Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-tdptf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-08T04:27:18.493Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Disgust sensitivity and support for immigration across five nations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 March 2022

Scott Clifford*
Affiliation:
University of Houston
Cengiz Erisen
Affiliation:
Yeditepe University
Dane Wendell
Affiliation:
Illinois College
Francisco Cantú
Affiliation:
University of Houston
*
Correspondence: Scott Clifford, University of Houston. Email: sclifford@uh.edu
Get access

Abstract

Immigration has become a focal debate in politics across the world. Recent research suggests that anti-immigration attitudes may have deep psychological roots in implicit disease avoidance motivations. A key implication of this theory is that individual differences in disease avoidance should be related to opposition to immigration across a wide variety of cultural and political contexts. However, existing evidence on the topic has come almost entirely from the United States and Canada. In this article, we test the disease avoidance hypothesis using nationally representative samples from Norway, Sweden, Turkey, and Mexico, as well as two diverse samples from the United States. We find consistent and robust evidence that disgust sensitivity is associated with anti-immigration attitudes and that the relationship is similar in magnitude to education. Overall, our findings support the disease avoidance hypothesis and provide new insights into the nature of anti-immigration attitudes.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Association for Politics and the Life Sciences

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aarøe, L., Osmundsen, M., & Petersen, M. B. (2016). Distrust as a disease-voidance strategy: Individual differences in disgust sensitivity regulate generalized social trust. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1038. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPSYG.2016.01038Google ScholarPubMed
Aarøe, L., Petersen, M. B., & Arceneaux, K. (2017). The behavioral immune system shapes political intuitions: Why and how individual differences in disgust sensitivity underlie opposition to immigration. American Political Science Review, 111(2), 277294. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055416000770CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aarøe, L., Petersen, M. B., & Arceneaux, K. (2020). The behavioral immune system shapes partisan preferences in modern democracies: Disgust sensitivity predicts voting for socially conservative parties. Political Psychology, 41(6), 10731091.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Acosta-García, R., & Martínez-Ortiz, E. (2015). Mexico through a superdiversity lens: Already-existing diversity meets new immigration. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 38(4), 636649.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakker, B. N., Lelkes, Y., & Malka, A. (2021). Reconsidering the link between self-reported personality traits and political preferences. American Political Science Review, 115(4), 14821498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakker, B. N., Schumacher, G., Gothreau, C., & Arceneaux, K. (2020). Conservatives and liberals have similar physiological responses to threats. Nature Human Behavior, 4, 613621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balzer, A., & Jacobs, C. M. (2011). Gender and physiological effects in connecting disgust to political preferences. Social Science Quarterly, 92(5), 12971313. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6237.2011.00819.xGoogle Scholar
Bansak, K., Hainmueller, J., & Hangartner, D. (2016). How econmic, humanitarian, and religious concerns shape European attitudes toward asylum seekers. Science, 354(6309), 217222. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aag2147CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Billingsley, J., Lieberman, D., & Tybur, J. M. (2018). Sexual disgust trumps pathogen disgust in predicting voter behavior during the 2016 U.S. presidential election. Evolutionary Psychology, 16(2), 147470491876417. https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704918764170CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boston, J., Homola, J., Sinclair, B., Torres, M., & Tucker, P. D. (2018). The dynamic relationship between personality stability and political attitudes. Public Opinion Quarterly, 82(S1), 843865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brader, T., Valentino, N. A., & Suhay, E. (2008). What triggers public opposition to immigration? Anxiety, group cues, and immigration threat. American Journal of Political Science, 52(4), 959978. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5907.2008.00353.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castañeda, X., Manz, B., & Davenport, A. (2002). Mexicanization: A survival strategy for Guatemalan Mayans in the San Francisco Bay Area. Migraciones Internacionales, 1(3).Google Scholar
Clay, R. (2017). The behavioral immune system and attitudes about vaccines: Contamination aversion predicts more negative vaccine attitudes. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8(2), 162172. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550616664957CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clifford, S., Jewell, R. M., & Waggoner, P. D. (2015). Are samples drawn from Mechanical Turk valid for research on political ideology? Research & Politics, 2(4), 19. https://doi.org/10.1177/2053168015622072CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clifford, S., & Piston, S. (2017). Explaining public support for counterproductive homelessness policy: The role of disgust. Political Behavior, 39(2), 503525. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-016-9366-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clifford, S., & Wendell, D. G. (2016). How disgust influences health purity attitudes. Political Behavior, 38(1), 155178. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-015-9310-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coppock, A., & McClellan, O. A. (2019). Validating the demographic, political, psychological, and experimental results obtained from a new source of online survey respondents. Research & Politics, 6(1), 205316801882217. https://doi.org/10.1177/2053168018822174CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craig, M. A., & Richeson, J. A. (2014). More diverse yet less tolerant? How the increasingly diverse racial landscape affects White Americans’ racial attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(6), 750761. