Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-sv6ng Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-18T22:38:20.160Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Cognitive and Social Sides of Epistemology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 February 2022

Alvin I. Goldman*
Affiliation:
University of Arizona

Extract

Any plausible treatment of human knowledge must acknowledge two shapers of opinion: the psychological and the social. The formation of almost any belief, even the mere perception of familiar objects or recollection of recent events, involves the execution of a complex array of mental operations or computations. So the psychological component in belief-formation is ubiquitous. The social component, while not quite ubiquitous, is almost as crucial. A large proportion of the mature cognizer's beliefs are acquired from others; and typically the vehicles of acquisition — public language, and internal techniques for assessing the claims of others — are themselves socially or culturally acquired. Scientists, of course, are no different in these fundamental respects. They too are cognizers with human brains, and they work in professional communities. Their beliefs result from use of their psychological equipment, and from the scientific lore and methodology that is largely indebted to interpersonal training and communication.

Type
Part IX. Epistemology
Copyright
Copyright © 1987 by the Philosophy of Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, John R. (1986) Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications, 2nd edition. San Francisco: W. H.Freeman and Company.Google Scholar
Barnes, Barry. (1977) Interests and the Growth of Knowledge. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Barnes, Barry and Bloor, David. (1982) “Relativism.Rationalism and the Sociology of Knowledge.” In Rationality and Relativism. Edited by Hollis, M. and Lukes, S.. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Pages 21-47.Google Scholar
Bloor, David. (1976) Knowledge and Social Imagery. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Feldman, J.A. and Ballard, D.H. (1982) “Connectionist Models and Their Properties.” Cognitive Science 6: 205-254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feyerabend, Paul. (1975) Against Method. London: New Left Books.Google Scholar
Firth, Roderick. (1978) “Are Epistemic Concepts Reducible to Ethical Concepts?” In Values and Morals. Edited by Goldman, A. and Kim, J.. Dordrecht: D. Reidel. Pages 215-229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forman, Paul. (1971) “Weimar Culture, Causality and Quantum Theory, 1918-1927: Adaptation by German Physicists and Mathematicians to a Hostile Intellectual Environment.” In Historical Studies in the Physical Sciences, No. 3. Edited by McCormmach, R.. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Pages 1-115.Google Scholar
Gelfand, A.A. and Solomon, H. (1975) “Analyzing the Decision-Making Process of the American Jury.” Journal of the American Statistical Association 70: 305-310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giere, Ronald. (1985) “Philosophy of Science Naturalized.” Philosophy of Science 52: 331-356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, Alvin I. (1986) Epistemology and Cognition. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goldman, Alvin I. (Forthcoming a). “Foundations of Social Epistemics.” Synthese.Google Scholar
Goldman, Alvin I. (Forthcoming b). “Strong and Weak Justification.” In Philosophical Perspectives, Vol. II. Edited by Tomberlin, James.Google Scholar
Grofman, Bernard. (1977) “Jury Decision-Making Models.” In Modeling the CriminalJustice System, Vol. VII, Justice System Analysis. Edited by S. Nagel. Beverly Hills, Cal.: Sage Publications. Pages 191-203.Google Scholar
Grofman, Bernard. (1980) “The Slippery Slope: Jury Size and Jury Verdict Requirements, Legal and Social Science Approaches.” Law and Politics Quarterly 2: 285-304.Google Scholar
Grofman, Bernard. (1981) “Mathematical Models of Juror and Jury Decision- Making.” InThe Trial Process. Edited by Sales, Bruce. New York: Plenum Press. Pages 305-351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grofman, B., Owen, G., and Feld, S. (1983) “Thirteen Theorems in Search of the Truth.” Theory and Decision 15: 261-278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harman, Gilbert. (1986) Change in View. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hinton, G.E. and Sejnowski, T.J. (1986) “Learning and Relearning in Boltzmann Machines.” In Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructures of Cognition, Vol. I, Foundations. By Rumelhart, David, McClelland, James and the PDP Research Group. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Pages 282-317.Google Scholar
Hinton, G.E. and Sejnowski, T.J. (1986) “Learning and Relearning in Boltzmann Machines.” In Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructures of Cognition, Vol. I, Foundations. By Rumelhart, David, McClelland, James, and the PDP Research Group. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Pages 282-317.Google Scholar
Kosslyn, Stephen. (1980) Image and Mind. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Laudan, Larry. (1977) Progress and Its Problems. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Laudan, Larry. (1984) “The Pseudo-Science of Science?” In Scientific Rationality: The Sociological Turn. Edited by James Brown. Dordrecht: D. Reidel. Pages 41-73.Google Scholar
Mannheim, Karl. (1936) Ideology and Utopia. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.Google Scholar
Newton-Smith, W.H. (1981) The Rationality of Science. London: Routledgeand Kegan Paul.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisbett, Richard and Ross, Lee. (1980) Human Inference: Strategiesand Shortcomings of Social Judgment. Englewcod Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Zenon. (1981) “The Imagery Debate: Analog Media versus Tacit Knowledge.” In Imagery. Edited by Ned Block. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Pages 151-206.Google Scholar
Quine, W.V. (1969a) “Epistemology Naturalized.” In Ontological Relativity and Other Essays. New York: Columbia University Press. Pages 69-90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quine, W.V. (1969b) “Natural Kinds.” la Ontological Relativity and Other Essays. New York: Columbia University Press. Pages 114-138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rips, Lance. (1983) “Cognitive Processes in Reasoning.” Psychological Review 90: 38-71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rorty, Richard. (1979) Philosophy and the Mirror of Nature. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Ross, Lee and Anderson, Craig. (1982) “Shortcomings in the Attribution Process: On the Origins and Maintenance of Erroneous Social Assessments.” In Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Edited by Kahneman, D., Slovic, P. and Tversky, A. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Pages 129-152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, L., Lepper, M.R. and Hubbard, M. (1975) “Perseverance in Self Perception and Social Perception: Biased Attributional Processes in the Debriefing Paradigm.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 32: 880-892.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D.E., Hinton, G.E. and McClelland, J.L. (1986) “A General Framework for Parallel Distributed Processing.” In Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructures of Cognition, Vol. I, Foundations. By David Rumelhart, James McClelland, and the PDP Research Group. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Pages 45-76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sarkar, Husain. (1983) A Theory of Method. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Shapin, Steven. (1975) “Phrenological Knowledge and the Social Structure of Early Nineteenth-Century Edinburgh.” Annals of Science 32: 219-243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapley, Lloyd and Grofman, Bernard. (1984). “Optimizing Group Judgmental Accuracy in the Presence of Interdependencies”. Public Choice 43: 329-343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, Roger and Cooper, Lynn. (1982) Mental Images and Their Transformations. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thibaut, John and Walker, Laurens. (1975) ProceduralJustice: A Psychological Analysis. Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Tribe, Laurence. (1971) “Trial by Mathematics: Precision and Ritual in the Legal Process.” Harvard Law Review 84: 1329-1393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar