Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-tn8tq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-29T10:53:47.954Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Clark Glymour, The Mind's Arrows: Bayes Nets and Graphical Causal Models in Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2001), 240 pp., $30.00 (cloth).

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2022

Charles Twardy*
Affiliation:
Monash University

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Book Review
Copyright
Copyright © The Philosophy of Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ahn, Woo-kyoung, and Bailenson, Jeremy (1996), “Causal Attribution as a Search for Underlying Mechanisms: An Explanation of the Conjunction Fallacy and the Discounting Principle”, Causal Attribution as a Search for Underlying Mechanisms: An Explanation of the Conjunction Fallacy and the Discounting Principle 31:82123.Google ScholarPubMed
Ahn, Woo-kyoung, et al. (1995), “The Role of Covariation versus Mechanism Information in Causal Attribution”, The Role of Covariation versus Mechanism Information in Causal Attribution 54:299352.Google ScholarPubMed
Baker, Andrew G., et al. (1993), “Selective Associations and Causality Judgments: The Presence of a Strong Causal Factor May Reduce Judgments of a Weaker One”, Selective Associations and Causality Judgments: The Presence of a Strong Causal Factor May Reduce Judgments of a Weaker One 19:414432.Google Scholar
Cheng, Patricia W. (1997). “From Covariation to Causation: A Causal Power Theory”, Psychological Review 104:367405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glymour, Clark (1998), “What Went Wrong? Reflections on Science by Observation and the Bell Curve”, What Went Wrong? Reflections on Science by Observation and the Bell Curve 65:132.Google Scholar
Glymour, Clark, and Cheng, Patricia (1998), “Causal Mechanism and Probability: A Normative Approach”, in Oaksford, Mike and Chater, Nick (eds.), Rational Models of Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 295313.Google Scholar
Hausman, Daniel M., and Woodward, James (2004), “Modularity and the Causal Markov Condition: A Restatement”, Modularity and the Causal Markov Condition: A Restatement 55:147161.Google Scholar
Herrnstein, Richard J., and Murray, Charles (1994), The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Kelley, Harold H. (1973), “The Processes of Causal Attribution”, The Processes of Causal Attribution 28:107128.Google Scholar
Tenenbaum, Joshua B., and Griffiths, Thomas L. (2001), “Structure Learning in Human Causal Induction”, in Leen, Todd K., Dietterich, Thomas G., and Tresp, Volker (eds.), Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 13. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 5965.Google Scholar