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167214524993CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtis, V., Aunger, R., & Rabie, T. (2004). Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 271(Suppl. 4), S131–133. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2003.0144CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtis, V., & Biran, A. (2001). Dirt, disgust, and disease: Is hygiene in our genes? Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 44(1), 1731. https://doi.org/10.1353/pbm.2001.0001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deacon, B., & Olatunji, B. O. (2007). Specificity of disgust sensitivity in the prediction of behavioral avoidance in contamination fear. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(9), 21102120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.03.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Vreese, C. H., & Boomgaarden, H. G. (2016). Projecting EU referendums: Fear of immigration and support for European integration. European Union Politics, 6(1), 5982. https://doi.org/10.1177/1465116505049608CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erisen, C., Vasilopoulou, S., & Kentmen-Cin, C. (2020). Emotional reactions to immigration and support for EU cooperation on immigration and terrorism. Journal of European Public Policy, 27(6), 795813. https://doi.org/10.1080/13501763.2019.1630470/SUPPL_FILE/RJPP_A_1630470_SM0365.PDFCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fan, Q., & Olatunji, B. O. (2013). Individual differences in disgust sensitivity and health-related avoidance: Examination of specific associations. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(5), 454458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faulkner, J., Schaller, M., Park, J. H., & Duncan, L. A. (2004). Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 7(4), 333353. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430204046142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fincher, C. L., Thornhill, R., Murray, D. R., Schaller, M., Anderson, R. M., May, R. M., Bond, R., Smith, P. B., Bouchard, T. J., McGue, M., Cohen, D., Cukur, C. S., Gusman, M. R. T. De, Carlo, G., Dobson, A. P., Carper, E. R., Ewald, P. W., Faulkner, J., Schaller, M., … & Stoehr, A. M. (2008). Pathogen prevalence predicts human cross-cultural variability in individualism/collectivism. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275(1640), 12791285. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.0094CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, M., & Milazzo, C. (2017). Taking back control? Investigating the role of immigration in the 2016 vote for Brexit. British Journal of Politics and International Relations, 19(3), 450464. https://doi.org/10.1177/1369148117710799CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, E. G. T., Krings, F., Staerkle, Bangerter, A., Clemence, A., Wagner-Egger, P., & Bornand, T. (2010). Keeping the vermin out: Perceived disease threat and ideological orientations as predictors of exclusionary immigration attitudes. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 20(4), 299316. https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.1037CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hainmueller, J., & Hangartner, D. (2013). Who gets a Swiss passport? A natural experiment in immigrant discrimination. American Political Science Review, 107(1), 159187. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055412000494CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hainmueller, J., & Hiscox, M. J. (2007). Educated preferences: Explaining attitudes toward immigration in Europe. International Organization, 61(2), 399442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hainmueller, J., Hiscox, M. J., & Margalit, Y. (2014). Do concerns about labor market competition shape attitudes toward immigration? New evidence. Journal of International Economics, 97(1), 193207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jinteco.2014.12.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hainmueller, J., & Hopkins, D. J. (2014). Public attitudes toward immigration. Annual Review of Political Science, 17, 225249. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-polisci-102512-194818CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hainmueller, J., & Hopkins, D. J. (2015). The hidden American immigration consensus: A conjoint analysis of attitudes toward immigrants. American Journal of Political Science, 59(3), 529548. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartman, T. K., Newman, B. J., & Bell, C. S. (2014). Decoding prejudice toward Hispanics: Group cues and public reactions to threatening immigrant behavior. Political Behavior, 36(1), 143163. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-013-9231-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haselton, M. G., & Nettle, D. (2006). The paranoid optimist: An integrative evolutionary model of cognitive biases. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(1), 4766. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hooghe, M., & Dassonneville, R. (2018). Explaining the Trump vote: The effect of racist resentment and anti-immigrant sentiments. PS: Political Science & Politics, 51(3), 528534. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1049096518000367Google Scholar
Kalmoe, N. P. (2020). Uses and abuses of ideology in political psychology. Political Psychology, 41(4), 771793. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12650CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kam, C. D., & Estes, B. A. (2016). Disgust sensitivity and public demand for protection. Journal of Politics, 78(2), 481496. https://doi.org/10.1086/684611CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karinen, A. K., Tybur, J. M., & de Vries, R. (2019). The disgust traits: Self–other agreement in disgust sensitivity and its independent from HEXACO personality. PsyArXiv. https://osf.io/pm2x6/Google Scholar
