’ The ‘Waldstein’ also modulates to the mediant (from C to E)Google Scholar

76 Mark Evan Bonds, ‘Haydn, Laurence Sterne, and the Origins of Musical Irony’, Journal of the American Musicological Society, 44 (1991), 5791 (p 58).Google Scholar

77 For a disruption-orientated analysis of this symphony, see Bonds, Wordless Rhetoric, 192–204. Webster (Haydn's ‘Farewell’ Symphony, 267–87) offers another interpretation, based on ‘the meaning of musical reminiscence’.Google Scholar

78 Bonds, ‘Haydn, Laurence Sterne, and the Origins of Musical Irony’, 63Google Scholar

79 Abrams, Meyer Howard, The Mirror and the Lamp Romantic Theory and the Critical Tradition (New York, 1953), introductionGoogle Scholar

80 Similarly, Beethoven recalls Haydn's II–I retransition (Symphony no 43 in E♭) in two other E♭ works. His Piano Sonata op. 31 no 3 and String Quartet op 127 both approach their recapitulations from F minor.Google Scholar

81 Beach, David, ‘A Recurring Pattern in Mozart's Music’, Journal of Music Theory, 27 (1983), 130Google Scholar

82 Ibid., 1.Google Scholar

83 Haydn's ‘Farewell’ Symphony, 138.Google Scholar

84 Ibid., 144.Google Scholar

85 See Clarke, Eric, ‘Improvisation, Cognition and Education’, Companion to Contemporary Musical Thought, ed. Paynter et al., 787802Google Scholar

86 See Marsden, Alan and Pople, Anthony, ‘Modelling Musical Cognition as a Community of Experts’, Contemporary Music Review, 3 (1989), 2942Google Scholar

87 Ricoeur, Paul, ‘The Metaphorical Process as Cognition, Imagination, and Feeling’, Philosophical Perspectives on Metaphor, ed Mark Johnson (Minneapolis, 1985), 228–47 (p 241)Google Scholar

88 Ludwig Wittgenstein, Philosophical Investigations, trans Elizabeth Anscombe (Oxford, 1984), 193208 ‘“Now he's seeing it like this”, “now like that” would only be said of someone capable of making certain applications of the figure quite freely The substratum of this experience is the mastery of a technique’ (p 208)Google Scholar

89 Marcus B. Hester, ‘Metaphor and Aspect Seeing’, Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 25 (1966), 205–12 (p 207)Google Scholar

90 Agawu, Playing with Signs, 30.Google Scholar

91 Gal, Susan, ‘The Political Economy of Code Choice’, Codeswitching, ed. Monica Heller (Berlin, 1988), 245–64 (p. 247)Google Scholar

92 Wardhaugh, Ronald, An Introduction to Sociolinguistics (Oxford, 1994), 258–81 (p 260)Google Scholar