Kentmen-Cin, C., & Erisen, C. (2017). Anti-immigration attitudes and the opposition to European integration: A critical assessment: European Union Politics, 18(1), 325. https://doi.org/10.1177/1465116516680762CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurzban, R., & Leary, M. R. (2001). Evolutionary origins of stigmatization: The functions of social exclusion. Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 187208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenz, G. S., & Sahn, A. (2021). Achieving statistical significance with control variables and without transparency. Political Analysis, 29(3), 356369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, D. L., Tybur, J. M., & Latner, J. D. (2012). Disgust sensitivity, obesity stigma, and gender: Contamination psychology predicts weight bias for women, not men. Obesity, 20(9), 18031814. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2011.247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liuzza, M. T., Lindholm, T., Hawley, C. B., Gustafsson Sendén, M., Ekström, I., Olsson, M. J., & Olofsson, J. K. (2018). Body odour disgust sensitivity predicts authoritarian attitudes. Royal Society Open Science, 5(2), 171091. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.171091CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luttig, M. D. (2021). Reconsidering the relationship between authoritarianism and Republican support in 2016 and beyond. Journal of Politics, 83(2), 783787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Major, B., Blodorn, A., & Major Blascovich, G. (2018). The threat of increasing diversity: Why many White Americans support Trump in the 2016 presidential election. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 21(6), 931940. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430216677304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, P. R., Flores, A. R., Haider-Markel, D. P., Lewis, D. C., Tadlock, B. L., & Taylor, J. K. (2017). Transgender politics as body politics: effects of disgust sensitivity and authoritarianism on transgender rights attitudes. Politics, Groups, and Identities, 5(1), 424. https://doi.org/10.1080/21565503.2016.1260482CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moran, J. B., Goh, J. X., Kerry, N., & Murray, D. R. (2021). Outbreaks and outgroups: Three tests of the relationship between disease avoidance motives and xenophobia during an emerging pandemic. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 7(4), 419429. 10.1007/s40806-021-00283-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, D. R., Schaller, M., & Suedfeld, P. (2013). Pathogens and politics: Further evidence that parasite prevalence predicts authoritarianism. PLOS ONE, 8(5), e62275. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0062275CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, D. R., Trudeau, R., & Schaller, M. (2011). On the origins of cultural differences in conformity: Four tests of the pathogen prevalence hypothesis. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(3), 318329. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210394451CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Navarrete, C. D., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2006). Disease avoidance and ethnocentrism: The effects of disease vulnerability and disgust sensitivity on intergroup attitudes. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(4), 270282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.12.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, B. J., Hartman, T. K., & Taber, C. S. (2012). Foreign language exposure, cultural threat, and opposition to immigration. Political Psychology, 33(5), 635657. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2012.00904.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 303321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olatunji, B. O., Adams, T., Ciesielski, B., David, B., Sarawgi, S., & Broman-Fulks, J. (2012). The three domains of disgust scale: Factor structure, psychometric properties, and conceptual limitations. Assessment, 19(2), 205225. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191111432881CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olatunji, B. O., Williams, N. L., Tolin, D. F., Abramowitz, J. S., Sawchuk, C. N., Lohr, J. M., & Elwood, L. S. (2007). The disgust scale: Item analysis, factor structure, and suggestions for refinement. Psychological Assessment, 19(3), 281297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development. (2022). Database on Immigrants in OECD Countries (DIOC). Retrieved accessed October 20, 2021 from https://stats.oecd.org/Index.aspx?DataSetCode=DIOC_SEX_AGEGoogle Scholar
Park, J. H., Faulkner, J., & Schaller, M. (2003). Evolved disease-avoidance processes and contemporary anti-social behavior: Prejudicial attitudes and avoidance of people with physical disabilities. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27(2), 6587. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023910408854CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, J. H., Schaller, M., & Crandall, C. S. (2007). Pathogen-avoidance mechanisms and the stigmatization of obese people. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(6), 410414. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.05.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, J. H., van Leeuwen, F., & Chochorelou, Y. (2013). Disease-avoidance processes and stigmatization: Cues of substandard health arouse heightened discomfort with physical contact. Journal of Social Psychology, 153(2), 212228. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2012.721812CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, J. H., van Leeuwen, F., & Stephen, I. D. (2012). Homeliness is in the disgust sensitivity of the beholder: Relatively unattractive faces appear especially unattractive to individuals higher in pathogen disgust. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(5), 569577. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.02.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petersen, M. B. (2015). Evolutionary political psychology: On the origin and structure of heuristics and biases in politics. Political Psychology, 36(S1), 4578. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12237CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petersen, M. B. (2017). Healthy out-group members are represented psychologically as infected in-group members. Psychological Science, 28(12), 18571863. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617728270CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reynolds, L. M., McCambridge, S. A., Bissett, I. P., & Consedine, N. S. (2014). Trait and state disgust: An experimental investigation of disgust and avoidance in colorectal cancer decision scenarios. Health Psychology, 33(12), 14951506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., McCauley, C., Dunlop, L., & Ashmore, M. (1999). Individual differences in disgust sensitivity: Comparisons and evaluations of paper-and-pencil versus behavioral measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 33(3), 330351. https://doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.1999.2251CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaller, M., & Park, J. H. (2011). The behavioral immune system (and why it matters). Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(2), 99103. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411402596CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheve, K. F., & Slaughter, M. J. (2001). Labor market competition and individual preferences over immigration policy. Review of Economics and Statistics, 83(1), 133145. https://doi.org/10.1162/003465301750160108CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherlock, J. M., Zietsch, B. P., Tybur, J. M., & Jern, P. (2016). The quantitative genetics of disgust sensitivity. Emotion, 16(1), 4351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shook, N. J., Terrizzi, J. A., Clay, R., & Oosterhoff, B. (2015). In defense of pathogen disgust and disease avoidance: A response to Tybur et al. (2015). Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(6), 498502. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.06.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sides, J., Tesler, M., & Vavreck, L. (2017). How Trump lost and won. Journal of Democracy, 28(2), 3444. https://doi.org/10.1353/jod.2017.0022CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sirin, C. V., Valentino, N. A., & Villalobos, J. D. (2017). The social causes and political consequences of group empathy. Political Psychology, 38(3), 427448. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12352CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sniderman, P. M., Hagendoorn, L., & Prior, M. (2004). Predisposing factors and situational triggers: Exclusionary reactions to immigrant minorities. American Political Science Review, 98(1), 3549. https://doi.org/10.1017/S000305540400098XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terrizzi, J. A., Shook, N. J., & McDaniel, M. A. (2013). The behavioral immune system and social conservatism: A meta-analysis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(2), 99108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.10.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tybur, J. M., Bryan, A. D., Lieberman, D., Caldwell Hooper, A. E., & Merriman, L. A. (2011). Sex differences and sex similarities in disgust sensitivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(3), 343348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tybur, J. M., Frankenhuis, W. E., & Pollet, T. V. (2014). Behavioral immune system methods: Surveying the present to shape the future. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 8(4), 274283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tybur, J. M., Inbar, Y., Aarøe, L., Barclay, P., Barlow, F. K., de Barra, M., Becker, D. V., Borovoi, L., Choi, I., Choi, J. A., Consedine, N. S., Conway, A., Conway, J. R., Conway, P., Adoric, V. C., Demirci, D. E., Fernández, A. M., Ferreira, D. C. S., Ishii, K., … Žeželj, I. (2016). Parasite stress and pathogen avoidance relate to distinct dimensions of political ideology across 30 nations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(44), 1240812413. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1607398113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tybur, J. M., Inbar, Y., Güler, E., & Molho, C. (2015). Is the relationship between pathogen avoidance and ideological conservatism explained by sexual strategies? Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(6), 489497. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.01.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., & Griskevicius, V. (2009). Microbes, mating, and morality: Individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 103122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R., & DeScioli, P. (2013). Disgust: Evolved function and structure. Psychological Review, 120(1), 6584. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030778CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
United Nations. (2019). International migrant stock. Monitoring Global Population Trends. Retrieved accessed October 20, 2021 from https://www.un.org/en/development/desa/population/migration/data/estimates2/countryprofiles.aspGoogle Scholar
van Leeuwen, F., Park, J. H., Koenig, B. L., & Graham, J. (2012). Regional variation in pathogen prevalence predicts endorsement of group-focused moral concerns. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(5), 429437. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.12.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Leeuwen, F., & Petersen, M. B. (2018). The behavioral immune system is designed to avoid infected individuals, not outgroups. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(2), 226234. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.EVOLHUMBEHAV.2017.12.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vanaman, M. E., & Chapman, H. A. (2020). Disgust and disgust-driven moral concerns predict support for restrictions on transgender bathroom access. Politics and the Life Sciences, 39(2), 200214. https://doi.org/10.1017/pls.2020.20CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vitriol, J. A., Larsen, E. G., & Ludeke, S. G. (2019). The generalizability of personality effects in politics. European Journal of Personality, 33(6), 631641. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, B.-P., & Chang, L. (2012). The social impact of pathogen threat: How disease salience influences conformity. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(1), 5054. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PAID.2012.02.023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Clifford et al. supplementary material

Appendix

Download Clifford et al. supplementary material(File)
File 89.6 KB